Thesis 2
Thesis 2
EXERCISE
Submitted by
Charles Heidrick
Department of Psychology
Fall 2015
Master’s Committee:
Brian Butki
Kathryn Rickard
Copyright by Charles Heidrick 2015
EXERCISE
Regular physical activity has been shown to have substantial physical and mental benefits,
ranging from protection against obesity to greater quality of life (Harvard School of Public Health,
2014; Faulkner & Taylor, 2005). Yet, a low percentage of people in the United States meet
recommended levels of physical activity (Troiano et al., 2008). Goal setting has been shown to be
an effective way to improve behavior (Locke & Latham, 1990; Latham & Budworth, 2006), but
may be impacted by underexplored social factors. This study examined the role that another
person, apart from the goal-setting exerciser, can have on physical activity goal pursuit. College
exercises (push-ups, planks, jumping jacks, and single-leg balancing), after which participants set
personal goals regarding their own imminent performance of these exercises. Participants were
randomly assigned to one of three conditions: 1) private goals: participants set goals and did not
share them with experimenter; 2) acknowledged goals: participants’ goals were positively
performance and goal attainment was seen for planks and a significant effect of condition on goal
attainment was seen for pushups. No significant effects were seen for jumping jacks or balancing.
Results indicate positive effects of goal acknowledgment on subsequent goal attainment and
ii
exercise performance and also suggest negative effects of having goals that could be
acknowledged go unacknowledged.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT…………………………………………………………………………………. ii
1. CHAPTER 1 - INTRODUCTION……………………………........................................... 1
1.1 - PHYSICAL ACTIVITY LEVELS……………….………………………………...... 1
1.2 - PHYSICAL INACTIVITY HEALTH RISKS……………………………………..... 1
1.3 - INCREASING PHYSICAL ACTIVITY RATES…………………………………… 2
1.4 - HEALTH BENEFITS OF PHYSICAL ACTIVITY…….…………………………... 3
1.5 - MENTAL BENEFITS…………………………….………………………………...... 3
1.6 - OBESITY…………………………………………………………………………….. 4
1.7 - CHRONIC DISEASE PREVENTION AND RECOVERY…………….................... 4
1.8 - GOAL SETTING………………………………………….…………………………. 5
1.9 - GOAL SETTING AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY……………………….….............. 6
1.10 - GOAL ATTAINMENT PRINCIPLES…………………………………………...... 7
1.11 - SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY ……………………….……………………. 8
1.12 - MANIPULATING ACKNOWLEDGMENT…………………………………….... 8
1.13 - EXERCISE GOALS AND ACKNOWLEDGMENT……………………………… 9
1.14 - THE GOAL ACKNOWLEDGMENT PARADIGM………………………………. 10
1.15 - THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR…………………………………………… 12
1.16 - HYPOTHESES…….…………………………….…………………………………. 13
2. CHAPTER 2 - METHOD……………………………….................................................... 16
2.1 - PARTICIPANTS ………….…..…………………………………………………...... 16
2.2 - PROCEDURE…………………………………..………………………………….... 16
2.3 - MEASURES………………………………..………………………………………... 19
2.4 - ANALYSIS…………………………………………….…………………................. 23
3. CHAPTER 3 - RESULTS………………………………………………………………… 25
3.1 - PARTICIPANT CHARACTERISTICS…………………………………………….. 25
3.2 - TESTS OF NORMALITY…………………………………………………………... 25
3.3 - MAJOR ANOVA ANALYSES…………………………………………………….. 27
3.4 - CORRELATION ANALYSES……………………………………………………... 30
4. CHAPTER 4 - DISCUSSION……………………………………………………………. 32
4.1 - CONCLUSION……………………………………………………………………… 42
REFERENCES………………………………….………………………………................... 44
APPENDIX A: GOALS SHEET……………………………………………………............. 51
APPENDIX B: BORG’S RATINGS OF PERCEIVED EXERTION (RPE) SCALE……… 52
APPENDIX C: SUBSET OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION INVENTORY (IMI)…………. 53
APPENDIX D: REJECTION SENSITIVITY…………….………………………………… 54
APPENDIX E: EXERCISE-SPECIFIC SELF-EFFICACY………….…………………….. 56
APPENDIX F: EXERCISE-SPECIFIC SOCIAL SUPPORT………………………............ 57
APPENDIX G: TABLES……………………………………………………………............ 58
APPENDIX H: FIGURES………………………………………………………………….. 61
iv
Chapter 1 - Introduction
There is a distressing trend occurring in the United States: a very high percentage of
people understand the implications of a sedentary lifestyle, yet a similarly high percentage of
people are engaging in just that: a sedentary lifestyle. In fact, 97% of Americans think that a lack
of physical activity is a risk factor for health (Martin, Morrow, Jackson, & Dunn, 2000; Pate et al.,
1995), and by one of the most objective measures of physical activity available, accelerometry,
less than 5% of Americans adhere to physical activity recommendations (Troiano et al., 2008).
