Addiction, Personality and Motivation
Addiction, Personality and Motivation
It is suggested that addictive behaviour, so called, ®ts into a psychological resource model. In other words, the habits
in question are acquired because they serve a useful function for the individual, and the nature of the functions they
ful®l is related to the personality pro®le of the `addict'. For some people this resource function develops into a form
of addiction, and it is suggested that the reason this occurs is related to excessive dopamine functioning. This in turn
is used to suggest the nature of the addictive personality. Excessive dopamine functioning is related to the person-
ality dimension of psychoticism, and evidence is cited to the eect that psychoticism is closely related to a large
number of addictions. The precise reasons for the addictive eects of dopamine are still being debated, but clearly
there is a causal chain linking personality and biological factors together in the production of addictive behaviour.
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 12: S79±S87, 1997.
No. of Figures: 3. No. of Tables: 0. No. of Refs: 61.
INTRODUCTION: THE RESOURCE MODEL these run as follows: (1) want; (2) freedom of
OF ADDICTION choice; (3) psychological dependence; (4) physical
The term `addiction' is widely used to characterize dependence, increased tolerance, escalation of
the tendency to indulge in certain types of dosage, withdrawal, craving; (5) moral deterior-
behaviour to an unusual and possibly harmful ation; (6) intellectual reduction; (7) mental dissolu-
extent, addicts often ®nding it dicult or impos- tion; (8) social collapse. He points out that while
sible to terminate such behaviour without outside alcohol and drugs ®t all but one of these (freedom
help, or even with it. Such behaviour often involves of choice), smoking does not; nor does it remove
drugs (alcohol, amphetamine, cocaine, heroin, freedom of choice. This does not remove the
etc.), but not necessarily so; in popular parlance possibility that alcohol and drugs may also have
one can be addicted to sex, sport, pornography, a resource component; the glib use of the term
travel, or work (workaholics). There are two major `addiction' for habituation serves no useful pur-
models of addiction, the medical or chemical pose. It may have some meaning if applied to
(physical addiction) and the psychological (resource certain drugs, and to certain people. No general-
model). As Gilbert (1995) and Warburton (1990) ization should be oered without speci®c proof
have pointed out, the term `addiction' has little covering Voss's eight points. These cover what we
scienti®c meaning, being employed in dierent might call `genuine' or `medical chemical' addi-
ways by dierent writers, and having no agreed tions; in this paper we are using the term in a much
interpretation or underlying theory. It is not even broader, non medical sense.
known whether addictions (using the term in its The view taken here is that the term `addiction'
widest, common sense meaning) is speci®c to one refers to certain types of behaviour that can be
substance or activity, or general, i.e. covering interpreted as constituting a resource for the person
several dierent areas. Often the term is used in a concerned; in other words, the behaviour confers
pejorative sense to suggest that the behaviour in certain bene®ts on that person, and hence the
question is a form of disease, requiring medical behaviour in question is continued even though
intervention. Voss (1992) has given a list of the there may be certain unwanted consequences,
criteria for distinguishing between habituation (or usually occurring only in a statistical fashion
resource use) and true or medical clinical addiction; (risk ratios), and after a considerable period of
CCC 0885±6222/97/S20S79±09$17.50
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
S80 H. J. EYSENCK
time. As an example, consider smoking. I have i.e. a type of person who is readily addicted to
argued that nicotine has a biphasic action, increas- certain types of behaviour which are reinforcing,
ing cortical arousal in smaller doses, and decreas- and will continue to indulge in these behaviours
ing tension in larger doses (Eysenck, 1980). These even after the circumstances giving rise to them
eects can be reinforcing, the former in extraverts have changed. It is this possibility that is being
attempting to raise their abnormally low level of discussed in this paper.
