CHAPTER - 2
REVIEW OF LI'iERATURE
Play currently received the attention of researchers who are
investigating the relationship of play to cognitive language, social
and gross motor development. The researchers were also becoming
aware of an ability to manipulate the quality and quantity of certain
types of play through environmental adjustments.
Even though limited studies were found on play and play
activities of pre-school children an attempt was made to bring some
relevant studies undertaken. To improve the clarity, the available
related literature regarding play in pre-school children were grouped
under the different following heads.
1. Play and its significance
2. Play for allround development.
3. Play styles and gender
4. Play as therapy for pre-school children.
5. Status of play in pre-schools.
6. Play and pre-school teachers.
7. Parent's expectations of play in pre-school education.
8. Value of play equipment
1. PLAYAND ITS SIGNIFICANCE
Play is of great importance in the life of every child. Need for
play is an universal phenomenon. As Day (1984) and Chowdhury
(1984), pointed out play is an indispensable activity for children,
accepted as such by society since the dawn of mankind. Play as a
topic of research seem to be of interest since 17th century. Several
Western and Indian philosophers emphasized the importance of play
for children. It is worthwhile to mention here the views of some of
the educational philosophers towards preschool curriculum and
importance of play in curriculum.
Western Educators
Comenius (IFh Century)
Comenius emphasized the need for shorter school hours, more
play, sound health, better bodily development and less strain. He
wanted the spirit of play to permeate the school subjects. He
suggested that the children should be given tools and allowed to
imitate different handicrafts by playing at farming, at politics, a t
being soldiers or architects etcetera.
Basedow (17th Century)
Basedow, the philanthropists work was well recognised. The
children and their teachers worked and played together from
morning till night. One most important thing to remember about
the philanthrinium was that within it's walls education was fun.
The children were not aware that their school days stretched from
dawn to eve because they were constantly engaged in play activities
and their learning was so incidental that they hardly noticed it.
Basedow's basic principle was that all education should be a means
of pleasant and entertaining play. He tried to introduce three
elements of play in to school work. He made education pleasant and
amusing. He presented work in game form. He banned all memory
work during child hood except by means of games.
Pestalozzi (1746-1827) (18th Century)
Pestalozzi took a singular pleasure in watching the games of
his pupils, which he considered of very great importance. His idea
being that children when not at work ought to enjoy themselves and
that a state of total inactivity was bad both physically and morally.
If he noticed a child taking no part in the game during play timc, he
could seldom rest till he had tried to find him some other
amusement.
Froebel(1782-1860) (18th Century)
According to him play was the most spiritual activity of man
during childhood and at the same time, typical of human life aa a
whole - of the inner hidden natural life in man and in all things. It
gave therefore joy, freedom, contentment inner and outer, rest and
peace with the world. It holds the sources of all that ia good. A child
18
that played thoroughly with self active determination perseveringly
until1 physical fatigue forbids will surely be a thorough determined
man capable of self - sacrifice for the promotion of the weifare of
himself and others. The play of childhood were the germinal leaves
of all later life for the whole man developed and shown in his
tenderest dispositions in his inner most tendencies. If the child was
injured at this period and the germinal leaves of the future tree of
his life were marred at this time, he will only with the greatest
difficulty and utmost effort grow in to strong manhood.
Indian Educators
Rabindranath Tagore (1861-1941)
Education must be gradual and progressive growth of an
organism - a child grows as an organism grows. Education in his
school began with training of instincts and emotions and in self
reliance and communal operations, then art, music and play were
introduced. His methods of teaching included Peripatetic method,
Activity method and Environmental approach. He was convinced
that in early years the child should arrive at truth through natural
process spontaneously by coming in to contact with things and
persons. This would pave the way for the widest possible
development of child's interests. Learning for the child should be
essentially, active and joyful.
Jiddu Krishnamurthy (1894 - 1973)
The basic theme of his educational phiiosophy was that
children are not be fed on other ideas, dogmas sayings, beliefs,
slogans and quotes. The present system of education according to
him has some inherent drawbacks. It does not awaken the
integrated intelligence of the child. It encouraged him to conform
to a rigid system. He aimed that education is not to acquire
knowledge and correlate it with other aspects of life. Education
should prepare the chlld for life as a whole. The right kind of
education should encourage thoughtfulness and concern for
others.
Aurobindo Ghosh (1872-1950)
His philosophy of education aims at all round development,
drawing out of what is best in a child. Education according to him
included education of the senses, education of body, education of
mind, moral education and religious education. He is against
teaching by snippets. He does not favour introduction of several
subjects at the early state. The chief aim of education should be able
to help the growing soul to draw out that in itself which is best
and make it perfect for a noble use.
Zakir Hussain (1897-1969)
The aim of education according to him was emotional
integration. Education instead of being bookish, should become an
integral part of the personality. The teacher should not merely be a
source of knowledge, rather she should be a source of love and
affection. The school should function as a society. He also gave
his ideas about the use of instructional materials at the pre-school
stage.
Western and Indian philosophers have all stressed the
importance of play in preschool ae a natural occupation of childhood
and as an instrument of learning. Montessori described the child at
play a s being in complete harmony with the basic law of endless
activity which manifests itself in every aspect of nature. Play, by
offering opportunities for success, could be the most powerful agent
in bolstering self confidence and nurturing a positive self concept.
Those early pioneers in the development of educational theory,
Froebel and Pestallozi were both especially sensitive to the
importance of role of play in the early childhood and advocate for
the systematic use of play as part of child rearing and educational
practices (Nair, 1984). Thus play was a spontaneous, voluntary, self
directed activity with out any external compulsions.
If human development was the key to economic development,
child development was the key to human development Rohatgi
(1981). Play was the child's way of learning and developing at his
own rate and in his own manner (Saroja, 1980).
Play represented a window to the child's mind. It was an
inner expression of the inner drama of the mind and the child
discovered how to use the world for his own purposes (Courtenry,
1983). Play was very essential for children because it was through
play that they gain mastery over diflicult concepts such as counting,
adding and multiplying (Muralidharan, 1980; Butler, 1981; Wolf,
1989 and Mowla, 1990).
For a chlld, play was work, he put as much energy, ingenuity
and concentrat~onin h ~ play
s as an adult ctocs lnto his work. Ills
fclt th;ll
lnvolvernent In play was so complcte that 1n:tny author~l~cbs
play was the most direct route to learning (Slnghirni, I!tXcj).
Malani (1984) rightly commented that play which was the
right of every child, was an essential input for the proper growth and
development of the child. During the pre-school years, the child
masters all the skills he requires for adult activities and he will do
this almost entirely through play. (Piers, 1980; Kaplan, 1981 and
Hart, 1985). The importance of play lies in the fact that it gave
maximum opportunities for the expression of freedom and creativity.