The implications of a sedentary lifestyle are a prime public health concern. Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) analyses show that physical inactivity was associated with
nine-million cases of cardiovascular disease in 2001; this was estimated to cost the United States
around $24 billion (Wang, Pratt, Macera, Zheng, & Heath, 2004). A meta-analysis determined
that those who completed at least 150 minutes per week of moderate-intensity physical activity
lowered their heart disease risk by 14% (Sattelmair et al., 2011). People who completed at least
Physical inactivity also contributes to cancer, strokes, type 2 diabetes, depression, and
dementia, among other chronic diseases (Mokdad, Marks, Stroup, & Gerberding, 2004; Bouchard,
Blair, & Haskell, 2007; Katzmarkzyk, Gledhill, & Shephard, 2000; Physical Activity Guidelines
Advisory Committee, 2008). A prime public health objective should be to increase the rate of
1
Increasing Physical Activity Rates
If people understand the health implications of living a sedentary lifestyle, yet still remain
inactive, does that mean that people simply do not want to be physically active? Not necessarily.
Contrary to objective measures of physical activity (where less than 5% are meeting
recommendation levels), self-report measures show that nearly half (48.1%) of Americans meet
recommendation levels (National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion,
2005). This discrepancy between objective and self-report measures of physical activity is
strikingly large, and is likely due to multiple reasons. One is that Americans may simply think
they are engaging in more physical activity than they really are (Baranowski, 1988); this may
account for some of the discrepancy. But another reason is that Americans may be presenting
themselves in what they perceive as a more favorable light to other people, themselves, or both
(Warnecke et al., 1997). This latter reason would suggest that these people seem to at least care
about physical activity levels to some degree: they value it for themselves or they recognize that
most others value it. Without this value of physical activity levels there would be little reason to
be dishonest. Therefore, not only it is intuitive to think that many Americans would like to engage
in more physical activity than they currently are, but it seems as if the discrepancy between self-
reported and objectively-measured physical activity data support that assumption as well.
This study aimed to gain a further understanding of factors that could act as either barriers
or enhancers to increasing physical activity levels. Specifically, this study examined the role of
goal acknowledgment from others in physical activity goal setting. Past research provided
evidence that the role of acknowledgment in setting goals may be an important aspect related to
2
Also, psychological theories suggest that acknowledgment may have a substantial impact
on the effectiveness of accomplishing one’s health goals, yet there is a lack of research that looks
studied experimentally, causal factors impacting a person’s physical activity levels after they set
certain goals can be identified. This information has implications in multiple realms of society,
including but not limited to: a personal trainer-trainee relationship, children’s fitness in grade
school (e.g., through the Presidential Fitness Challenge, which encourages kids to make health
goals), a person making personal fitness goals for themselves, and also people making activity
goals that may not be related to physical activity or fitness, but to a wide variety of other
outcomes such as hours reading per week or time spent with family at night. Since the goals in
each of these outcomes are capable of being acknowledged by others, the implications could be
It is important to first establish the immense benefits that occur with regular physical
activity. Physical activity is defined as “any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that
results in energy expenditure” (Caspersen, Powell, & Christenson, 1985, p. 126). In relation, a
term that will be frequently used in this paper, exercise, is defined by the same authors as a
structured time to engage in physical activity with the objective to improve physical fitness.
Mental Benefits
There are numerous mental benefits associated with physical activity, including decreased
risk for depression and anxiety, improved self-concept, and a greater quality of life (Faulkner &
Taylor, 2005). Physical fitness, or “a set of attributes that are either health or skill related”
(Caspersen, Powell, & Christenson, 1985, p. 126), is intimately related to physical activity levels
3
and has been shown to be a crucial element in cognitive functioning (Kramer, Erickson, &
Colcombe, 2006). Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000) have shown that physical activity also
Obesity
Currently in the United States there is an epidemic of obesity (condition where a person
has accumulated excess body fat) and its related health concerns. According to 2011-2012 data
from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), more than one-third of
adults and nearly one-fifth of youth were obese (Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2014). Obesity
has been shown to influence the development of Heart Disease, Type 2 Diabetes, and many
different types of cancer – which are but a few of obesity’s possible negative health consequences
According the Harvard School of Public Health, physical activity protects against obesity
in multiple ways, such as through increased energy expenditure and decreased body fat (2014).