cortical arousal, the latter in emotionally unstable In this sense of there existing an `addictive
people attempting to lower their tenseness. This personality', we would expect genetic factors to
analysis suggests that smoking may be related to play an important role, because genetic factors are
personality, in the sense that people high on extra- known to be a major determinant of practically all
version or neuroticism are more likely to smoke known personality traits, and because the major
than people low on either or both these personality dimensions of personality implicated in addiction
traits (Eysenck, 1980). As Gilbert (1995) has in particular are known to have high heritabilities
shown, both propositions have found considerable (Eaves et al., 1989). Turner et al. (1995) have
support in a number of empirical studies. discussed genetic approaches in behavioural
It would seem to follow that if people smoke to medicine in detail, and there seems to be little
receive certain bene®ts from smoking (resource doubt about the involvement of genetic factors in
theory), they would continue in this behaviour alcoholism (Cardoret et al., 1985; Searles, 1988;
because it was reinforcing, and it would be dicult McGue, 1995), and smoking (Eaves and Eysenck,
to wean them away from it. The problems encount- 1980; Rowe and Linver, 1995; Heath and Madden,
ered by most `quit smoking' programmes bear 1995). Eating disorders and obesity, too, have been
testimony to this; initial successes are usually shown to have a genetic basis (Cardon, 1995;
followed by large scale returns to smoking by Meyer, 1995; Spelt and Meyer, 1995). There is no
many subjects of such trials. It would also seem to direct evidence that identical genes are involved in
follow that if we could oer smokers alternative dierent types of addiction, but if they are, then
ways of obtaining the type of satisfaction they similar personality factors should appear in con-
obtain from smoking, e.g. by teaching high nection with each.
neuroticism scorers relaxation methods to reduce What is meant by `personality' here is much
tenseness, the eect on smoking would be stronger more than just a characterization of a person in
and more lasting. O'Connor and Stravinski (1982) terms of traits of one kind or another. Figure 1 will
have demonstrated that this is indeed so, thus make it clear that psychometric traits do indeed ®ll
giving strong support to the resource theory. the centre of the picture, but such trait character-
ization is only part of a much larger nomological
network (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985). There is
PERSONALITY AND ADDICTION much evidence that all aspects of personality are
Can we extend such a personality type theory to strongly determined by genetic factors (Eaves et al.,
the problem of addiction? Obviously some people 1989). DNA cannot, of course, aect behaviour
®nd it easier than others to give up addictive directly, and hence we have biological intermedi-
sources of grati®cation. Many US soldiers acquired aries (proximal antecedents) linking DNA and
the habit of smoking opium in Vietnam, but had behaviour. Theories of personality can be tested in
no diculty giving it up on their return; others the experimental laboratory (proximal conse-
became hopeless addicts. One possible dierence quences), and ®nally give rise to predictions
may be found in the circumstances encountered by involving social behaviour (distal consequences).
the people concerned. Many people took up The alleged `addictive behaviours' would fall into
smoking during the war because of the stress this last category, and hence would require not only
involved, and had no diculty in giving it up after a link with psychometric personality traits, but also
the war, because the stress was removed. A resource with biological antecedents. We have tried to go
model can easily explain such examples of quitting some way towards ®lling in the various parts of
made easy by changing circumstances. However, such a systemic view.
clearly this is not enough, because `addictive' The ®rst step in such a search for causal connec-
people remain wedded to their addiction in spite tions must be an inductive one, namely a search for
of changing circumstances. This raises the possibi- personality correlates of addiction. There are three
lity that there may exist an `addictive personality', major dimensions of personality, P (psychoticism),
HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
ADDICTION, PERSONALITY AND MOTIVATION S81
E (extraversion) and N (neuroticism); these are may be useful in introducing it. The underlying
uncorrelated with each other, and cover dierent theory states that there is a dimension of person-
areas of personality (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985). ality which relates to a person's liability to func-
As we shall see, it is particularly the psychoticism tional psychosis, as shown in Figure 2 (Eysenck,
dimension that has been found to be correlated 1992). Psychoticism measures a dispositional vari-
with addictive behaviour, and hence a few words able; P has to be combined with stress to produce
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997)
S82 H. J. EYSENCK
actual psychiatric symptoms. We are dealing this scale, addicts had mean scores almost twice as
throughout with non-psychotic individuals in our high as controls (Gossop and Eysenck, 1980).
studies, but of course the biological substratum of P The personality patterns of criminals are similar
would have to be similar to, or identical with, that to those of drug addicts, particularly in having high
of schizophrenia to make the theory acceptable. P and N scores (Eysenck and Gudjonsson, 1989).
