100% (2) 100% found this document useful (2 votes) 1K views 396 pages Second Language Learning Theories
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content,
claim it here .
Available Formats
Download as PDF or read online on Scribd
Go to previous items Go to next items
Save Second Language Learning Theories For Later Rosamond Mitchell, |
Florence Myles and
Emma Marsden
a AN IIe). aTHIRD EDITION
SECOND
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
THEORIES
Rosamond Mitchell
Florence Myles
Emma MarsdenFirst published 1998 by Hodder Arnold
Second edition published 2004 by Hodder Education,
‘This edition published 2013 by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge isan imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 1998, 2004 Rosamond Mitchell and Florence Myles
(© 2013 Rosamond Mitchell, Florence Myles and Emma Marsden
‘The right of Rosamond Mitchell, Florence Myles and Emma Marsden,
to be identified as authors ofthis work has been asserted by them.
in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and
Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced
or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other
‘means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and
recording or in any information storage or retrieval system, without
permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks
or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and
‘explanation without intent to infringe,
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data
A catalog record for this book has been applied for
ISBN: 978-1-444-163-100 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-0-415-82583-2 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-0-203-77079-5 (ebk)
‘Typeset in Kepler Light by Phoentx Photosetting, Chatham, Kent
Printed and bound in Great Britain by the MPG Books GroupTo Paul, Francis, David, Katie and ClaireCONTENTS
ist of illustrations ix
‘Acknowledgements xi
Preface xiv
1 Second language learning: key concepts and issues 1
1.1 Jntcoductian. = 1
1.2 What makes for a good theory? 2
13. Views on the nature of language 6
1.4 The language learning process 10
1.5 Views of the language learner 19
1.6 Links with social practice 25
LZ_Conclusion 26
2 The recent history of second language learning research 27
2.1 Introduction 27
22 The 1950s and 1960s 28
23 The 1970s 31
2.4 The 1980s: a turning point 46
2.5 Continuities in the research agenda 49
2.6 Second language learning timeline 50
3 Linguistics and language learning: the Universal Grammar approach 61
1_Introduction 6)
3.2 Why a Universal Grammar? 62
Whi inive rammar consist of? 6
3.4 Universal Grammar and first language acquisition 7
3.5 Universal Grammar and second language acquisition 83
3.6 Evaluation of Universal Grammar-based approaches to second language
acquisition 94
4 Cognitive approaches to second language learning (1): general, implicit
learning mechanisms 98
4.1 Introduction 98
4.2 Input-based emergentist perspectives 99
433 Processing-based perspectives 114
4.4 Evaluation of implicit, cognitive approaches 125
5 Cognitive approaches (2): the role of memory systems and conscious
learning 130
5.1. Memory systems and their role in L2 learning 131
5.2. Explicit knowledge, information processing and skill acquisition 136
5.3 Awareness and attention in second language acquisition 146
5.4 Working Memory and second language acquisition 151
5.5 Evaluation of cognitive approaches (2): memory systems and conscious
learning 156
viiSECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES
6 Interaction in second language learning 160
6.1 Introduction 160
6.2 The revised Interaction Hypothesis (Long, 1996): an appeal to cognitive
‘theory 162
6.3: Interaction and the learning of target L2 structures and vocabulary 163
64 The role of feedback 168
6.5 The Output Hypothesis and the role of prompts in corrective feedback 175
66 The problem of ‘noticing’ 179
6.7 Learner characteristics and engagement 182
68 Evaluation 184
7 Meaning-based perspectives on second language learning 188
7.1 Introduction 188
7.2 Early functionalist studies of second language learning 189
7.3: Functionalism beyond the case study: the European Science Foundation
project 195
7.4 ‘Time talk’: developing the means to talk about past time 201
7.5 The Aspect Hypothesis and the acquisition of past tense 203
7.6 Cognitive linguistics and ‘thinking for speaking’ 206
7.7 Second language pragmatics 210
8 Evaluation 216
8 Sociocultural perspectives on second language learning 220
‘8.1 Introduction 0
8.2 Sociocultural theory 220
8.3 Applications of sociocultural theory to SLL 227
84 Evaluation 246