Also, muscle strengthening activities (such as weight-lifting) increase muscle mass; muscle
strengthening activities therefore result in an increase in calories burned throughout the day (from
rebuilding and increasing of muscle tissue), even while at rest (Harvard School of Public Health,
2014).
Overall, the case for regular physical activity is strong. In 1994, The Surgeon General’s
Report deemed a lack of physical activity as one of the leading causes of all deaths. For the next
decade and a half, it is estimated that physical inactivity in the United States contributed to
roughly 200,000 deaths per year (Danaei et al., 2009). A study examining the relationship between
physical inactivity and chronic disease found them to be “heavily correlated” and that physical
4
activity is now considered a “principal intervention for primary and secondary prevention of
Currently, overall death rates from cancer have been decreasing, but a report from the
journal Cancer still concluded that “excess weight and a lack of sufficient physical activity
contribute to the increased incidence of many cancers, adversely affect quality of life for cancer
survivors, and may worsen prognosis for several cancers” (Eheman et al., 2012, p. 2338). Physical
activity also seems to have an intimate relationship with stroke recovery. The American Heart
Association found that functional ability after a stroke was significantly predicted by self-reported
physical activity levels prior to the stroke (McDonnell et al., 2015). Finally, type 2 diabetes, a
condition that is increasing in prevalence and is associated with a much shorter life expectancy, is
more likely to develop in people who do not meet recommended levels of physical activity
Goal Setting
A technique to combat the public health concern of a lack of physical activity may be the
setting of health goals. Simply setting health goals has been shown to improve health in relation to
not setting any health goals at all (Locke & Latham, 1990). Beyond simply knowing whether or
not to set any health goals, research also suggests it is important to know what type of goals to set
and how to set them, and that this can have a substantial effect on behavior. What to do after
Although the theory of setting goals has received an abundance of research, it is ever-
changing and cannot seem to find a permanent identity in the realm of physical activity. The most
commonly known theory among goal research is named ‘S.M.A.R.T.,’ an acronym which calls for
goals to be Specific, Measurable, Achievable, Relevant, and Time-targeted. But the principles of
5
goal setting also go beyond S.M.A.R.T. goals. Improved performance has been linked to goals
that are not only easy, as might be implied by the ‘attainable’ criterion, but for difficult goals as
The mechanics of how setting goals improves performance and increases the rate of
acquisition of desired outcomes has also been researched. According to Latham and Budworth
(2006), in the workplace goals narrow attention, lead to higher levels of effort, cause more
persistence, and lead to improved cognition in relation to the goal and the associated behaviors
needed to accomplish it. In general, goals that are specific and high in difficulty increase
performance by orienting an individual’s attention, activating knowledge and skills related to the
goal, and increasing persistence on goal-related tasks (Locke & Latham, 2013).
These studies argue and show that certain characteristics of setting goals leads to improved
outcomes. But, goal setting strategies are not the only factors that go into goal accomplishment
rates (how often one achieves a goal they set). There is less research on another aspect of goal
accomplishment: goal attainment principles. These principles concern the actions one takes after
setting a goal instead of the mechanics of how one sets a goal. Psychological theories make
predictions for what people should do after setting a goal, but in order to have a better
understanding of those predictions in the realm of physical activity, more research needs to be
done that looks directly at the role of goal attainment principles after setting physical activity
knowledge that goal setting in physical activity has shown mixed results. A literature review
looking at goal setting as a strategy for physical activity behavior change found this strategy to be
6
inconclusive (Shilts, Horowitz, & Townsend, 2004). Although some studies found support for
goal setting in increasing physical activity levels, overall only 32% of the studies fully supported
goal setting as a strategy for physical activity behavior change. Considering that goal setting has
been shown to be a good strategy in behavior change in other areas, it may be that there are unique
A more recent review though of physical activity interventions found that self-regulatory
techniques, which in many studies included goal setting, consistently increase the effectiveness of
interventions to boost physical activity levels (Greaves et al., 2011). So although the effectiveness
of goal setting in physical activity has room for improvement, it seems as if programs that
There has been some research on positive goal attainment principles. An example of this is
having proper feedback from an outside source (not yourself) during the process of trying to
achieve your goal. Todd and O’Connor (2005) argued that providing feedback in the short term
not only increases motivation but also the commitment level to achieve the goal. The researchers
also found that feedback was more effective, resulting in higher percentages of goal
accomplishment, when created in a context with positive language (from the person giving the
feedback).