(Segal, 1981).
22
An exploratory study of play preferences of pre-school children
in Delhi was conducted by Muralidharan and Benerji (1972). The
sample consisted of 68 children attending pre-school. Observation
technique was used for five days after giving same type of equipment
to all the children. They found that the slides, sandpit and nesting
frames were most popular in out door play. The least popular were
ball, ring, see-saw and jungle gym. Regarding indoor play activities,
crayon drawing and brush painting were found to be most popular.
Block play, paper cutting and collage work was preferred by younger
boys and girls. Beads and constructive materials, puzzles, sorting
materials were found to draw little attention fiom the younger
groups. Number cards, blocks and water play aroused minimum
interest.
Rubin and Waston (1978) conducted a study on free play
behaviours in pre-school and kindergarten children. The sample size
was 15 male and 12 female children in pre-school group and 14 male
and 14 female children in kindergarten. The study revealed that
kindergarten children displayed unoccupied, onlooker, solitary and
functional activity and more group and dramatic play than pre-
schoolers. Examination of the combined pattern on play scale
revealed, preschoolers differed in and choose sigmficantly more
solitary, functional and parallel functional play. Children
participated in less parallel constructed, parallel dramatic and
group dramatic play than their kindergarten counter parts.
Smith and Dutton (1979) have undertook a study on the
relative importance of play opportunities and training experience for
problem - solving in 4 years old children. The children who had
play opportunities were faster than those who lacked training
experience in solving the problems and needed fewer hints. Both set,
of children did better than control children without additional pre-
task experience.
Space arrangements in pre-schools were important to children
to facilitate behavioural changes. This was studied by Sharon
and Sue (1979). On play behaviour of pre-school children in low and
high quality space arrangements a t three places. 11 two year oldu,
14 three year olds, and 14 four year olds were observed during the
first phase in one of three class rooms determined to be low space
quality. Twenty behavioural samples for each ch~ldwcre collected
and recorded in the following categories; slze of the room, child-child
interaction, teacher-child ~nteraction, level of involvement with
materials, and location of play in or out of designated arcas. D u r i n ~
the second phase, the room was arranged to achieve a high quality
space rating, with 20 more behavioural samples per child being
collected in the same manner as before. The third phase consisted of
a return to the low quality space condition, with observations again
being made for each child. Results indicated that room arrangement
did facilitate some changes In children's behaviour, specifically in
24
the category of child-child interaction, there were more verbal
interactions, and less on looking and non-interactive behaviour in
the high quality conditions than in the low quality conditions.
In the category of teacher-child interaction, children were
found to be in a group talking to the teacher more in the high quality
condition than in the low quality condition. There were more
instances of equipment being used in its designated location in the
high quality space arrangement than in the low quality space
arrangement. In terms of involvement with materials, children were
found to use more constructive behaviour in the high quality
conditions. There was significantly less deviant and random
behaviour in the high quality space, as compared to the low quality
conditions. Interactions effects and age a s a factor were reported in
all instances except for level of involvement with materials and use
of materials in or out of designated areas. These findings have led to
the conclusion that teachers can alter children's social interactions
and involvement with materials by manipulating the over all space
quality of a classroom.
Green wood (1981) observed four hundred and sixty one
children (aged three years to seven years eleven months) during
thirty minutes play periods and their activity was compared to
information supplied by their teachers concerning their social
interactive behaviour, pre-school experience and demographic
25
variables. No effects were found for prior pre-school experience.
Topographic analysis indicated that even the lowest interactor in the
sample received and reciprocated interactive bids a t a high
probability level, demonstrating the power of peer social interactions
to produce reciprocal responding. Low interaction and females
typically received more initiations than they made to peers.
Constructive and cognitive play influenced high creativity
in children and problem solving behaviour, this was studied by
Pepler and Ross (1981). They demonstrated a distinction between
convergent problems, which have only one correct solution, and
divergent problems, which have multiple solutions. They found that
playing with puzzle pieces and form boards led to better solving of
convergent puzzle problems. Non - task related, divergent play
(Playing with puzzle pieces as if they were blocks) tpnded to
interfere with the solutions of puzzle tasks. Divergent play did,
however, resulted in a wider variety of problem - solving strattgieu
and facilitated the solving of divergent problem8 such a s using
blocks to build a make believe village.
Vendenberg (1981) examined the impact of differing play
environments, a s well a s cognitive and social egocentrism factors, on
the social play of twenty eight children, of forty-sixty three months
old pre-schoolers. Subjects were allowed free access to two distinctly
different environments containing play materials aimed at
26
exercising either big or fine muscle coordination. Subjects cognitive
abilities and social egocentrism were also measured. Results
indicated that the play environment strongly influenced type of
social play as well as size of the play groups. Differences in cognitive
level and social egocentrism strongly influenced subjects choice of
environment suggesting an organism - environment interaction in
their social play.
Developmental features of children's play with objects was
studied by Vandenberg (1981). In this study five years to ten years
old were given open-ended play materials, and aspects of their play
such as type of material used, types of constructions, and duration of
play were scored. Subjects aged four to five years of age made
several simple constructions using few materials and made little
attempt to integrate desperate types of materials in their
constructions. Subjects aged six to seven years of age made
constructions using more of the materials. Subjects of eight to ten
years made fewer but much more extensive constructions and
integrated desperate types of materials.
Fahndrich and Schncider (1987) conducted a study on
emotional reactions of pre-school children while exploring and
playing with a novel object. They observed forty two children
ranging in age from three years six months to six years two month,
while exploring and playing with a novel object in their pre-schools
for fifteen minutes. Video recordings were made of the subjects
gross motor behaviour and facial movements. Results showed that
interest and joy were the most prominent emotional reactions during
both activities and there was no difference in the amount of interest
shown as measured by facial expression between two activities and
subjects exhibited more joy during play than during explorations.
Another study by Mounts and Roopnarine (1987) observed six
class rooms of three and four years old. Overall there was a decrea-sc
in functional play and an increase in dramatic play and games with
rules over this period. Although there was a decrease in parallel
constructive play, there was increase in solitary constructive play
and associative constructive play such that overall constructive play
did not change in frequency greatly.
As for influence of age on type of play, Takhvar (I!)H7)
observed eight children for four 20 minutes period a week over a
nine month period. The children were aged three and a half to four
a t the start of the observation. Functional play showed a consistent
decrease in occurrence for all the children. Constructive play
showed a n increase in six of the eight children during the last two
months while dramatic play increased in three of the eight children.