9 Sociolinguistic perspectives 250
‘9.1 Introduction 0
9.2. Sociolinguistically driven variability in second language use 251
9.3 Second language socialization 260
9.4 Communities of practice and situated second language learning 269
9.5 Second language learning, agency and the (re)construction of identity 276
9.6 Affect, emotion and investment in second language learning 280
9.7 Evaluation: the scope and achievements of sociolinguistic enquiry 282
10 Conclusion 285
10.1_One theory or many? 285
10.2 Main achievements of recent second language learning research 286
10.3 Future directions for second language learning research 288
10.4 Second language learning research and language education 289
Glossary 292
References 308
Subject index 365
Name index 374
viiiList of illustrations
Tables
4.1 Input profile of difficult and easy constructions 101
42 Miniature article system 12
43 Predicted and actual degree of difficulty based on isomorphism 124
5.1 Operationalizing the constructs of L2 implicit and explicit knowledge 137
5.2 Learners’ reports about noticing and learning 149
6.1 Examples of interactional modifications in NS conversations 163
6.2 Immersion teachers’ feedback moves 170
6.3 Negative feedback and modified output, by group 178
6.4 Question development by group 178
6.5 Correlations between grammatical accuracy in picture description and four
cognitive variables 183
7.1 Pragmatic and syntactic modes of expression 190
7.2 Paratactic precursors of different TL constructions 195
9.1 [t}/{d] deletion in Detroit African-American speech 254
9.2 VARBRUL results for {t]/[d] deletion by African-American speakers from
Detroit 255
9.3 [tI/{d] absence by grammatical category in Chinese-English interlanguage
and in native English dialects 256
9.4 Optional de use by all groups 260
Figures
1.1. Spolsky's general model of second language learning 4
2.1 Acquisition hierarchy for 13 English grammatical morphemes for
Spanish-speaking and Cantonese-speaking children 38
2.2 Comparison of adult and child acquisition sequences for eight grammatical
morphemes 39
3.1 Parametric variation for a functional category in English and French 76
3.2 [yemy mother’s holiday in the Caribbean islands] |v» was fantastic] 78
3.3 [ye My brother] [ypregularly wins the first prize] 78
3.4 [yp She became] [,pincredibly clever at making excuses] 79
3.5 [ypHe did this] [pp quite without reason] 79
3.6 Summary of the hierarchical structure of phrases in English shown in
Figures 3.2 to 3.5 80
3.7 Hierarchical structure in languages such as Japanese 80
3.8 Hierarchical structure in languages such as Malagasy 80
4.1 Simple recurrent network 107
42 Learners gradually ‘move up' the structure of a sentence 17
6.1 Mean accuracy of three groups on test of possessive determiners (oral
picture description task) 172
6.2 Performance of ‘low’ and ‘high’ students on grammaticality judgement task
(written error correction task) 173
6.3 Procedure for assessing ‘noticing’ 183SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES
7.1 Expected direction of spread of preterit and imperfect forms in L2 Spanish
across lexical classes
7.2. Diagrammatic representation of modal verb ‘will’
7.3 Distribution of request types by learner group
8.1 Model of an activity system
8.2 Regulatory scale for error feedback — Implicit (strategic) to Explicit
8.3 Microgenesis in the language system
8.4 Teacher's mediating moves
85 Dynamic assessment in action
8&6 Methods of assistance occurring during classroom peer interaction
87 Didactic model for mood selection in Spanish
9.1 A typology of variation in interlanguage
9.2 Frequency of de use (Chinese NSs vs. CSL. learners)
204
207
213
226
237
238
240
241
242
246
252
259Acknowledgements
‘The authors would like to thank the following people for their willingness
to make material available in advance of publication: Maria Arche, Laura
Dominguez, Alison Mackey, Leah Roberts, Nicole Tracy-Ventura, and John
Williams. We are grateful to Andrew Watts for his editorial work, and to Greg
Marsden for his personal encouragement and support throughout the project.
The authors and publishers wish to thank the following for permission to use
copyright material:
Academic Press/Elsevier: figure from ‘From discourse to syntax: grammar as a
processing strategy’, by T Givén, in Syntax and Semantics 12, 1979.
Addison Wesley Longman/Pearson Education: extracts from Achieving
understanding: discourse in intercultural encounters, by K. Bremer et al., 1996.
Cambridge University Press: table from ‘Corrective feedback and learner
uptake’, R. Lyster and L. Ranta, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 19, 1996.