This research on feedback involves social factors for the accomplishment of a goal, and
gives clues to the techniques for improved goal accomplishment. There is also some correlational
research related to the social factors of health goals. Murcia, San Roman, Galindo, Alonso, and
Gonzalez-Cutre (2008) found that when non-competitive exercisers were surrounded by peers
7
Self-Determination Theory
Murcia et al. (2008) argued that one of the reasons this “peer motivational climate” of
social support predicted both motivation and enjoyment of exercise was the perceived improved
relatedness to their peers of those exercising. They described this process through self-
determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000), which explains three psychological needs that must be
fulfilled for optimal motivation towards an activity. The first is competence towards the activity,
where the person feels as if they possess the aptitude to excel. The next is autonomy, where the
person feels as if they control their own actions concerning the activity. Lastly, there is a need for
a feeling of relatedness; this is the need to feel connected to others. Murcia et al. (2008) claimed
that part of the reason for the motivation climate to increase motivation and enjoyment was
through this concept of relatedness, where the exerciser felt more connected to those around them,
and as a result experienced increased motivation and enjoyment towards the exercise activity, as
Through Murcia et al.’s (2008) research and experiments done in behavioral economics
(detailed below), there are reasons to hypothesize that positive acknowledgment of exercise goals,
rather than the absence of acknowledgment, could lead to better exercise performance. This body
of research also suggests that positive acknowledgment would lead to better accomplishment of
Manipulating Acknowledgment
Behavioral economics has shown how perceived meaning of a task, manipulated through
differing levels of acknowledgment from a peer, can change the effort put forth towards the
accomplishment of a task. Ariely, Kamenica, and Prelec (2008) observed this through a simple
worksheet completion paradigm. They brought participants into a lab and had them fill out a
8
worksheet for a certain amount of money. The participants could keep filling out more worksheets
for more money, but every subsequent worksheet earned them a little less money. The researchers
found differences in how many worksheets the participants filled out through three different
conditions. Their first condition involved the researchers taking every worksheet when the
participant was done with them and looking over them slowly, nodding their head as they did so.
This resulted in participants filling out the highest amount of worksheets. The second condition
involved the experimenter taking every worksheet when the participant was done with each one
and immediately placing it, face-down, on a table next to them without looking over it. This
resulted in participants filling out a significantly smaller amount of worksheets than in the first
condition. The last condition involved the researchers shredding each worksheet immediately after
the participant completed each one, and this result was not significantly different to the second
condition, but still resulted in significantly fewer worksheets being filled out than the first
The researchers argued that the perceived meaning of the task at hand was altered by the
level of acknowledgment the researchers showed towards the worksheets. When they
acknowledged the participants’ work by looking over it slowly, the participants perceived the task
Knowing that positive feedback can promote goal accomplishment (Todd & O’Connor,
2005), that enjoyment and motivation for physical activity is related to positive social support
when exercising (Murcia et al., 2008), and that acknowledgment can play a role in the perceived
meaning of a task (Ariely et al., 2008), there is reason to believe these concepts might interact.
Together, they may point to the conclusion that an exercise goal could be perceived as meaning
9
less with less (or more negative) acknowledgment from a peer. Further, these lines of work
together suggest that the lack of positive feedback could lead to decreased effort being put forth
from someone who partakes in physical activity, especially when they expect some sort of
feedback in a social setting. According to the self-determination theory, this “negative” or lack of
feedback could decrease the perceived levels of relatedness between a goal setter and the person
in the position to provide feedback, and therefore decrease the motivation of the one receiving
The present study adapted Ariely et al.’s (2008) basic paradigm to an exercise-goal
situation to observe the phenomenon seen in their study – that is, acknowledgment of something
from an outside source improving behavior in some way, and the lack of acknowledgment
worsening behavior. This paradigm had participants make goals that are relevant to a physical
activity session that directly followed the goal setting. In one condition, the participants kept their
goals to themselves and were told not to expect any feedback on their goals. In another condition,
the participants gave their goals to an experimenter who provided positive acknowledgment of
those goals. Lastly, a third condition had participants once again give their goals to an
experimenter, but this time the experimenter ignored the goals and did not provide any feedback.
It was hypothesized that positive acknowledgment of an exercise goal would lead to improved
exercise outcomes over the absence of feedback, especially when feedback is possible. It was also
hypothesized that when feedback is not possible, the absence of acknowledgment would have no
effect on outcomes. There are two primary reasons that a condition with no possible feedback is
being included: 1) as a control to see if positive acknowledgment improves outcomes and the
10
feedback is possible, and 2) to expand the implications of the results to more individuals in
situations outside of this study, where it is recognized that sometimes people in society keep their
goals to themselves.