Five of the children showed opposite changes in constructive and
dramatic play, three showed similar changes for the two categories.
28
Baruah and Baruah (1998) conducted a study on "Play
activities of pre-school children in East Siang district of Arunachal
Pradesh. 120 pre-school children in the age group of 3-6 years were
selected for the study. The study attempted to find out the play
activities enjoyed by children and also to find out rural, urban
differences in their play activities. The results of the study showed
that rural children enjoyed Gee and spontaneous play having roles
laid by themselves while urban children played with toys and made
appointments before play. Parents of urban children provided new
toys and helped in handling them. Rural parents helped in preparing
play materials from indigenous materials. Teacher's were not aware
of the importance and need of play in the school curriculum.
A study by Kaur (1992) revealed that children in rural areas
engaged predominantly in informal games followed by games with
rules and work cum play. Children often played outside home, on
farms, road sides, school grounds, in and near rivulets with play
differences among boys and girls and school going and non school
going children.
Longitudinal study in play performance would give more clear
picture about impact of play on children's performance. Lenzen
(1999) exploratively investigated play performance in structured
materials (blocks, legos and carpentry) and its relationship to later
school success in mathematics. This longitudinal study began in
1982 with 37 four years old participants. They were selected from
the same pre-school program where they had daily opportunities to
play with construction materials. The participants were assessed in
their play using the Lunzer five point scale in several different kinds
of play including blocks, legos and carpentry. Results of the study
showed that in the early grades and elementary grades, there was
little significance found between a participants play performance
and mathematical acluevement.
Lobo and Lily (2003) examined the use of pedometry to
evaluate the physical activity levels among pre-school children in
Hong Kong. The sample consisted of 86 boys and 62 girls of age
group three, four and five years. The Digi-walker Sw-200 electronic
pedometers were utilized to collect activity data d u r ~ n gdaily regular
physical activities in three pre-schools. School 'A' was located in
rural area, whlle schools B and C were localed in urban areas. An
observation inventory, children activity r a t ~ n gscale (CARS), were
used to categorize the intensity level of the physical activities and to
validate the pedometer counts. Results of the study indicated that
A two (gender) X three (age groups) analy~isof variance indicated
that both main effects were statistically eignificant. Older children
in the school (School A) with a larger outdoor space in a rural
district, were much more active than counter parts in urban schools
(Schools B and C) with limited outdoor space.
In the selection of the sample if the researchers had taken two
rural schools one with larger space and the other with smaller space
for comparison with the two urban schools probably it would have
given better results.
Kara, Kim, Sook, Whiren and Alice (2003) studied the effect of
verbal scaffolding on the complexity of pre-school children's block
constructions. The study had three goals. The first was to
investigate vygotsky's theory in action in the area of block play
specially to determine the effect of adult use of verbal scaffolding on
the structural complexity of children's block construction. The
second goal was to establish a more comprehensive measure for the
complexity of children's block structures. The third goal was to
further establish support for the levels of complexity reported as
stages, arches and dimensionality. Subjects were fifty pre-schoolers
aged 38-71 months in both the experimental and control groups.
Results provided support Vygotsky's proposition that learning leads
to performance and perhaps might also lead to development. When
paired with a more knowing adult, children were able to use higher
levels of complexity in their block structures over all and in the area
of dimensionality.
From the above cited relevant studies it was observed that
functional play, constructive play and dramatic play categories
were more important measures of intelligent and conversation
ability. All these had a positive correlation with pre-school children's
achievement and intelligence.
After considering the significant value of play in the pre-
school children some of the related studies on play with regard to all
round development of the children were listed below.
2. PLAYFOR ALLROUND DEVELOPMENT
Jefrey, Dansky and Silver man (1973) studied effects of
play on associative fluency in pre-school children. The sample
consisted of 90 pre-school subjects from two pre-schools. The
children were randomly assigned to three treatment conditions, such
as play, imitation and control. The findings supported the
hypothesis that playful activity can facilitate associative fluency.
In yet another study, Muralidharan and ot,hcrs (1083)
investigated whether language and cognitive skills could be
inculcated among tribal children by the use of pictures, songs and
games. This study was conducted in Tokapal of Madhyapradesh.
Another objective of the study was to know whether Anganwadi
workers show interest in promoting pre-school education through
learning activities organized around locally developed pictures songs
and games. The sample consisted of twelve anganwadis randomly
selected out of the 62 in the Tokapal project under the ICDS scheme.
The actual intervention ranged from 7 to 11 weeks. Results.showed
that the intervention with experimental group had been a definite
advantage, consequent on the intervention, the test scores of the
experimental group were consistently higher than those of the
control group. The results have not been surprising and were
keeping in with studies in other social settings with adequate
planned interaction. Children gained remarkably in language and
cognitive development through interactions.
Playful precursors of problem solving in pre-schoolers was
studied by Cheyne and Rubin (1983). In this study an analysis was
made on object play components, to determine whether the specific
skills evidenced in the combinational activity of play and / or the
flexible set suggested by the use of fantasy were related to
performance on a problem solving task. The sample consisted of
76 female and 64 male pre-school children. They were permitted to
play for 8 minutes with a number of sticks of varying length and
with a number of blocks. A subset of these materials was
subsequently made available in a problem solving session in
which solution could be achieved by joining sticks to create a tool
to retrieve a lure. Results indicated that play served to create
specific skills as well as a flexible set for innovative thinking and
problem solving.
Leonard, Stacie and Ann (2002) examined group trajectories of
pre-school children's non social play. Three types of pre-school
children's non social play, solitary passive play, solitary active play
and un-occupied behaviours were examined in the study. The
sample consisted of 167 children (86 boys and 81 girls).
Trajectories were compared to static measures of non social play to
determine if children's patterns of play overtime were driven by
the individual differences (i.e., gender, anxiety, regulatory ability)
that have been shown to be associated with non social play in cross
sectional studies. The results indicated that anxious boys used
solitary passive play as coping mechanism to ease social fears,
where as anxious girls tended to regulate their social fears by
remaining on the periphery of social interaction. Those childrtw
engaged in relatively high levels of solitary pi1sslvc play and
unoccupied behaviours were rated as sign1ficantly less popular
than their peers, who were engaged in moderstc or low Irvcls of
these types of play. Solitary active play appeared to be a sign of
social immaturity a t low and moderate levels and an indicator of
poor regulating ability for girls and anxiety for boys when
displayed a t relatively high levels.