Gray et al. (1991) have argued that there is indeed Gossop and Eysenck (1983) tested 221 drug addicts
such a similarity as we shall discuss presently. and over 1000 criminals on the P, E, N and L scales.
The actual traits which intercorrelate together to They found addicts higher on P, lower on E, higher
make up the higher-order factor of psychoticism on N (particularly the women), and lower on L. In
are shown in Figure 3; the evidence for the exist- other words, the dierences in personality patterns
ence of such a factor, and the evidence for its are similar to those obtained with normal controls.
identi®cation as psychoticism, are given elsewhere These studies were done with traditional drug
(Eysenck, 1992). Is it true that addictive behaviour takers. Smokers, if we are willing to consider them
is largely determined by P, and to a smaller extent `addicted' in the sense of continuing to smoke
by N (neuroticism)? Early studies by Gossop cigarettes in spite of many health warnings, have
(1978) and Teasdale et al. (1971) showed that been found to have high-P scores, and it may be
drug-dependent groups had typically high levels of noted that nicotine is an indirect dopamine agonist
psychoticism, together with elevated scores on (Spielberger and Jacobs, 1982; Gilbert, 1995). The
neuroticism; they also had somewhat lower levels relevance of this point will be made clear below;
of extraversion than controls. just note that dopamine plays an important part in
A larger and more detailed study comparing the Gray et al. (1991), as well as in other theories of
drug addicts and controls was carried out by schizophrenia.
Gossop and Eysenck, (1980) who found that for As far as alcoholism goes, two dimensions
both males and females high-P was an important appear to be relevant to its aetiology (Sher, 1991;
discriminant, with high neuroticism (N) also McGue, 1995). The ®rst resembles psychoticism,
important, but less so for women than for men. with characteristics like impulsivity, inattention
They also suggested that the high-N scores might and character disorders. The second is neuroticism,
have been in¯ated for various reasons. Low extra- or `negative emotionality', with a tendency to
version (E) scores were also again characteristic of experience negative moral states and psychological
drug addicts. The test used also contained a Lie distress.
Scale (L) which essentially measures conformist Rather more interesting and unusual is work
behaviour, and usually correlates negatively with P; with bulimics who have been suggested to share
low L scorers were characteristic of the drug many similarities with addicts (Garrow et al.,
addicts. On the basis of these results, the authors 1975). The outcome was a clear con®rmation of
constructed an addiction scale consisting of the the hypothesis, with patients having higher P and
32 most discriminating items (all at p < 0001). On N scores, and lower E and L scores, than controls
HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
ADDICTION, PERSONALITY AND MOTIVATION S83
(Feldman and Eysenck, 1986). The study was con®rms a clear relationship between neuroticism
repeated by Silva and Eysenck (1987), with similar and the use of drugs and alcohol'. However, `the
results, comparing 59 female patients suering from relationship between extraversion and the use of
anorexia nervosa with 122 bulimics; the bulimics drugs and alcohol is much less clear'. Francis adds
score signi®cantly higher than the anorexics on P his own rather novel investigation of personality
and N, and lower on L. On the addiction scale they and attitude towards substance use among 13±
also scored signi®cantly higher. Another unusual 15-year-old children, using a large sample of
sample was made up of gamblers (Blaszczynski 19 349 subjects. A negative attitude to drug usage
et al., 1985), who had signi®cantly higher P and N correlated ÿ034 with P, 033 with L, ÿ016 with
scores than controls. E and ÿ003 with N. Controlling for sex slightly
Observed personality characteristics of drug raised the correlation for P, L and E, leaving that
addicts are not culturally determined but can be for N unchanged. In so far as attitude is predictive
observed in other cultures as well as in Europe. A of use and abuse, these ®gures support ®ndings on
Saudi Arabian group of drug addicts was tested by addiction, except for the low values for N.