There are some distinct differences between Ariely et al.’s (2008) experiment and the one
conducted here – in Ariely et al.’s (2008) experiment, persistence on the same task was assessed
of the goal setting activity happens only once, and persistence is measured in a separate activity –
the exercise itself. Therefore, for the hypothesis of this experiment to be correct, the goal setting
and subsequent physical activity must be intimately related. There are strong reasons to believe
First, since the goal setting activity explicitly involves and describes the physical activity
session, the physical activity session itself is analogous to the worksheet completion task. In other
words, the worksheet completion task was a means unto itself, and the participants were aware of
that. In this experiment though, participants are aware that the goals are a means unto something
There is also another way to parallel the paradigm for this experiment to the worksheet
completion task. The acknowledgment in Ariely et al.’s (2008) paradigm changed the perceived
meaning of the worksheet that the participants were completing; in this experiment, the perceived
meaning of the participants’ goals may be what is changing. Fortunately, this is exactly what is of
interest in this experiment. If the independent variable of interest did not involve goals, the
connection between the two studies (the worksheet completion task and the proposed study here)
would not be as meaningful. But, since goal attainment principles are of interest in this study, and
social influences have been shown to relate to exercise quality (Murcia et al., 2008), this
11
connection of goal feedback and exercise quality is what the present study is testing. It is argued
that goal feedback is a form of social influence, and therefore the act of providing goal feedback
should be treated as a social situation. In the sense that feedback is a form of social influence, we
could justify why the perceived meaning of the task in Ariely et al.’s (2008) paradigm changed.
Ariely et al. (2008) argues that differing levels of acknowledgment come from recognition of
one’s work (or lack thereof), which results in differing levels of purpose felt in the activity - but
their explanation stops there. This study sought to further explain what psychological phenomena
may have been occurring in Ariely and colleagues’ study, while also seeing the implications of a
similar paradigm in a different field. Self-determination theory, along with the theory of planned
behavior (detailed below), form the basis for the argument that peer-acknowledgment is indeed a
social influence that can cause differing, tangible outcomes on multiple behaviors besides simply
a worksheet-completion task.
There are other reasons, besides those provided by self-determination theory, to believe
differing acknowledgment of physical activity goals may lead to differing exercise behaviors. A
theory often used in health research is the theory of planned behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1985). The
TPB is a theory that predicts behavior by a person’s attitudes, subjective norms (measured by
asking respondents to rate the extent to which ‘important others’ would approve or disapprove of
their performing a given behavior), and perceived control related to that behavior (Ajzen, 1991).
Linking those predictors to this experiment may provide evidence that the independent variable of
The TPB has been shown to be a reliable predictor of physical activity behavior. A meta-
analysis was conducted that examined the relationship between predictors from the TPB and
12
subsequent physical activity behavior. Seventy-two studies analyzed together revealed that the
major predictors in the TPB (attitudes, subjective norms, perceived control) were supported in
Are any of the predictors in the TPB linked to the independent variable in this experiment?
Returning to the definition of subjective norms, these are measured by the extent that ‘important
others’ would feel about their behavior and whether they would approve or disapprove in them
(Ajzen, 1991). ‘Important others’ may be linked to a person acknowledging goals. In the lab, the
experimenter holds a position of authority, and in terms of the protocol of the experiment and
giving directions for what behaviors to perform, he/she would be seen as a person of importance.
In the real world, there are situations that would also parallel the one in the lab. In terms of a
personal trainer and their trainee, the trainee has put him or herself in position to listen to the
trainer because they believe the trainer is a person of importance, at least in terms of physical
activity. If a person is making personal fitness goals by themselves without a personal trainer, it is
intuitive to think that if they would show those goals to others, it would be someone who they
highly value or believe possesses valuable information regarding physical activity. In either case,
the person being shown the goals would be an important person in the context of that
‘acknowledgment’ situation.
Hypotheses
The expected variations between groups in the following hypotheses are due to the
expected positive effects on physical activity (perceived effort, intrinsic motivation, and
performance on varying tasks) of positive acknowledgment of related goals and negative effects
was expected that these effects (both positive and negative) would be absent when there is no
13
possible acknowledgment (when participant keeps their goals private). Therefore, the hypotheses
Hypothesis 1
Average exertion will vary by group. Participants who receive positive acknowledgment of
short-term exercise goals from a peer prior to an exercise session will put more effort into that
possible. The absence of possible acknowledgment will result in levels of exertion between those
Hypothesis 2
Intrinsic motivation will vary by group. Participants who receive positive peer
acknowledgment of short-term exercise goals prior to an exercise session will report more
intrinsic motivation for that exercise session than participants who receive no acknowledgment
when acknowledgment is possible. The absence of possible acknowledgment will result in levels
Hypothesis 3
Performance on the exercise session, measured by the difference between one’s goals and
their repetitions of pushups and jumping jacks as well as duration of planks and foot balances, will
vary by group. Participants who receive positive peer acknowledgment of short-term exercise
goals prior to an exercise session will have better performance in that exercise session than
possible acknowledgment will result in levels of performance between those demonstrated by the
14
participants receiving positive acknowledgment and those expecting acknowledgment, but
receiving none.