The use of play assessment to evaluate the cognitive skills of
two and three year old children was studied by Vance, Ryails,
Brigette, Katrina and Gill (2002). This study explored the use of
34
play assessment with young children to determine its efficacy in
assessing cogmtive levels of development. The sample consisted of
eight 2- years old and eight 3%years old playing in an unstructured
format. Results indicated that exploratory play became more
complex with age and was displayed more than pretend play. In an
unstructured play session, children's play primarily consisted of
exploratory play, pretend play and problem solving and planning
skills. Several of the supplemental sub domains measured by play
assessment were observed not to occur in the spontaneous play
format. Results of this nature cannot be generalized as the sample
was very small.
The impact of peer mediated intervention on pre-school
children's participation in routine child care activities was
investigated by the Robertson, Green, Alper, Schloss and Koher
(2003) targeted three skills for intervention for two children with
reported developmental delays, including engaged on task
behaviour, interactive play with other children, and appropriate
participation in circlelstory time. Results indicated that a peer-
mediated intervention consisted of songs and finger plays, attention
to photographs of children displayed desired behaviours and peer
verbal cues were effective in increasing each of these skills. Further,
both children continued to perform their newly learned skills for up
to six weeks after the termination of intervention. Observations
indicated that both peer trainee exhibited high levels of appropriate
behaviour through out the study. Taking only two children as
sample for the study seems to be inadequate and conclusions drawn
on such a sample could not be considered and studies of this nature
need to be replicated with large sample.
The relation between multiple forms of solitary play and
convergent and divergent thinking skills in pre-school children was
examined by Bronmweri and Nin (2003).The sample consisted of
72 pre-school children, 42 boys and 30 girls in the age group of 4.5
years. Social and cognitive play types and use of materials were
recorded. Children were administered the Thinking Creatively in
Action and Movement test (TCAM) Vorrance, 1981) to nssees
divergent thinking and the PPVT test and the picture completion
subtest WISC-R to measure convergent thinking. h s u l t s ind~cateti
that reticent behaviour was more strongly negatively associated
with convergent and divergent thinking than either solitary - active
or solitary - Passive play, whereas solitary-active play was more
strongly positively related to divergent thinking. Recommendations
were made for supporting solitary play in the early childhood
classroom.
Perhaps more number of such studies are required in
different environments and even among different ethnic groups
with larger sample to recommend solitary play in the classrooms.
Pre-schoolers emotional competence links to pretend and
~hysicalplay was studied by Indsey and Colwelt (2003), with a
sample of 44 pre-school children. The children participated in
emotion understanding interviews. Mothers rated children's emotion
regulation ability and teacher's rated children's emotional
competence with peers. In addition, the amount of children's pretend
and physical play during same sex dyadic play with a friend from
their pre-school classroom was assessed. Findings suggested that
emotion regulation and emotion understanding make unique
contributions to teacher ratings of children's emotional competence
with peers. However different patterns of association were found for
boys and girls. Results indicated that children's emotional
competence with peers might account for association between
children's play and quality of peer relationships.
Peer play interaction was found to demonstrate more
competent emotional regulation, initiation and self determination.
In a study Fantuzzo, Sekino and Cohen (2004), reported the
contributions of interactive peer play to salient classroom
competencies for urban head start children. Dimensions of peer
play were examined concurrently with emotion regulations,
37
autonomy, and language. Children exhibiting high levels of peer
play interaction were found to demonstrate more competent
emotional regulation, initiation, self determination and receptive
vocabulary skills. Positive engagement in play in the early years
were associated with lower levels of aggression, shy withdrawn and
adjustment problems, at the end nf the year. Children who
successfully interacted with peers early in the year evidenced
greater social, cognitive and movement I co-ordination outcomes.
Disruptive and disconnected peer play behaviours were associated
with negative emotional and behavioural outcomes.
Gagnon, Glover and Richard (2004) studied the relationships
between peer interactive play and social competence in a sample of
85 pre-school children who were considered a t risk of academic
difficulties. Ratings of peer interactive play and social emotional
development were collected from parents and tcacher'ti on the penn
interactive peer play scale and (PIPPS) Vineland soc~al,emotional
maturity scale. Multivariate analysis techniques revealed significant
correlation between parent and teachers ralings on the measures.
b s u l t s were discussed within the context of conducting
multifaceted, ecological assessments of pre-school children.
Implications for school psychologists were noted with regard to the
important associations between peer interactive play and social
emotional developments.
In the light of above reported studies, it was observed that
play influenced all areas of development like intellectual, social,
emotional and physical activities. Play was not only an enjoyable
pursuit, but it was also an important device by which children
practice skills, solve problems, socialize, co-operate, discover,
communicate and exercise all the essential abilities, which were to
be developed in childhood.
Studies related to play styles, gender of pre-school children
were listed hereunder.
3. AND GENDER
PLAY STYLES
Alexander and Hines (1994) studied gender labels and play
styles and their relative contribution to children's selection of play
mates. The relative contribution of gender labels and play styles in
playmate selection was evaluated in 60 children between the ages of
4 and 8 years using a novel interview measure. Results indicated
that when target gender labels and target play styles were
presented as competing dimensions, boys of all ages chose female
targets with masculine play styles over male targets with female
play styles. In contrast, younger girls (4-5 years old) chose female
targets with masculine play styles while older girls (6-8years old)
chose male targets with female play styles.
Faber, Martin and Laura (2003) studied children's play
qualities in same- other and mixed sex peer groups. It was
examined how children's play varied as a function of the sex of the
chdd, the sex of the play partner and whether they played in dyads
or groups. The sample consisted of 203 children (97 boys and 106
girls) with mean age of 52 months. They were observed during
free play. Findings confirmed that ch~ldren'sexperiences in peer
groups varied depending on the sex of the child and sex of the play
partners.
In another study, Neppl, Tricia and Kay (1997) investigated
gender differences, social dominance and co-operation in play anlong
pre-schoolers. They wanted to find out the effect on social
dominance when boys and girls play with same and cross gender
play materials. The sample consisted of 90 children in the age
group of 4 and 5 paired into either girl girl, girl-boy and boy-boy
dyads engaged in both a traditionally masculine (cow boy ranch)
and feminine (doll house) stereo typed activity. Ibsults for social
dominance found that children who have a dominant personal~tyled
children with low personality dominance during play. Also when
girls paired with boys girls led more during the feminine than the
masculine activity. Social interactions revealed that girls had
higher amount of co-operative play than boys. While boys had high
parallel play than girls. Also children with both same and mixed
gender pairing displayed more co-operation than in parallel play.
The conclusion on the above mentioned studies is that children
experiences in peer groups varied depending on the gender of the
child and gender of the play partners.
Along with the general effects, researchers were also
interested in finding out the impact of play therapy on the children
with behaviour problems. Studies related on this topic were listed
hereunder.