Abu-Arab and Hashem (1995), showing again the Given that P is the major element in addiction,
same high P-high N patterns observed in European it may be worthwhile to enquire about the preva-
subjects. These authors also refer to another study lence of addiction in two large groups character-
by Abu-Arab (1987), showing similar correlations ized by high P scores, namely criminals (Eysenck
with alcoholism (see also Hurlburt et al., 1982). and Gudjonsson, 1989) and creative people and
In a recent study, Mann et al. (1995) used the geniuses (Simonton, 1994). It is hardly necessary to
NEO Personality Inventory (Costa and McCrae, discuss in detail the close relationship between
1991), which has two scales (A Ð agreeableness addiction and criminality; this is too well known to
and C Ð conscientiousness) which have a high require elaboration. As regards creative artists and
negative correlation with P; they also have scales scientists, the evidence has been reviewed by
for E and N. They compared a group of addicts Simonton (1994). Note also that excessive dopa-
with controls, and found the expected dierences, mine functioning has also been found in criminals
with addicts lower on A and C, and also on E, but (Raine, 1993; Masters and McGuire, 1994). These
higher on N. I have been unable to ®nd any studies links are at present merely correlational, and would
of addiction that found results in a direction surely deserve closer study, particularly as concerns
opposite to that indicated, i.e. high P, high N, causal mechanisms.
low L and possibly low E. Sex dierences do not
change this pattern, but women have less elevated
N dierences. Particularly impressive are the BIOLOGICAL ANTECEDENTS
universally high P scores of addicts, as demanded OF ADDICTION
by our theory. We must now turn to the biological antecedents
These are just a few of the early studies using the which characterize addiction; if our theory cannot
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire scales. Francis accommodate such ®ndings, then clearly it cannot
(1996) has listed all available studies for addiction serve the unifying function hoped for it. In what
to alcohol, opium, heroin, benzodiazepines, etc.; in follows I shall rely very much on a theory put
all, he found 19 studies speci®cally linking P and forward by Joseph et al. (1996), based as it is on
addiction, and 23 linking N and addiction. (The much empirical work. As they point out, drugs
larger number of studies using N is due to the fact often associated with abuse and addiction char-
that many more investigators used N scales than P acteristically share the feature of being able to
scales). Extraversion gave 10 negative and two increase neurotransmission in the mesolimbic dopa-
positive correlations with addiction, as well as mine system. This system ascends from the neural
12 studies without signi®cant results. The Lie Scale tegmental area in the midbrain to the limbic areas
shows seven studies giving negative correlations associated with emotions, including the nucleus
with addiction, two with positive, and three with accumbens (NAc) and the amygdala. Di Chiara and
insigni®cant correlations. Imperato (1988) have shown that various addictive
Francis summarized his survey of addictive drugs, such as amphetamine, cocaine, nicotine,
behaviour by saying that the literature `con®rms morphine and alcohol aect extracellular dopamine
that psychoticism is a key personality factor in this levels in the NAc of the rat. The data suggest
area'. Furthermore, `the majority of studies also innervation from the AIO nucleus in the ventral
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997)
S84 H. J. EYSENCK
tegmental area, and leave little doubt that all of 1988; Stolerman, 1992). What renders these results
these drugs of abuse can activate the mesolimbic interesting is that NAc activity is also increased
system at low doses. This is so although these during and ends shortly after food reward, water
common eects are brought about by diverse reward, and sexual activity in male rats. These are
pharmacological action of the drugs in question. all rewarding, in the sense that they reinforce
The relationship between addiction and dopa- (increase) the likelihood of behaviour associated
mine activity is given further support by a recent with them Ð in other words, animals will work to
study (Fowler et al., 1996). They report that brains obtain access to these reinforcers. Stimuli asso-
of living smokers show a 40 per cent decrease in the ciated with these reinforcers also acquire secondary
level of monoamine oxidase relative to non reinforcing properties through a simple Pavlovian
smokers, or former smokers. They draw attention conditioning process. Such secondary rewards are
to the fact that MAO is involved in the breakdown also associated with dopamine activity in the NAc
of dopamine. `MAO inhibition is therefore associ- (Damsma et al., 1992; Phillips et al., 1993). This
ated with enhanced activity of dopamine' (p. 733). combination of ®ndings might suggest a simple
The authors remark on the prevalence of smoking resource theory: drugs of abuse produce an
in psychiatric disorders, and of course low increase in dopamine in the NAc. Addiction occurs
MAO levels are associated with psychopathological because the drugs involved produce stronger
behaviour (Zuckerman, 1991). What is important reinforcing eects in the brain systems of high-P
for our theory is that dopamine functioning has (dopamine active) people than those of low-P
been linked experimentally with high P scoring (dopamine inactive) people.