15
Chapter 2 - Method
Participants
Psychology, and PSY 250, Research Methods in Psychology, were recruited through the
PSY250 are required to participate in research as a part of their course grade; they receive
compensation for the time with course credit. Participants must have been in good enough
physical condition to exercise for at least fifteen minutes at an intensity level of their choosing.
This information was communicated to participants before they signed up for the experiment, and
they each signed a consent form before participation detailing that they are in good enough
physical condition to partake in the activity. It would have been ideal to recruit and run at least
144 participants total (~48 participants per condition) since a power analysis (GPower3.1.7;
Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) suggested that ~48 participants in each of the three conditions
would be sufficient to detect large-size effects with a high degree of confidence – 143 participants
Procedure
Participants met the experimenter in a lab space at Colorado State University. The
experimenter began by explaining the experiment to the participant. In this explanation was that
the participant would be working out by conducting four different exercises, that the participant
would be writing down and thinking about specific personal goals for their workout, and also that
the participant would give their goals to the experimenter OR keep their goals private (this
depended on condition) before beginning the workout. It was explained to the participant that the
experimenter would be watching them go through their workout and would give them a report
16
afterwards on how hard they thought the participant worked and the quality of their workout. The
experimenter did not actually give the participant a report on their workout, and this was
explained to the participant in the debriefing. The purpose of this cover story was to create a more
realistic situation, such as a personal trainer and their trainee, where the personal trainer
(experimenter) is actively watching and evaluating the exercise session of the trainee (participant).
The experimenter then gained informed consent from the participant. The experimenter then gave
more detailed instructions regarding the exercise session in the experiment. These instructions
included what specific four exercises the participant would be performing, which included planks
(variations shown to accommodate different levels of physical fitness included planks held on
forearms and toes, planks held on forearms and knees, and planks held on hands and toes such as
the start of a pushup), pushups (variations shown included pushups on hands and toes, pushups on
hands and knees, and pushups on hands and knees using a stable table as an incline), jumping
jacks, and a one-foot balancing challenge. These four exercises were included to attempt to
accommodate a wide array of fitness interests from participants and to have exercises that may
generate large amounts of variation in performance from participants (if only a couple exercises
were included and there was not much variation in performance, possible effects from the goal
Also included in this explanation to the participants was that they were not to push
themselves to their limits in performing these exercises (so as to avoid injury), but they were to try
hard in order to receive an accurate gauge of their current fitness level. This explanation, along
with the participants getting however much time they wished to warm up before conducting the
four exercises, provided a blend of a safe experience for the participant while also getting accurate
fitness and effort evaluations. Lastly, it was explained to the participant that they should write
17
down specific goals for each of the four exercises: repetitions of pushups they can accomplish,
length in seconds they can hold a plank, the number of jumping jacks they can do in one minute,
and how long they can balance on one leg with their eyes closed.
The experimenter then gave the participant a sheet of paper (Appendix A) and a pencil to
write down their goals and to spend a moment thinking about those goals. When the participant
was done, what the experimenter did with the goals depended on the participant’s condition. In
condition 1, the participant simply kept their goals for themselves. They had previously been told
they would do this, so once they were done writing down and thinking about their goals, they
simply moved on to the next part of the experiment. In condition 2, the experimenter took the
goals, read the goals slowly, gave an approving “uh-huh” sound, and said “these look great!”
before setting the goals face-up on a desk next to them. In condition 3, the experimenter took the
goals and immediately set them on a desk next to them, face-down, without reading the goals. The
participants knew if they were going to give their goals to the experimenter or not, but they were
not told (in conditions 2 and 3) what they experimenter would do with their goals. This
uncertainty reflected real-world social interactions in which people often do not know how others
Directions for Borg’s Perceived Exertion Scale (Borg, 1998) were then explained to the
participant. This scale was then administered directly after each of the four exercises. All
participant exertion scores were then averaged to create an overall average perceived exertion
score for their workout. The reason for doing multiple measures was to prevent bias that may be
18
The participant then conducted each of the four exercises, with every participant going
through the same order. The experimenter recorded repetitions accomplished for pushups and
jumping jacks as well as time accomplished in holding a plank and balancing on one foot.