4. PLAYAS THERAPY
FOR PRESCHOOL
CHILDREN
Not only social psychologists but also Pediatricians accepted
the importance of play in pre-school children in addressing their
behaviour problems. One such study was undertaken by Miho and
Akira (2002). They observed the pre-schooler's aggressive
behaviour in free play in relation to gender and peer group status.
The sample consisted of 16 boys and 18 grls in the age group of four
and five years old pre-schoolers. The pre-schoolers were observed in
a natural setting for one year. A total of 160 aggressive
behaviours were observed and classified in to 3 categories;
Proactivelovert aggression, instrumentau bullying and relational
aggression. Results showed that boys showed more proactive) overt
aggression and instrumental / bullying aggression than girls. While
girls showed more relational aggressive behaviour than boys. In
addition, children were aggressive according to the peer group
41
p+gLj-+p
status. Children who were classified as 'nuclear' in their peer group
had higher frequencies of relational aggressive behaviour. Children
who were other wise classified as 'isolated' suffered more than others
from relational aggression.
P l a y . was critical component of pre-school children
development for children with autism. Restricted play skills
eliminate common tools needed to build independent performance
and peer relationships. A study was initiated by Morrison, Sainato,
Bencheeban, Delia and Endosayaka (2002) to improve the
independent performance of pre-schoolers with autism during
playtime in an inclusive setting. A multiple baseline design across
subjects were employed to determine the effectiveness of
correspondence training and activity schedules on thc on-task and
play correspondence behaviour of four pre-schoolers with autism
aged 42-70 months. Partial interval recording was used to measure
on task behaviour and experimental prompts, where NY a frequency
count was used for on schedule behaviour. Results indicated that
four participants on task and play correspondence behaviour
increased while experimental prompts gradually decrea~ed.
The identification of efficient teaching procedures to address
deficiencies in imaginative play skills which were commonly seen in
children with autism, was a challenge for professionals who were
/ I
6Ys
' I ; ,,
42 If, I%?
designing treatment programs. In a study conducted by
Alenopatrica, Parello, Kathleen, Taylor (2002), video modelling was
used to teach play skills to a pre-school child (female aged 3 years
and eight moths) with autism. Video taped play sequences included
both verbal and motor responses. A multiple base line procedure
across three response categories (tea party, shopping and banking)
were used to demonstrate experimental control. No experimenter
implemented reinforcement or correction procedures were used
during the intervention. Results indicated that the video modelling
intervention led to the rapid acquisition of both verbal and motor
responses in all play sequences. This procedure was shown to be
an efficient technique for teaching relatively long sequence of
responses in relatively few teaching sessions in the absence of
chaining procedures. More such experiments were required on more
number of autistic children to confirm the findings of this study.
Lawrence and Karen (2002) studied care giver and peer
responses to children with language and motor disabilities in
inclusive pre-school programs. The free play social behaviours of
24 children (Aged 30-6.1 months) with special needs in two
discovery - oriented inclusive pre-school programs were compared
with twenty four of their typically developing classmates (age 30-
58 months) who were matched by age and sex. Subjects with special
needs consisted of children with developmental delays and other
disabilities. Subjects were videotaped atleast three times for
approximately twenty minutes each and video's were coded. Results
showed that subjects who used little or no productive language
spent more time in solitary pursuits. They seldom initiated
interactions and when they did, used different strategy than other
groups. Subjects who were incapable of independent locomotion
were largely dependent on adults for initiating changes in their
activities and for social exchanges.
Improving social communication skills of pre-school children
a t risk in a play context was examined by Unkefer and Kaiser
(2002) to find out the effects of a three component intervention on
the social communicative interactions of a six pre-school children
with in the age group of three years five months and three years
one month, a t risk for language delays and behaviour problems. In a
multiple baseline design across three dyads, children wcre taught Lo
(1) plan their play (2) use conversational social interaction
strategies and (3) self evaluate their play interactions. The number
of social communicative behaviours by each chld increased following
introduction of the intervention condition as did their use of
descriptive and request utterances during play sessions. Increase
in linguistic complexity, diversity, and play complexity also were
associated with the intervention. Results suggeated that this
44
intervention hold promise for improving social, linguistic and play
behaviours in pre-schoolers at risk for language delays and
behaviour problems.
Halstadtro and Halstadro (2003) investigated training play
behaviour in a five year old boy with developmental disabilities.
The purpose of the study was two fold (a) To investigate if it was
possible to train three different games (both as a leader and a s
participant) to a child with developmental disabilities through
interaction with four typically developing peers and (b) To
examine if correct response would be maintained after the training
stopped. A multiple probe design across three games were used.
The results showed that the child learned both skills as a leader and
as a participant in all the three games and the skills were
maintained for a period of three weeks after the training had
stopped.
From the above cited related studies it was clear that play
served as therapy for behavioural problems in children like
aggression autism and with developmental disabilities. However
the samples used for study were very small. To draw definite
conclusions and to make generalizations, similar studies need to be
made using large samples.
Since play had a vital role in allround development of the
pre-school chlld. Therefore related studies on play during pre-school
children and their status in pre-schools were listed below.
5. OF PLAY
STATUS IN PRESCHOOLS
Sharma (1970) studied the status of state run pre-schools
when compared to private run pre-schools in Punjab. The state run
pre-schools bear a close resemblance to privately run nursery
schools. When the study was conducted, in the whole of Punjab there
existed 4 nursery schools run by the government. When compared to
private schools there were no play grounds, no flowers, no grass, no
good drinking water or toilet facilities. The fee charged waa RR.201-
to 301- rupees per month. There was no set of syIIabus or courms.
Narayana Reddy (1973) studied the play prefcrcncen of prc-
school children in Tirupati, Andhra Pradesh. 120 pre-school
children were selected from two pre-schools with junior, senior and
pre- primary children, whose ages were four years, four and half
years and five years respectively. He found sex differences in
play preferences in plays like car riding, running, scooter riding,
cycle riding, sand and kitchen thngs. Influence of socio-economic
status of the children was observed on preferences of activities like
running race, scooter riding, horses and ducks, buses, piano and
cars. Age differences were also noted in play preferences of the
outdoor play libe scooter riding, slides, horses and ducks and balls.
Junior nursery school children preferred activities like solid
blocks, nest of boxes, sand tray, buses, toys and boats.
Dorothy (1974) in her article "The Day's child in school"
expressed that children of today were in danger. They were on the
one hand threatened by loss of feeling and imagination on the
other hand they were being drawn early in to initiative, mechanical
robot - like right answers that interfere with independent thinking.