(Gray et al., 1994); this is the fundamental link Wise and Rompre (1985) have reviewed the huge
between personality and biological reality. literature on brain dopamine and reward. They
Von Knorring and Oreland (1985) lend further point out that `the evidence is strong that dopamine
support to the involvement of MAO in addiction in plays some fundamental and special role in the
a paper which studied the smoking habits of an rewarding eects of brain stimulation, psycho-
unselected series of 1129 18-year-old men from the motor stimulants, opiates, and food' (p. 270); they
general population. `Regular smokers were found go on to say that dopamine is not the only reward
to be extraverts, sensation seekers who were easily transmitter, and that dopaminergic neurons are not
bored and with a strong tendency to avoid mono- the ®nal common path for all rewards. They
tony. They also had a lower than average intellec- conclude that `in all likelihood, the dopamine
tual level, and were more prone to the abuse of system plays some very general role in mood and
alcohol, glue, cannabis, amphetamine and mor- movement, a role that is essential to reward
phine. Furthermore, they had a low platelet MAO function as well as to other aspects of motivated
. . . Ex smokers had personality traits, intellectual behavior' (p. 221).
levels and platelet MAO of the same magnitude as There are problems associated with such a
non smokers: this may be the reason why they were theory. In the ®rst place many animal studies have
able to give up smoking' (p. 327). This study shown that stresses and aversive stimuli of various
strongly supports the hypothesis of a generally sorts are also associated with increased dopamine
addictive person, and connects this addictive release in the NAc (e.g. Young et al., 1992;
personality to low platelet MAO. The personality Saulskaya and Marsden, 1995). Thus not only
traits involved pertain to P as much or more than rewarding but also punishing stimuli produce
to extraversion; it is regrettable that the P scale was increased dopamine release in the NAc. And, as
not used in this study. in the case of rewarding stimuli, secondary aversive
These data suggest the existence of a general properties acquired through a process of Pavlovian
basis for addiction, in enhanced dopamine func- conditioning also gain the ability to increase
tioning, regardless of what substance is involved. dopamine activity in the NAc (Young et al.,
One possible hypothesis to explain this general- 1992). Thus, as Joseph et al. (1996) point out,
ization is based on the fact that the mesolimbic `while all rewarding stimuli so far studied increase
dopamine system is involved with the mechanism DA activity in the NAc, it is by no means the case
of reward and reinforcement. Studies involved in that all stimuli which increase DA activity in the
this type of research relate to experiments on NAc are rewarding' (p. 58).
self stimulation, drug self administration, and Another problem with a simple reward theory is
conditioned place preference (Fibiger and Phillips, that neutral stimuli presented in a regular temporal
HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
ADDICTION, PERSONALITY AND MOTIVATION S85
association, resulting in the formation of a con- novel) stimuli produce an orienting reaction which
ditioned association, as indexed in the sensory pre- increases cortical arousal levels. Increased DA
conditioning paradigm, are also associated with release will reduce the probability that stimuli will
increased DA activity in the NAc, while the be ignored as familiar and non productive. Again,
identical stimuli presented in a non-associated as before, we must note that much further work will
manner are not (Young et al., 1995). Thus, as be required (perhaps by looking at indices of
Joseph et al. (1996) summarize the evidence, cortical arousal) before this hypothesis is regarded
`increased DA activity in the NAc is associated as acceptable. If reduction in latent inhibition does
with primary and secondary motivational stimuli, indeed raise cortical arousal, this might account for
whether rewarding or aversive, and with associa- the fact that low extraversion is sometimes corre-
tions between neutral stimuli which result in lated with addiction; low arousal is the major
conditioning. Perhaps the common factor here is psychophysiological precursor of extraversion, and
that all of these stimuli, or con®guration of stimuli, heightening arousal is regarded as desirable by
are salient to the animal' (p. 58). extraverts. High P scorers, of course, also have low
The fact that dopamine has several dierent arousal levels.