The participant was then offered water and a quick break to rest before completing the
remainder of the study. The participant was then given a survey packet that addressed
demographic variables, how they felt after giving their goals to the experimenter (if they were in
their exercise-specific social support. After completion of the packet, participants were measured
for height and weight using a calibrated scale and stadiometer and then debriefed about the details
and purpose of the study. Participants who were in condition 1 were also politely asked if they
would be willing to now give their goals to the experimenter (all obliged). Finally, they were
thanked for their participation. Participation lasted approximately 40 minutes. The experimenter
then recorded the variation of pushups and planks the participant engaged in (they did not have to
do this for jumping jacks and balancing on one foot since there were no variations of these
exercises) and the goals the participants had made for themselves. Also on this sheet were space
for the experimenter to write down their initials and the date of the experiment; the participant
number and the randomly assigned condition was also listed. Lastly, the experimenter gathered
and organized the materials from the experiment and made the room ready for the next participant.
Measures
Goal acknowledgment is the independent variable in this experiment. This variable has
three levels: 1) acknowledgment not possible, 2) acknowledgment possible and received, and 3)
acknowledgment possible but not received. There are six dependent variables in this experiment
along with three potential covariates. The dependent variables are the participant’s average
19
exertion (Borg, 1998), their intrinsic motivation (Ryan, 1982), and the goal-to-result difference for
repetitions of pushups, seconds held in plank position, repetitions of jumping jacks in one minute,
and seconds balancing on one foot with eyes closed. The potential covariates included the
participant’s rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996), their exercise-specific self-efficacy
(Sallis, Pinski, Grossman, Patterson, & Nader, 1988), and their exercise-specific social support
Four of the dependent variables were exercise performance: this was measured by the
goal-to-result difference for pushups, planks, jumping jacks, and balancing on one leg. For
example, if a participant makes a goal to accomplish 20 pushups but only accomplishes 15, a goal-
to-result score of -5 will be recorded in this measure (calculated from goal minus result). Fitness
level is expected to be similar across conditions due to random assignment, but this goal-to-result
measure is to protect against the possibility that similarity between conditions does not occur. If
participants in one level of the independent variable outperform their goals while participants in a
different level underperform, this would also be of interest relative to this study’s hypotheses,
even though their actual performance numbers could be identical. This will help protect against
the fact that goal averages across conditions will likely not be the same.
Participants’ perceived effort during the physical activity session is the next dependent
variable, and was measured with Borg’s Perceived Exertion Scale (Appendix B; Borg, 1998). This
measure is simply a scale from 6 to 20 where 6 is “no exertion at all” and 20 is “maximal
exertion.” Participants point to a number that reflects their current feeling of exertion. This
measurement was taken at multiple time points directly after the participant conducted each of the
four exercises. The overall exertion level was then averaged to form a measure of that
participant’s effort during their physical activity session. The purpose of having this measure in
20
the experiment is that it allows for a measure of physical activity quality. Generally, in light of the
fact that certain intensity levels of physical activity are needed to meet recommendation levels
(Troiano et al., 2008), the more a participant is exerting themselves during their workout, the
higher quality their physical activity session will be. If we found that one randomly assigned
group exerted themselves more than the other, that result would be seen as a positive physical
activity measurement that separates the groups in a meaningful way. Test-retest reliability for this
scale has been found to be good, with correlations consistently being around 0.9 (Borg & Ohlsson,
1975; Ceci & Hassmen, 1991; Lamb, 1995; Eston & Williams, 1998). There has also been good
evidence for the validity of Borg’s Perceived Exertion Scale (Borg, 1977; Borg & Ottoson, 1986).
The next dependent variable is intrinsic motivation specific to the participant’s exercise
session (Appendix C). This scale was taken as part of a questionnaire the participants completed
after their physical activity session. The purpose of having this measure is to determine if
randomly assigned condition affects enjoyment and intrinsic motivation to engage in physically
active behaviors (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Murcia et al., 2008). The interest and enjoyment subscale of
the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory was used to assess this measure (Ryan, 1982). This subscale is
regarded as the specific measure of intrinsic motivation for the relevant behavior in which a
researcher is interested. The physical activity session in the experiment was the subject of the
questions. Research has found the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory to be adequately valid and
reliable in the realm of sports (McAuley, Duncan, & Tammen, 1989), and other experiments
related to other forms of physical activity (endurance tests) have found it to be reliable as well
(Tsigilis & Theodosiou, 2003). An example of a question on this scale is “I enjoyed doing this
activity very much” which is then rated on a likert scale from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true).