The investigation of Kesavadas and Pavanasam (1978) was
an attempt to study the existing pattern and facilities of pre-
school education in Coimbatore district of Tamil Nadu. The findings
were based on information collected from 80, pre-schools. 50 run by
local bodies and 30 by private bodies. The findings indicated that
most of the pre-schools had maintained a teacher-child ratio of 1:40
to 1:50. Majority of the teachers studied up to SSC or Matriculation.
It was also found, that fifty percent of the schools have no
adequate space for both indoor and outdoor activities. The study
clearly indicated the poor condition of Pre-schools in all the aspects.
Sumathi (1979) conducted a critical survey of pre-schools in
Tirupati town. She found that 75 percent of schools were not having
adequate space. The school buildings were also very small. 62.5
percent of schools were not having any play equipment only 12.5 per
cent were having sufficient equipment. The remaining 25 percent
were having some small equipment like balls, rocking boards, old
tires etcetera. It was also found that many of the pre-schools were
not taking much interest in chddren's play and they aimed mainly
in inculcating discipline among the children
Elizabeth George (1989) conducted a survey in 10 schools in
Trichur district of Kerala. She pointed out that majority of pre-
schools and primary schools do not have adequate play equipment.
Those schools which do have play equipment are reluctant to let
the chddren play with them for the fear that the children may
damage the equipment. Majority of the teachers thought play as a
waste of time. Only one of the ten schools had proper play
equipment and let the children have free access to it. However,
the sample size is small. Studies on n larger sample help to
generalize the findings.
Godalakshmi (1991) evaluated pre-schools in Tirupati town
of Chittoor district. The sample consisted of 50 pre-schools. Including
one Laboratory nursery school and 200 mothers of pre-school
children. Pre-schools were evaluated in terms of physical set up,
curriculum, teacher-child ratio, management administrative
procedures and the relationship of the school with parents. The
information was collected through observation cum interview
schedule, questionnaire and pre-school evaluation scale. ,Results
indicated that majority of the pre-schools were not meeting the
developmental needs of the pre-school children. There seems no
difference between educated and uneducated mothers regarding
awareness of the importance of pre-school education in their child's
life. Both were ignorant. Facilities for both indoor and outdoor play
were absent in most of the pre-schools.
Studies Related to ICDS and Anganwadi
A pilot project was carried out in 1985 by NIPCCD to
construct and identify indicators relevant for monitoring the
ICDS program Thirteen blocks, seven urban, three rural and three
tribal were selected for the study. The study was carried out in
Kerala, Maharastra, Rajasthan, Tamilnadu, Uttarpradesh and
Delhi. The study revealed that a majority of Angawadis were
unsatisfactorily functioning with inadequate equipment for
teaching and playing.
Jain (1985) in his study of thirty eight anganwadi centres
selected at random in Garhi tribal block of Banswada district
concluded that most of the Anganwadis were running at
unsuitable places and physical environment was conducive for the
occurrence of communicable diseases. Majority of Anganwadi
workers were between 21 and 30 years of age group, 52.63% had
middle level education, 84.25%. Anganwadi workers belonged to
scheduled tribes and 63.15% worked purely for economic
compulsions. 37.7 percent of the centers were not provided any
utensils for cooking food and 57.9 per cent centers were not
having formal education kit. Record keeping was inadequate in
7.8 per cent or the centers. It was suggested that Anganwndi
centres should be well equipped with play material and stationary
etcetera. Regulation reorientation, replenishments and guided
supervision would go long way in the successful implementation of
the program.
NIPCCD (1987) conducted a study in Mangalpuri ICIIS project
to assess the implementation of pre-school educational component.
It reported that activities conducted by Anganwadi workers wrrc
not adequately stimulating and most of the Anganwadi workers
spent very little time in interacting with children in a
meaningful way.
A follow up study in 1988 also indicated that the Anganwadi
workers lacked appropriate shlls in utilising low cost locally
available materials for making aids to facilitate pre-school
activity. This necessitates awareness and training workahopa in
learning to prepare and utilize low cost indigenous play materiale.
Ahuja (1988) in a study regarding Anganwadi workers
perception about the contribution made by the available play
materials to child's development observed that almost ninety six
percent of the workers thought that play materials only
contributed towards mental development of the children. But forty
six percent of the workers also felt that it helped both in mental
and physical development.
Several studies have been undertaken since the inception of
the ICDS programme to assess its impact on the targeted groups.
Among these, two recent and relatively -more comprehensive
reviews undertaken by National Institute of public co-operation
and child development (NIPCCD)in 1992 and National Council of
Applied Economic Research (NCAER) in 2001 highlighted the
benefits of the scheme and identified problems and bottle necks.
The overall findings of the NIPCCD report inlcated positive
impact on the health of the pre-school children particularly
immunisation, coverage and nutritional status. Approximately 89
per cent of children with pre-school continued to school as
compared to 52-60 percent of children with out pre-school
experience. The findings of the NCAER report were some what
different from the NIPCCD report as they highlighted that more
than 50% of the eligible children between 3-6 years of age were
51
enrolled for pre-school and of them only half have been retained.
The Incentive for the children to attend Anganwadis continue to be
the nutritional supplement. Scarce availability of play materials
and lack of trained Anganwadi workers have been identified a s
hindrances for the delivery of effective ECCE.
From the above cited studies it was clear that play was in
deporable state in majority of the pre-schools in India and most of
pre-schools have inadequate indoor and outdoor space and lack
physical facilities.
Even the physical facilities are inadequate a good teacher can
help chdd to get more benefit of the pre-school. The teacher in pre-
school should be adequately acquainted with the materials and
should possess teaching skills to cope with a rigidly structured
curriculum. Studies related to play and pre-school teachers were
listed hereunder.
6. PLAYAND PRESCHOOL
TEACHERS
Tiffany (1980) observed eighty white middle class three to
four years old in four day care classrooms varying in teacher-child
ratio b g h and low) and physical layout (large, open spacea
partitioned in to smaller special play area). Several child and
teacher behaviours, peer interactions and fantasy play in the
presence or absence of teachers who did or did not disrupt the
52
interactions were observed. Several sex and group differences
emerged, suggesting that the optimal classroom for facilitating
peer interactions and fantasy play among middle class pre-school
children in a classroom featuring a low teacher child ratio and
partitioned special play areas.
Joanson (1982) suggested that nursery school teachers tend
to intervene frequently in children's play in ways that were
concerned with intellectual development. Joanson suggested that
skilled adult interaction may facilitate and enhance the imaginative
quality of representational play. Skillful interaction required 3
kinds of relationship (incidental, instrumental and intrinsic)
between play and intellectual development. Two styles of
interaction, redirecting and extending styles synchronising with
children's intentions, were educationally profitable and at the
same time valued children's play in its own right.
