Thanks to visit codestin.com
Credit goes to www.scribd.com

0% found this document useful (0 votes)
145 views2 pages

Attachment Styles: Evolutionary Insights

This document summarizes research on attachment styles in both human and non-human animals. It discusses how early research on animal behavior and imprinting informed studies of human infant attachment to caregivers. Three common styles of attachment were identified in infants - secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent. Later research translated these into styles often seen in adult romantic relationships as well. Attachment theory provides a framework for understanding emotions in relationships from infancy through adulthood.

Uploaded by

Tubocurare
Copyright
© Attribution Non-Commercial (BY-NC)
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOC, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
145 views2 pages

Attachment Styles: Evolutionary Insights

This document summarizes research on attachment styles in both human and non-human animals. It discusses how early research on animal behavior and imprinting informed studies of human infant attachment to caregivers. Three common styles of attachment were identified in infants - secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent. Later research translated these into styles often seen in adult romantic relationships as well. Attachment theory provides a framework for understanding emotions in relationships from infancy through adulthood.

Uploaded by

Tubocurare
Copyright
© Attribution Non-Commercial (BY-NC)
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOC, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 2

Attachment Styles: An Evolving Taxonomy of Evolutionarily Adaptive and Maladaptive Affectional Bonds

The above model (taken from Bartholomew, 1990) is one representation of attachment styles, or ways of dealing with attachment, separation, and loss in close personal relationships. Attachment was first studied in non-human animals, then in human infants, and later in human adults. Basic research on animal behavior, if it is to apply to humans, must assume that there is homology between the animal and human processes. Homology means that anatomical or behavioral structures in different species share a common function and common underlying mechanisms due to the fact that the species evolved from a common ancestor. Ethological studies on imprinting might appear relevant to human attachment research, since imprinting appears to be a variety of parent-offspring attachment. But as one author says, "The charming tales of geese and cranes that court their keepers (to whom they were imprinted as hatchlings) have beguiled us all" (Klopfer, 1984, p. 157). While imprinting may appear similar to human parent-offspring bonding, imprinting in geese, cranes, ducks, etc. probably does not meet the homology requirement, since birds and mammals both evolved from lizards, and present-day lizards show no evidence of parentoffspring bonding (Crnic, Reite, & Shucard, 1982). Research shows that attachment occurs in dogs and monkeys (Crnic et al., 1982; Gacsi, Topal, Miklosi, Doka, & Csanyi, 2001; Topal, Miklosi, Csanyi, & Doka, 1998). A line of animal research that might be relevant to human attachment is Harlow's experiments

showing that infant monkeys prefer a soft terry cloth mother surrogate to a wire one, even when only the wire one dispenses milk. Also, severely deprived infant monkeys often come to behave differently from their normally reared peers (Novak & Harlow, 1975). When a human or non-human primate infant is separated from its parent, the infant goes through a series of three stages of emotional reactions. First is protest, in which the infant cries and refuses to be consoled by others. Second is despair, in which the infant is sad and passive. Third is detachment, in which the infant actively disregards and avoids the parent if the parent returns (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The fundamental assumption in attachment research on human infants is that sensitive responding by the parent to the infant's needs results in an infant who demonstrates secure attachment, while lack of such sensitive responding results in insecure attachment (Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, Charnov, & Estes, 1984). Theorists have postulated several varieties of insecure attachment. Ainsworth originally proposed two: avoidant, and resistant (also called ambivalent; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). This triarchic taxonomy of secure, avoidant, and resistant attachment was developed as a way of classifying infant behavior in the "strange situation." Secure infants either seek proximity or contact or else greet the parent at a distance with a smile or wave. Avoidant infants avoid the parent. Resistant / ambivalent infants either passively or actively show hostility toward the parent. Attachment theory provides not only a framework for understanding emotional reactions in infants, but also a framework for understanding love, loneliness, and grief in adults. Attachment styles in adults are thought to stem directly from the working models (or mental models) of oneself and others that were developed during infancy and childhood. Ainsworth's three-fold taxonomy of attachment styles has been translated into terms of adult romantic relationships as follows (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Secure adults find it relatively easy to get close to others and are comfortable depending on others and having others depend on them. Secure adults don't often worry about being abandoned or about someone getting too close to them. Avoidant adults are somewhat uncomfortable being close to others; they find it difficult to trust others completely, difficult to allow themselves to depend on others. Avoidant adults are nervous when anyone gets too close, and often, love partners want them to be more intimate than they feel comfortable being. Anxious / ambivalent adults find that others are reluctant to get as close as they would like. Anxious / ambivalent adults often worry that their partner doesn't really love them or won't want to stay with them. Anxious / ambivalent adults want to merge completely with another person, and this desire sometimes scares people away.

You might also like