eects does not necessarily rule out the possibility The dierent hypotheses mentioned here to
that it is its eect on the pleasure centres that is account for dopamine action increasing addictive
crucial for addictive behaviour. Aversive and behaviour are not antagonistic to each other. It is
conditioning eects may be irrelevant to addiction; quite possible that all are correct, and operate to a
the complexity of neurotransmitter activity is well diering degree in dierent people, depending on
known. But in order to maintain this theory we their position on P, E and N. There seems little
would require more experimental support than is doubt that personality plays a prominent part in
available at the moment. Possibly conditioning relation to addiction, regardless of the type of
provides another plausible bridge between addic- addiction, and that dopamine plays a large media-
tion and dopamine, in that addictive behaviours ting role between DNA and personality, particu-
are conditioned behaviours, with the positive eects larly P. These facts suggest the direction in which
of the addictive behaviours acting as uncondi- future research may go with advantage; there are
tioned response variables reinforcing these beha- plenty of promising hypotheses to keep such
viours more strongly in people more likely to form research going.
strong conditioned responses.
Joseph et al. (1996) put forward a rather dierent
theory, linked with latent inhibition (LI) (Lubow, REFERENCES
1989). LI refers to an experimental two-stage Abu-Arab, M. (1987). Some correlates of personality in
arrangement in which a neutral stimulus is pre- a group of alcoholics. Protialkoholicky Obzor, 22, 36.
sented a number of times without any contingent Abu-Arab, M. and Hashem, E. (1995). Some personality
reinforcement, e.g. a sound is presented randomly correlates in a group of drug addicts. Personality and
while the subject is carrying out an (irrelevant) Individual Dierences, 19, 649±653.
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. and Gray, J. (1988). Latent
learning task. Later on this neutral stimulus is used inhibition and `psychotic proneness' in normal sub-
as the CS (conditioned stimulus) in a proper jects. Personality and Individual Dierences, 9, 777±
conditioning experiment, and it is found that this 783.
pre-exposure impairs the ability of the stimulus to Blaszczynski, M., Buhrich, N. and McConaghy, N.
become conditioned to any UCS. Latent inhibition (1985). Pathological gamblers, heroin addicts and
is largely absent in schizophrenia, and is also much controls compared on the EPQ `Addiction Scale'.
reduced in high P scorers (Lubow et al., 1987, 1992; British Journal of Addiction, 80, 315±319.
Baruch et al., 1988). Latent inhibition is increased Cadoret, R., O'Gorman, T., Troughton, E. and
by dopamine antagonists, e.g. haloperidol, and Heywood, E. (1985). Alcoholism and antisocial
decreased by dopamine agonists, e.g. amphetamine personality: Interrelationships, genetic and environ-
mental factors. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42,
and nicotine (Joseph et al., 1993; Warburton et al.,
161±167.
1994). Given these facts, Joseph et al. (1996) argue Cardon, L. (1995). Genetic in¯uences on body mass
that the eect of disrupting latent inhibition is to index in early childhood. In: Behavior Genetic
make familiar stimuli salient, or perhaps curiosity Approaches in Behavioral Medicine, Turner, A.,
arousing. This, they suggest, may serve to make a Cardon, L. and Hewitt, J. (Eds), Plenum Press, New
boring life more interesting; salient (apparently York, p. 133±144.
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997)
S86 H. J. EYSENCK
Costa, P. and McCrae, P. (1991). NEO-PI Manual. of a prison population. Personality and Individual
Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessam, FL. Dierences, 4, 207±209.