Table from ‘Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: a
psychometric study’, by R. Ellis, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27, 2005
Tables from ‘Identifying the impact of negative feedback and learners’ responses
on ESL question development’, by K. McDonough, Studies in Second Language
Acquisition 27, 2005. Figures from ‘One size fits all? Recasts, prompts and L2
learning’, by A. Ammar and N. Spada, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 28,
2006.
Elsevier BV: extracts from ‘Interactional routines and the socialization of
interactional style in adult learners of Japanese’, by A.S. Ohta, Journal of
Pragmatics 31, 1999.
Equinox Publishing Ltd: figure from ‘Revolutionary pedagogies: learning that
leads (to) second language development’ by E. Negueruela, in Sociocultural
theory and the teaching of second languages, edited by |.P. Lantolf and
M, Poehner, 2008.
Gunter Narr Verlag: extracts from The syntax of conversation in interlanguage
development, by C. Sato, 1990.
xiSECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES,
John Benjamins Publishing Company: extracts from Utterance structure:
developing grammars again, by W. Klein and C. Perdue, 1992, p. 76 and
pp. 152-3. Table from ‘Competing constraints on variation in the speech of
adult Chinese learners of English’ by RJ. Bayley, in Second language acquisition
and linguistic variation, edited by R. Bayley and D.R. Preston, 1996, p. 109. Tables
from ‘Variationist perspectives on second language acquisition’ by D.R. Preston,
in Second language acquisition and linguistic variation, edited by R. Bayley
and D.R. Preston, 1996, p. 4, p. 10. With kind permission by John Benjamins
Publishing Company, Amsterdam/Philadelphia. www.benjamins.com.
John Wiley: figure from ‘Rethinking the language of thought’, by S. Schneider
and M. Katz, WIREs Cognitive Science 3, 2012.
John Wiley/Blackwell Publishing: figure from ‘Negative feedback as regulation
and second language learning in the Zone of Proximal Development’, by
A. Aljaafreh and J.P. Lantolf, Modern Language Journal 78, 1994. Table from
‘Some input on the easy/difficult grammar question: an empirical study’, by
L. Collins et al., Modern Language Journal 93, 2009. Figures from 'Sociolinguistic
variation in the speech of learners of Chinese as a second language’, by X. Li,
Language Learning 60, 2010.
John Wiley/Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages Inc: table
from ‘The impact of interaction on comprehension’, by T. Pica, R. Young and
C. Doughty, TESOL Quarterly 21, 1987.
Lawrence Erlbaum: extracts from Second language acquisition processes in the
classroom by A. S. Ohta, 2001.
Oxford University Press: figures from Language two by H. Dulay, M. Burt and
S. Krashen, reproduced by permission of Oxford University Press. Copyright ©
1982 Oxford University Press, Inc. Figure from The study of second language
acquisition, by R. Ellis, reproduced by permission of Oxford University Press.
Copyright © Rod Ellis 2008. Figure from Conditions for second language
learning, by B. Spolsky, reproduced by permission of Oxford University Press.
Copyright © B. Spolsky 1989. Table from ‘A note on canonical word order’ by
W. O'Grady, Applied Linguistics 26, 2005. Table from ‘Feedback, noticing and
instructed second language learning’ by A. Mackey, Applied Linguistics 27, 2006.
Figure from ‘How effective are recasts? The role of attention, memory, andAcknowledgements
analytical ability’ by P. Trofimovich et al, in Conversational interaction in second
language acquisition, edited by A. Mackey, 2007. Extracts from ‘Interaction-
driven L2 learning: characterizing linguistic development’, in Conversational
interaction in second language acquisition, edited by A. Mackey, 2007. Extracts
from ‘Proficiency and sequential organisation of L2 requests’ by S. Al-Gahtani
and C. Roever, Applied Linguistics 33, 2012.
Routledge/Taylor and Francis: extract from Dialogue and the development of
children’s thinking: a sociocultural approach, by N. Mercer and K. Littleton, 2007,
p.80. Figure from The articulate mammal, 5" edition, by J. Aitchison, 2008, p. 80.