Seshamma (1985) conducted a study to know the difference
in the attitude of pre-school teacher's towards play and find out
the facilities available in three different kinds of pre-schools. She
concluded that majority of the pre-school teacher's (97% of
Laboratory nursery school teacher's, 97% Anganwadi teachers and
93.8% other pre-school teacher's) felt that play was extremely
essential for the development of the child. Anganwadi workers
53
(80%) and Laboratory nursery school teacher's (100%) had positive
attitude towards organised play activities and play with water,
clay, sand and paints. However all the nursery school teachers had
a negative attitude towards organised play activities. Anganwadis
(93%), Laboratory nursery school (100%) and other pre-schools
(100%) had facilities for play activities associated with learning of
alphabets and numbers like arranging materials such as stones
along a line, educational IQ, watch ABC play, counting stones
without seeing etc.
Anuradha (1985) investigated the attitudes of pre-school
teacher's towards play in pre-school education programme. She
interviewed fifty six teachers belonging to three different types of
pre-schools like laboratory nursery school, Anganwadi centre^ of
ICDS and other private pre-schools. The results were compared
with the training of the teachers in the three different pre-schools.
She found that majority of the teacher's felt that play was
very essential for pre-school children irrespective of training of the
teachers and irrespective of the philosophy of the school. From the
data it was found that the teachers who expressed play as not
necessary haven't undergone any training but it does not seem to
be a reason for their opinion because other teachers who also have
not undergone any training expressed play a s essential component
in Pre-schools. Depending upon her findings she concluded that
neither the training nor the philosophy of the school makes the
teacher's feel play as essential but it is their opinion as individuals
depending upon their past childhood experiences.
Berg and Samuelsson (2003) studied pre-school teacher's play
experiences then and now. Many pre-school teacher's recognised
the importance of play for children's development and learning.
T h s study explored how teacher's remembered their own childhood
play and how they perceived children's play today. The sample
consisted of 20 pre-school teacher's. They were interviewed
regarding their views on play. Two perspectives were identified, the
idealized and the pragmatic. Findings suggested that the idealized
perspective was more common than the pragmatic among the pre-
school teacher's. Two different themes from childhood stand out as
significant in the comparison of play in the past to the role of play
today, time for play and the effect of media on play.
Virginia, Fantuzo and John (2003) examined whether the
Penn interactive peer play scale (PIPPS), a teacher rating
instrument of interactive play behaviours for early childhood was
valid for urban, low income children in kindergarten. The study
yielded three dimensions of interactive peer play, play interaction
and play disruption and play disconnection. These constructs
were congruent with the dimensions found for pre-school children.
Concurrent validity was demonstrated with a standardized
instrument assessing global social skills and academic competence.
Children who displayed highly interactive peer play were given high
ratings by teachers for social slulls and were ranked higher in the
class for academic competence. Those children who were disruptive
or disconnected in play were viewed by teacher's as having more
problem behaviours and had lower academic achievement as
compared to the peers. Results indicated children who were
reported by their teacher's to have effective peer interactions during
play had higher teacher ratings of academic success than children
who were considered disruptive or disconnected in play.
Prakash and Kavita (2003) explored the interface between
young children's socio emotional characteristics and the nature of
their interactions with teacher's. The sample consisted of 135 pre-
school children (62 males and 73 femaleti). Play behaviour was
observed during teacher's supervision of free play with peers.
Results indicated that children who most frequently initiated
interactions with teacher's were rated by the teacher's as being more
aggressive than their peers, In constrast, children who most
frequently received initiations form teachers were more shy and
anxious than their peers. Finally children who spent less time
with the teacher's were more sociable, less solitary, and evidenced
fewer behaviour problems than peers who interacted more
frequently with the teacher's. It is one interesting finding
suggesting less interaction of the Pre-school children with their
teachers was more beneficial to the child. More such studies were
required to confirm or to negate these findings.
Today through out the world education has become most
important input in the development of human resource and India
is no exception. Educational institutions from elementary schools to
Universities and Professional colleges are mushrooming through
out the country. Parents expectations on their children have
increased many folds and they believe that the Pre-schools lay the
foundation for their wards further education. Some studies
conducted regarding parental expectations from pre-school education
were reviewed below.
7. PARENTS OF PLAYI N PRE-SCHOOL
EXPECTATIONS EDUCATION
Suresh (1970) carried out a study on a group of twenty
parents to find out their opinion and expectations regarding pre-
school education. They were provided with six choices regarding
their expectations of children's achievement in pre-school.
Findings indicated that majority of the parents expressed that the
pre-school programme attended to the needs of the children. The
parents expected the child to be independent, able to count
numbers, acquire reading skills and also wanted their children to
learn indirectly from play. Most of the parents showed their
willingness to accompany the child to the pre-school in the beginning
and stay there if necessary.
Manjula (1973) enquired in to the attitude of the parents
towards pre-school education. For this study forty urban and ten
rural parents were selected. Results indicated that majority of the
parents gave top priority to strict discipline, obedience and were
interested in formal education. A few parents in the urban area
considered too much play was bad for pre-school children. Most of
the parents were positively disposed towards parents teachers co-
operation.
Sarala Kumari (1976) also conducted simllar ~ t u d y ,play -
preferences of nursery school children. The tichool selected for the
study was Sri Venkateswara University laboratory nursery school,
I'lrupati, Chittoor district. She found age and sex differences in the
play preference of pre-school children. Influence of socio-economic
status on play preference was also seen. Majority of the parent8
were providing opportunities to their children to play. Thua parent
child relationships were positive in encouraging the child to play.
Masur and Gleason (1980) studied parent child interaction and
the acquisition of lexical information during play. Individual play
interactions of parents with their pre-school aged boys and girls
were examined to determine the ways mothers and fathers provided
and elicited lexical information about names and functions of the
parts of a complex toy car. The sample consisted of 14 pairs of
parents and their first born pre-school aged children, seven boys and
seven girls. Results indicated that parent child interaction during
play suggested that fathers as well as mothers may exert an active
and direct influence'on the language development of pre-school aged
girls and boys.
Sarada (1989) studied the opinions of mothers of pre-school
children towards play. The sample consisted of 200 private pre-
school children and their mothers. Interview schedule was used to
elicit opinion of the mothers. Results indicated that play was very
necessary for allround development of the child. Almost all the
mother's expressed that play was necessary for this age group. But
the knowledge about play and its relation in all around development
of the child, was very low among the less educated mothers. Where
as mothers with higher education were aware of the importance of
play in the lives of their children.