Damsma, G., Pfaus, J., Wenkstern, D., Phillips, A. Gray, J., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J., Hemsley, D. and Smith,
and Fibiger, H. (1992). Sexual behaviour increases A. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens of Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 14, 1±84.
male rats: comparison in the novelty of locomotion. Gray, N., Pickering, A. and Gray, J. (1994). Psycho-
Behavioural Neuroscience, 106, 181±191. ticism and dopamine D2 binding in the basal ganglia
Di Chiara, G. and Imperato, A. (1988). Drugs abused by using single photon emission tomography. Personality
humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine and Individual Dierences, 17, 431±434.
consumption in the mesolimbic system of freely Heath, A. and Madden, P. (1995). Genetic in¯uences on
moving rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of smoking behavior. In: Behavior Genetic Approaches to
Sciences, USA, 85, 5274±5278. Behavioral Medicine, Turner, J., Cardon, L. & Hewitt,
Eaves, L. and Eysenck, H. (1980). The genetics of smok- J. (Eds), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 45±66.
ing. In: The Causes and Eects of Smoking, Eysenck, Hurlburt, G., Gade, E. and Fuqui, D. (1982). Inter-
H. J. (Ed.), pp. 140±149. correlational structure of the personality question-
Eaves, L., Eysenck, H. J. and Martin, N. (1989). Genes, naire with an alcoholic population. Psychological
Culture and Personality: An Emprical Approach. Reports, 51, 515±520.
Plenum Press, New York. Joseph, M., Peters, S. and Gray, J. (1993). Nicotine
Eysenck, H. J. (1980). The Cause and Eects of Smoking. blocks latent inhibition in rats: evidence for a critical
Maurice Temple Smith, London. role of increased functional activity of dopamine
Eysenck, H. J. (1992). The de®nition and measurement in the mesolimbic system at conditioning rather than
of psychoticism. Personality and Individual Dier- pre-exposure. Psychopharmacology, 110, 187±192.
ences, 13, 757±785. Joseph, M. H., Young, A. M. J. and Gray, J. A. (1996).
Eysenck, H. J. and Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality Are neurochemistry and reinforcement enough Ð can
and Individual Dierences. Plenum Press, New York. the abuse potential of drugs be explained by common
Eysenck, H. J. and Gudjonsson, G. (1989). The Causes actions on a dopamine reward system in the brain?
and Cures of Criminality. Plenum Press, New York. Human Psychopharmacology, 11, 555±563.
Feldman, R. and Eysenck, S. (1986). Addictive person- Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned
ality traits in bulimic patients. Personality and Attention Theory. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
Individual Dierences, 7, 923±926. bridge.
Fibiger, H. and Phillips, A. (1988). Mesocorticolimbic Lubow, R. E., Ingberg-Sachs, V., Zalstein, N. and
systems and reward. Annals of the New York Academy Gewirtz, J. (1992). Latent inhibition in low and high
of Sciences, 437, 206±216. `psychotic-prone' normal subjects. Personality and
Fowler, J., Volkow, N., Wang, G., Pappas, N., Logan, Individual Dierences, 13, 563±572.
J., MacGregor, R., Aleso, D., Shea, C., Schlyer, D., Lubow, R. E., Weiner, I., Schlossberg, A. and Baruch, I.
Wolf, A., Warner, D., Zazulkowa, I. and Cilento, R. (1987). Latent inhibition and schizophrenia. Bulletin
(1996). Inhibition of monoamine oxidase B in the of the Psychosomatic Society, 25, 464±467.
brain of smokers. Nature, 379, 733±736. Mann, L., Wise, T., Trinidad, A. and Kohanski, R.
Francis, L. (1996). The relationship between Eysenck's (1995). Alexithymia, aect recognition, and ®ve
personality factors and attitude towards substance use factors of personality in substance abuse. Perceptual
among 13±15-year-olds. Personality and Individual and Motor Skills, 80, 35±40.
Dierences, in press. Masters, R. and McGuire, M. (Eds) (1994). The Neuro-
Garrow, J., Crisp, A., Jordan, N., Meyer, J., Russell, G., transmitter Revolution. Southern Illinois University
Silverstone, I., Stunbrand, A. and van Hallie, T. Press, Cushiondale.
(1975). Pathology of eating group report. In: Dahlem McGue, M. (1995). Mediators and moderators of alco-
Konferenzes, Life Research Report, No. 2, Berlin. holism inheritance. In: Behavior Genetic Approaches in
Gilbert, D. (1995). Smoking: Individual Dierences, Behavioural Medicine, Tanner, N., Cardon, L. and
Psychopathology and Emotion. Taylor & Francis, Hewitt, J. (Eds), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 17±44.