Figure from ‘Cognitive linguistics and second language instruction’, by K Tyler. In
The handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition, edited by
P. Robinson and N.C. Ellis, 2008, p. 473.
Sage Publications: figures from ‘Dynamic assessment in the classroom:
Vygotskian praxis for second language development’, by J.P. Lantolf and
M. Poehner, Language Teaching Research 15, 2010.
University of Hawaii Press: extracts from ‘Artifacts and cultures of use in
intercultural communication’ by S. Thorne, Language Learning and Technology
7, 2003. Extracts from ‘Second language socialization in a bilingual chat
room: global and local considerations’ by W.S.E. Lam, Language Learning and
Technology 8, 2004.
Walter de Gruyter and Company: Figure from ‘Pragmatic development in the
Spanish as a FL classroom’, by J. C. Félix-Brasdefer, Intercultural Pragmatics 4,
2007.Preface
Aims of this book
This book is the result of collaboration between researchers interested in
second language acquisition from a range of perspectives: linguistic (Myles),
cognitive (Marsden) and social/educational (Mitchell). As in previous editions,
our general aim is to provide an up-to-date, introductory overview of the
current state of second language learning studies. Our intended audience is
wide: undergraduates following first degrees in language/linguistics, graduate
students embarking on courses in foreign language education/EFL/applied
linguistics, and a broader audience of teachers and other professionals
concerned with L2 education and development. Second language learning is
a field of research with potential to make its own distinctive contribution to
fundamental understandings, e.g. of the workings of the human mind or the
nature of language. It also has the potential to inform the improvement of social
practice in a range of fields, most obviously in language education. We ourselves
are interested in second language learning (SLL) from both perspectives, and are
concerned to make it intelligible to the widest possible audience.
All commentators recognise that while the field of second language learning
research has been extremely active and productive in recent decades, we have
not yet arrived at a unified or comprehensive view of how second languages
are learned. We have therefore organised this book as a presentation and critical
review of a number of different theories of SLL, which can broadly be viewed
as linguistic, psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic. Indeed, the overall ‘map’ of
the field we proposed in the first edition largely survives today, reflecting the
fact that key strands of research already active 20 years ago have continued to
flourish and develop. No single theoretical position has achieved dominance,
and new theoretical orientations continue to appear. Whether this is a desirable
state of affairs or not has been an issue of some controversy for SLL researchers
(see discussion in Chapter 1). On the whole, while we accept fully the arguments
for the need for cumulative programmes of research within the framework
of a particular theory, we incline towards a pluralist view of SLL theorizing.
In any case, it is obvious that students entering the field today need a broad
introduction to a range of theoretical positions, with the tools to evaluate their
goals, strengths and limitations, and this is what we aim to offer.
xivPreface
Previous editions (1998, 2004) were strongly influenced by the 1987 volume
by McLaughlin, Theories of second language learning, which provided a selective
and authoritative introduction to key L2 learning theories of the day. In this
third edition, our primary aim remains the same: to introduce the reader to
those theoretical orientations on language learning which seem currently most
productive and interesting for our intended audience. But we have revised our
text throughout to reflect the substantial developments that have taken place
in the field in the last few years, so that the work aims to be fully up-to-date for
a twenty-first-century readership. In particular, the new edition takes account
of recent substantial developments in cognitive approaches to second language
learning, and our treatment of this area has been split into two new chapters,
with Emma Marsden as lead author for this domain. The rise of Minimalism in
generative (Chomskyan) linguistics and its impact on generativist approaches to
SLL is given thorough treatment; finally the strength of the ongoing ‘social turn’
in second language learning research has been acknowledged, with substantial
revisions of later chapters dealing with functional, sociocultural and sociolinguistic
perspectives. Throughout the book, key theoretical and methodological advances
are presented and explained, greater attention has been paid to research on
internet-based language learning, and new studies (in a range of languages) have
been incorporated as examples. The evaluation sections in each chapter have been
expanded and generally the book is rebalanced in favour of newer material.
As one sign of the vigour and dynamism of second language learning research,
a good number of surveys and reviews are now on the market. Reflecting the
variety of the field, these books vary in their focus and aims. Some are written to
argue the case for a single theoretical position (e.g. Atkinson, 2011; Ayoun, 2003;
Cook and Newson, 2007; Hawkins, 2001; Herschensohn, 2000; Lardiere, 2007;
Leung, 2009; Mackey, 2007; Paradis, 2009; Thomas, 2004; White, 2003); some are
encyclopaedic in scope and ambition (e.g. R. Ellis, 2008; Gass and Mackey, 2012;
Herschensohn and Young-Scholten, 2013; Ritchie and Bhatia, 2009); some pay
detailed attention to research methods and data analysis (e.g. Larsen-Freeman
and Long, 1991; Mackey and Gass, 2012).