Julia and Livy (2003) investigated parental reports of pre-
school children's social behaviour, relations among peer play,
language competence and problem behaviour. A sample of 113 pre-
school children aged 33.4 and 67.2 months enrolled in a head
start program. Parent's and teacher's provided information on the
above mentioned three aspects. A subset of these children (n = 34)
who participated in a second year of this early intervention pre-
school ~rograrnwas assessed at Cmonths follow up. Correlationsl
analysis confirmed relations between parent ratings of play
behaviour and teacher ratings of play performance at time one.
Parent ratings of play were also related to assessments of children's
language competence at the beginning of the following school year
where a s parent ratings of problem behaviour were not related to
subsequent language competence. The findings provided additional
validity for a parent measure of school children's peer play and
support, the need for greater inclusion of parental assessment of
young children's social behaviour during early childhood.
8. VALUEOF PLAYEQUIPMENTAND MATERIALS
No studies have been found regarding the value of play
equipment and materials. However an attempt has been made
to summarize the opinions and views of different authors.
Young (1985) stated that play equipment and accessory toys
should help stretch growing musclee and imagination. Singh (1982)
pointed out that picture puzzles, or wooden blocks were essential for
developing creative and for ability and dolls or utensils for
60
dramatising adult life activities. Hammond (1985) remarked. that
puzzles provide opportunities for eye hand co-ordination, use of
small muscles, recognition of shape, colour and use of visual
memory. Puppetry had been found to be useful to restore self-
confidence, particularly among withdrawn children (Naid, 1984).
Children think, plan and create while playing. They learn in an
atmosphere of delight and pleasure. Riley (1983) com.mented that
play was the dynamic tool for learning, growing and the integration
of experiences. Through play children train themselves for useful
work when they grow up. Play was expected to help to build a
healthy figure and a sound mind. It was also essential for the
development of a normal, well adjusted personality.
Out door play equipment like jungle gym, slide, swing, rope
ladders, tricycle, bicycle and other outdoor play equipment helped
the pre-school children to develop large muscles through the
activities like running, jumping, hopping, skipping, swinging.
Indoor play equipment and materials like wooden or plastic
construction sets, threading beads, etc. helped in the development of
finer muscles. It helps in eye hand co-ordination. Play seemed
to help in the development of bones and internal organs.
Mohanty (1996).
Play equipment like peg boards, hammer etc, helped to release
surplus energy. Play equipment and materials enabled the child to
learn shapes, sizes, colours, textures, numbers and qualities of
objects as well as their significance. Exploring, collecting and
experimenting with play things furnish the child with information
that cannot be obtained by any other means. (Vandenberg, 1981).
Play offers an outlet for the child to express his natural
instincts and emotions. It was vital in preparing small boys and
girls to be confident and to be able to express themselves. Toys serve
the purpose of companionship. Children had been known to sleep
with a well loved stuffed toy, just to keep some of their imagined
fears away from them. (Battacharya, 1985)
Play helps the child to comprehend and control the world in
which he lived and to distinguish between reality and fantasy. Play
was an activity which was all absorbing and was expected to give
pleasure and represented some degree of creativeness and was not
concerned with the attainment of an end result apart from the
activity it self. (Hurlock, 1986).
Muralidharan (1972) stated that play helped the child to
adjust in society. Through games with peers and adults, the child
learnt to give and take, co-operate, be tolerant and ultimately make
better soclal adjustment. Play has therapeutic value in that it acted
62
as a catharsis for the release of pent up feelings. Play therapy had
been employed in dealing with children's behaviour problems.
Autistic children have uttered their first words in response to the
tape-recorded conversations of a doll.
Play gave children a chance to be a good sport and helped
children learn to follow rules and to be a good loser or winner.
Exercise during play helped the children to develop good appetite
and sleep. (Devadass and Jaya, 1981).
Thus value of play in child's life was well recorded and it was
important part of the child's pre-school education. During pre-
school period, with play activities the child becomes very active and
energetic and likes to spend most of the time in pre-schools only.
Indoor and outdoor play material and equipment enables the child
to develop physically, intellectually, socially and emotionally in the
right path.
OVERVIEW
The review of extensive literature about play and its important
role in preschool children has made it possible to make certain
salient observations that would be used in proceeding further with
the investigation of present problem. Some studies used very small
number of subjects and some studies gave laborious observation
procedure. The following were the main observations of the above
reported studies.
Play serves to create specific skills for innovative thinking and
problem solving.
Exploratory play became more complex with age and was
displayed more than pretend play.
Play has been receiving the attention of researchers, who are
investigating the relationship of play to cognitive, language
and gross motor development.
Play equipment helped large growing muscles and
imagination.
Play was expected to help healthy body and sound mind and
well adjusted personality of the child.
The value of play cannot be over emphasized.
Play helped children to develop good appetite and sleep.
Children who had play opportunity, had expreusion for
freedom and creativity.
Availability of space improved child-child interaction.
Education and program in class room can influence play
development.
Constructive measures of play had a positive correlation.
Parents of urban children provided new toys where as rural
parents helped children in preparing play materials from
indigenous materials.
Playful activity can facilitate associate fluency.
Children's emotional competence with peers might account for
association between children's play and quality of peer
relationships.
Disruptive and disconnective peer play behaviours were
associated with negative emotional and behaviour out comes.
Important play associations were found in social and
emotional development.
Children's experience in peer group varied depending on the
sex of the child and the sex of the play partners.
Play served as therapy for children with autism 1 behavioural
problems.
Children at risk of language and behaviour problem could be
helped by intervention to improve social, linguistic and play
behaviour.
Children learnt and retained the skills even after 3 weeks of
the training.
Teacher-pupil ratio varied from 1:20 to 1:40.
Several studies of pre-schools found poor conditions in the
pre-schools.
Teacher's opinion on play was not influenced by their training
but it was influenced by their childhood experiences.
Though mothers realized, that play was important for pre
schoolers, their educational level iduenced their knowledge
about the usefulness and value of play.
In the Indian context the studies showed that majority of the
preschools and parents had not yet realized the importance of play
for preschool children. The preschools concentrate more on teaching
three 'K s (Reading, writing and Arithmetic) to the children. The
management and teachers in many preschools neither know the
importance of the play nor they provide sufficient space for the
children to play.
Based on these observation the problem for investigation was
defined. An extensive review of literature has made it poasible for
presentation of some salient observations. All. these observations
were used in selection of variabies and formation of reeearch
questions and hypotheses for experimental verification. The
objectives and hypotheses formulated were presented in the
following chapter.