New York. Meyer, J. (1995). Genetic studies of obesity across the life
Gossop, M. (1978). A comparative study of oral and span. In: Behavior Genetic Approaches in Behavioral
intravenous drug dependent patients on three dimen- Medicine, Turner, N., Cardon, L. and Hewitt, J.
sions of personality. International Journal of the (Eds), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 145±166.
Addictions, 13, 135±142. O'Connor, K. and Stravinski, A. (1982). Evaluation of
Gossop, M. and Eysenck, S. (1980). A further investiga- a smoking typology by use of a speci®c behavioural
tion into the personality of drug addicts in treatment. substitution method of self-control. Behaviour
British Journal of Addiction, 75, 305±311. Research and Therapy, 20, 279±288.
Gossop, M. and Eysenck, S. (1983). A comparison of Phillips, A., Atkinson, L., Blackburn, J. and Blaha,
the personality of drug addicts in treatment with that C. (1993). Increase of extracellular dopamine in the
HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
ADDICTION, PERSONALITY AND MOTIVATION S87
nucleus accumbens of the rat alienated by a condi- principles, methods and terms. Trends in Pharmaco-
tional stimulus for food: An electrodynamic study. logical Sciences, 13, 170±176.
Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 71, Teasdale, J., Seagraves, R. and Zacune, J. (1971).
387±393. Psychoticism in drug users. British Journal of Social
Raine, A. (1993). The Psychopathology of Crime. and Clinical Psychology, 10, 160±171.
Academic Press, New York. Turner, N., Cardo, L. and Hewitt, J. (1995). Behavior
Rowe, D. and Linver, M. (1995). Smoking and addictive Genetic Approaches in Behavioral Medicine. Plenum
behaviors. In: Behavior Genetic Approaches in Beha- Press, New York.
vioral Medicine, Turner, J., Cardon, L. and Hewitt, J. von Knorring, L. and Oreland, L. (1985). Personality
(Eds), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 67±83. traits and platelet monoamine oxidase in tobacco
Saulskaya, N. and Marsden, C. (1995). Conditions of smokers. Psychological Medicine, 15, 327±334.
dopamine release: dependence upon NMDA recep- Voss, T. (1992). Smoking and Common Sense. Peter
tion. Neuroscience, 67, 57±63. Owen, London.
Searles, J. (1988). The role of genetics in the patho- Warburton, D. (Ed.) (1990). Addiction Controversies.
genesis of alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- Harwood Academic Publishers, London.
ogy, 976, 153±167. Warburton, E., Joseph, M., Feldon, J., Weiner, I. and
Sher, K. (1991). Children of Alcoholism: A Critical Gray, J. (1994). Antagonism of amphetamine-induced
Appraisal of Theory and Research. University of disruption of latent inhibition in rats by haloperidol
Chicago Press, Chicago. and ondansetron: implications for a possible anti-
Silva, P. and Eysenck, S. (1987). Personality and addict- psychotic action of ondansetron. Psychopharmacol-
iveness in anorexic and bulimic patients. Personality ogy, 114, 657±664.
and Individual Dierences, 8, 749±751. Wise, R. and Rampre, P. (1985). Brain dopamine reward.
Simonton, D. (1994). Greatness: Who Makes History and American Review of Psychology, 140, 191±225.
Why. Guilford Press, New York. Young, A., Joseph, M. and Gray, J. (1992). Latent
Spelt, J. and Meyer, J. (1995). Genetics and eating inhibition of conditioned dopamine release in rat
disorders. In: Behavior Genetic Approaches in Beha- nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience, 54, 5±9.
vioral Medicine, Turner, N., Cardon, L. and Hewitt, J. Young, A., Ahier, D., Upton, R., Gray, J. and Joseph,
(Eds), Plenum Pess, New York, pp. 167±188. M. (1995). Dopamine and conditioning: micro-
Spielberger, C. and Jacobs, G. (1982). Personality and dialysis studies during sensory pre-conditioning. Brain
smoking dierences. Journal of Personality Assess- Research Association Abstracts, 12, 57.
ment, 46, 396±403. Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of Personality.
Stolerman, I. (1992). Drugs of abuse: Behavioural Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. HUMAN PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, VOL. 12, S79±S87 (1997)