This particular book is intended as a unified introduction to the field, for
students without a substantial background in linguistics. We begin with an
introduction to key concepts (Chapter 1), and a historical account of how the
xvSECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES
SLL field has developed (Chapter 2). In later chapters (3-9) we have made a
selection from across the range of second language learning studies of a range
of theoretical positions, which we believe are most active and significant. To
represent linguistic theorising on SLL, we have concentrated on the Universal
Grammar approach (Chapter 3). In Chapters 4 and 5 we deal with a selection of
cognitive theories: in Chapter 4, we examine the application to SLL of general
and implicit learning mechanisms, concentrating on emergentist and processing
perspectives, while in Chapter 5, we explore the place of memory, explicit
knowledge and attention in SLL, and their contribution to skill acquisition in
particular. Chapter 6 explores the concept of interaction and its contribution
to SLL, tracing earlier and later versions of the Interaction Hypothesis and
related theories. Chapter 7 examines a range of theoretical positions which
assume the centrality of meaning-making for SLL (functionalism, ‘cognitive
linguistics’, L2 pragmatics). Chapter 8 deals with sociocultural theory and some
of its more recent extensions (activity theory, dynamic assessment, concept-
based instruction). In Chapter 9, we turn to the emergence of socially patterned
variation in L2, and examine L2 socialization theory, as well as theories of
identity, agency and investment as applied to SLL. Each of these theoretical
positions is explained, and then illustrated by discussion of a small number of
key empirical studies that have been inspired by that approach. We use these
studies to illustrate: the methodologies that are characteristic of the different
traditions in SLL research (from controlled laboratory-based studies of people
learning artificial languages, to naturalistic observation of informal learning in
the community); the scope and nature of the language ‘facts’ that are felt to be
important; and the kinds of generalizations that are drawn. Where appropriate,
we refer our readers to parallels in first language acquisition research, and also to
more comprehensive treatments of the research evidence relevant to different
theoretical positions. Each chapter concludes with an evaluation section (see
below).
Other new features of this third edition which are intended to help the new
reader build up an overall picture of the field include a timeline of important
milestones in the development of SLL research, and a glossary explaining key
terms used in the bookPreface
Comparing SLL perspectives
We want to encourage our readers to compare and contrast the various
theoretical perspectives we discuss in the book, so that they can get a better
sense of the kinds of issues which different theories are trying to explain, and
the extent to which they are supported to date with empirical evidence.
In reviewing our chosen perspectives, therefore, we evaluate each systematically,
considering the nature and extent of empirical support and paying attention to
the following factors:
© the claims and scope of the theory;
© the view of language involved in the theory;
© the view of the language learning process; and
@ the view of the learner.
In Chapter 1 we discuss each of these factors briefly, introducing key
terminology and critical issues that have proved important in distinguishing one
theory from another.4 Second language learning: key
concepts and issues
1.1 Introduction
‘This preparatory chapter provides an overview of key concepts and issues which
will recur throughout the book. We offer introductory definitions of a range of
terms, and try to equip the reader with the means to compare the goals and
claims of particular theories with one another. We also summarize key issues,
and indicate where they will be explored in more detail later in the book.
‘The main themes to be dealt with in the following sections are:
1.2. What makes for a good’ explanation or theory?
1.3 Views on the nature of language
1.4 Views of the language learning process
1.5 Views of the language learner
1.6 Links between language learning theory and social practice.
First, however, we must offer a preliminary definition of our most basic concept,
‘second language learning’. We define this broadly, to include the learning of any
language, to any level, provided only that the learning of the ‘second’ language
takes place sometime later than the acquisition of the first language.
Simultaneous infant bilingualism is a specialist topic, with its own literature,
which we do not try to address in this book. For overviews see Dépke (2000),
relevant sections in Bhatia and Ritchie (2004) and Miiller (2009). However, we
do take some account of growing research interest in interactions and mutual
influences between ‘first’ languages and later-acquired ‘second’ languages,
surveyed for example in Cook (2003) and Pavlenko (2011); aspects of this work
are discussed in some later chapters.
For us, therefore, ‘second languages’ are any languages learned later than
in earliest childhood. They may indeed be the second language the learner
is working with, in a literal sense, or they may be his/her third, fourth, fifth
language ... They encompass both languages of wider communication
encountered within the local region or community (for example, in educational
institutions, at the work place, or in the media), and truly foreign languages,
which have no substantial local uses or numbers of speakers. We include ‘foreign’
languages under our more general term of ‘second’ languages because we believe
that the underlying learning processes are essentially the same for more local
and for more remote target languages, despite differing learning purposes and
circumstances. (And, of course, such languages today are likely to be increasingly
accessible via the internet, a means of communication which self-evidently cuts
across any simple ‘local’/‘foreign’ distinction.)SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES,
‘We are also interested in all kinds of learning, whether formal, planned and
systematic (as in classroom-based learning), or informal and unstructured
(as when a new language is ‘picked up’ in the community or via the internet).
Following the proposals of Stephen Krashen (1981), some second language
researchers have made a principled terminological distinction between formal,
conscious learning and informal, unconscious acquisition. Krashen's distinctive
‘Acquisition-Learning’ Hypothesis is discussed further in Chapter 2; however,
most researchers in the field do not sustain any principled distinction between
the two terms. Unless specially indicated therefore, we ourselves will be using
both terms interchangeably. (And in Chapters 4 and 5, where the distinction
between conscious and unconscious learning is central, we will use the terms
‘implicit’ and ‘explicit’ learning to distinguish the two.)
1.2 What makes for a good theory?
Second language learning is an immensely complex phenomenon. Millions of
human beings have experience of second language learning, and may have a
good practical understanding of the activities which helped them to learn. But
this practical experience, and the common-sense knowledge which it leads to,
are clearly not enough to help us understand the process fully. We know, for a
start, that people cannot reliably describe the language system which they have
somehow internalized, nor the inner mechanisms which process, store and
retrieve many aspects of that new language.
We need to understand second language learning better than we do, for two
basic reasons:
(a) Improved knowledge in this particular domain is interesting in itself, and
can also contribute to more general understanding about the nature of
language, of human learning, and of intercultural communication, and thus
about the human mind itself, as well as how all these are interrelated and
affect each other.
(b) ‘The knowledge will be useful; if we become better at explaining the learning
process, and are better able to account for both success and failure in L2
learning, there will be a payoff for millions of teachers, and tens of millions
of students and other learners, who are struggling with the task.
We can only pursue a better understanding of L2 learning in an organized
and productive way if our efforts are guided by some form of theory. For our
purposes, a theory is a more or less abstract set of claims about the entities
which are significant within the phenomenon under study, the relationships
which exist between them, and the processes which bring about change.
‘Thus a theory aims not just at description, but at explanation. Theories
may be embryonic and restricted in scope, or more elaborate, explicit and1.2 What makes for a good theory?
comprehensive. They may deal with different areas of interest; thus a property
theory will be primarily concerned with modelling the nature of the language
system which is to be acquired, while a transition theory will be primarily
concerned with modelling the change/developmental processes of language
acquisition (Gregg, 2003b; Jordan, 2004, Chapter 5). (A particular transition
theory for L2 learning may itself deal only with a particular stage or phase of
learning or with the learning of some particular sub-aspect of language: or it
may propose learning mechanisms which are much more general in scope.)
Worthwhile theories are collaboratively produced, and evolve through a
process of systematic enquiry, in which the claims of the theory are assessed
against some kind of evidence or data. This may take place through a process
of hypothesis-testing through formal experiment, or through more ecological
procedures, where naturally occurring data is analysed and interpreted.
(There are now many manuals offering guidance on SLL research methods in
both traditions; for example, Mackey and Gass, 2012. We will provide basic
introductions to a range of research procedures as needed, throughout the
book, and also in the glossary.) Finally, the process of theory building is a
reflexive one; new developments in the theory lead to the need to collect new
information and explore different phenomena and different patterns in the
potentially infinite world of ‘facts’ and data. Puzzling ‘facts’, and patterns which
fail to fit in with expectations, in turn lead to new, more powerful theoretical
insights.
‘To make these ideas more concrete, an early ‘model’ of second language learning
is shown in Figure 1.1, taken from Spolsky, 1989, p. 28.
‘This model represents a ‘general theory of second language learning’, as the
proposer describes it (Spolsky, 1989, p. 14). The model encapsulates this
researcher's theoretical views on the overall relationship between contextual
factors, individual learner differences, learning opportunities and learning
outcomes. It is thus an ambitious model in the breadth of phenomena it is trying
to explain. The rectangular boxes show the factors (or variables) which the
researcher believes are most significant for learning, that is, where variation can
lead to differences in success or failure. The arrows connecting the various boxes
show directions of influence. The contents of the various boxes are defined at
great length as consisting of clusters of interacting ‘Conditions’ (74 in all: 1989,
pp. 16-25), which make language learning success more or less likely. These
summarize the results of a great variety of empirical language learning research,
as Spolsky interprets them.
How would we begin to ‘evaluate’ this or any other model, or even more
modestly, to decide that this was a view of the language learning process with
which we felt comfortable and within which we wanted to work? This would
depend partly on the extent to which the author seems to have taken account
of empirical evidence available in the field, and provided a synthetic account ofSECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING THEORIES
provides
Social context
leads to
a
Attitudes:
(0f various kinds)
PPS eee ese eee
which appear in the
learner as
¥
Motivation
which joins with other personal
characteristics such as
| y ¥ ¥
, Previous
Age Personality knowledge
all of which explain the use the
learner makes of the available
Learning opportunities (formal or informal) }«————!
the interplay between learner
and situation determining
y
Linguistic and non-linguistic
‘outcomes for the learner
Figure 1.1. Spolsky’s general model of second tanguage learning (source: Spolsky, 1989, p. 28)11.2 What makes for a good theory?
it. It would also depend on rather broader philosophical positions: for example
are we satisfied with an account of human learning which sees individual
differences as both relatively fixed, and also highly influential for learning?
Finally, it would also depend on the particular focus of our own interests
within second language learning; this particular model seems well adapted for
the study of the individual learner, for example, but has relatively little to say
about the social relationships in which they engage, the way they process new
language, nor the kinds of language system they construct.
Since the mid-1990s, there has been increasing debate about the adequacy
of the theoretical frameworks used to underpin research on second language
learning, One main line of criticism has been that SLL research and theorizing
(as exemplified by Spolsky, 1989) has historically been too preoccupied with the
cognition of the individual learner, and sociocultural dimensions of learning
have been neglected. From this perspective language is an essentially social
phenomenon, and second language learning itself is a ‘social accomplishment’,
which is ‘situated in social interaction’ (Firth and Wagner, 2007, p. 807), and
discoverable through scrutiny of second language use, using techniques such as
conversation analysis (Pekarek Doehler, 2010; Kasper and Wagner, 2011). A
second ~ though not unrelated — debate has concerned the extent to which SLL
theorizing has become too broad. Long (1993) and others argued that ‘normal
science’ advanced through competition between a limited number of theories,
and that the SLL field was weakened by theory proliferation. This received a
vigorous riposte from Lantolf (1996) among others, advancing the postmodern
view that knowledge claims are a matter of discourses, which ‘cannot be
extralinguistically described’ (Lantolf, 1996, p. 734). From this point of view,
all scientific theories are merely ‘metaphors that have achieved the status of
acceptance by a group of people we refer to as scientists (p. 721), and scientific
theory building is all about ‘taking metaphors seriously’ (p. 723). For Lantolf, any
reduction in the number of official metaphors’ debated could ‘suffocate’ those
espousing different world views.
‘These debates about the nature of knowledge, theory and explanation in second
language learning have persisted up to the present. It is probably fair to say
that the majority of SLL researchers today adopt some version of a ‘realist”
or ‘rationalist’ position (Jordan, 2004; Sealey and Carter, 2004; Long, 2007),
grounded in the philosophical view that an objective and knowable world exists
(that is, not only discourses), and that it is possible to build and test successively
more powerful explanations of how that world works, through systematic
programmes of inquiry and of problem-solving, and this is the position taken
in this book However, like others such as Jordan (2004) and Ortega (2011), we
acknowledge that a proliferation of theories is necessary and desirable to make
better sense of the varied phenomena of SLL, the agency of language learners,
and the contexts and communities of practice in which they operate. We