21
Given the possibility that participants who could receive acknowledgment, but do not,
might interpret this lack of acknowledgment as rejection, a potential covariate in this experiment
is rejection sensitivity (Appendix D). A scale was used in the questionnaire that measures
rejection sensitivity as “the disposition to anxiously expect, readily perceive, and overreact to
rejection.” The scale has shown good reliability (internal reliability: a = .83; test-retest reliability:
a = 0.78) and good validity (Downey & Feldman, 1996). This scale gives examples of situations
to participants, such as “you approach a close friend to talk after doing or saying something that
seriously upset him/her” and then goes on to ask 1) how concerned or anxious they would be
about their friend’s response, and 2) how they would expect their friend to respond. Each of the
eight items follows a similar pattern of concern/anxiety along with expectations in varying
situations.
(Appendix E). This measure is included because self-efficacy towards physical activity has been
found to be a very important predictor of subsequent exercise behavior (Rodgers & Brawley,
1991). The physical activity-specific self-efficacy measure for this experiment has been shown to
be both valid and reliable (Sallis, Pinski, Grossman, Patterson, & Nader, 1988). Items in this scale
are premised with the question “How sure are you that you can do these things?” An example item
Physical activity-specific social support was another potential covariate (Appendix F).
This potential covariate was included because some participants’ higher initial levels of social
support may act as a buffer to the ‘rejection’ of acknowledgment that exists in condition 3 (Cohen
& Wills, 1985). This was assessed by a thirteen item scale that asks questions about support from
family and friends in terms of exercise; this scale has shown acceptable reliability and validity
22
(Sallis, Grossman, Pinski, Patterson, & Nader, 1987). An example of an item on this scale is
“During the past three months, my family (or members of my household) or friends gave me
Lastly, a demographic variable used was body mass index (BMI). To be able to calculate a
person’s BMI, participants’ height and weight measurements were needed; these measurements
were then used to provide a proportion of mass to height (kg/m2). The higher that proportion is,
the higher the person’s BMI. BMI was used to obtain a measure of body fat percentage in our
participants. Although BMI is not a direct measurement of body fat, research has shown that it
correlates well with more direct measurements (Mei et al., 2002; Garrow & Webster, 1985). BMI
has been shown to be significantly related to physical activity levels (Thorp, Owen, Neuhass, &
Dunstan, 2011).
Analysis
First, all continuous outcomes were tested to ensure that they were normally distributed.
visually observe normality, 2) computing Skewness and Kurtosis in SPSS (version 23.0, Armonk,
NY), and 3) running the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality in SPSS. If an outcome was not normally
distributed, either non-parametric tests were conducted that do not assume normality or the
outcome variable was transformed before running parametric tests that do assume normality.
An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was then conducted for each potential covariate to see
if they differed by level of the independent variable. If any potential covariate differed by
condition, they would have been controlled for in the later analyses. ANOVAs were then used to
measure the effect of our independent variable on the six dependent variables, using an alpha level
of 0.05 to determine statistical significance. Chi-square analyses were then used to measure if goal
23
attainment differed by condition for each performance measure. Finally, correlation analyses were
used to determine if any potential covariate variables correlated significantly with any dependent
variables.
24
Chapter 3 - Results
Participant Characteristics
58 identified as female and 84 identified as male. One participant did not specify their gender. The
sample was composed primarily of first and second year undergraduates with a median age of 19
years old (M = 19.74, SD = 2.23). The sample was 73% Caucasian, 6% Hispanic, 4% Mexican,
3% Black, and 2% Chinese, while 12% identified as more than one race. Using measured height
and weight to calculate body mass index (BMI), the sample was predominantly (85%) of healthy
weight (BMI between 18.5 and 24.9), with 13% overweight (BMI between 25 and 29.9) and 2%
obese (BMI > 30). The average height of females was 64.9 inches (5’4.9”) with an average weight
of 138.2 pounds (BMI: M = 23.1 kg/m2, SD = 1.9). For males, the average height was 70.5 inches
(5’10.5”) with an average weight of 170.2 pounds (BMI: M = 24.1 kg/m2, SD = 2.2).
Tests of Normality
All continuous variables were tested to ensure normal distribution. If variables were non-
parametric statistical tests were then used. There were three methods by which normality was
tested. First, variables were plotted on histograms to visually assess normal distribution. Second,
kurtosis and skewness were tested against a comparison of +- 3.29 for each statistic divided by
their respective standard error. This number, with below 3.29 representing a normal distribution,
was used due to the sample of this experiment being medium-sized (Kim, 2013). Lastly, Shapiro-
Wilk’s test of normality was used at an a level of 0.001 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). A
25
Rejection sensitivity showed a normal distribution with skewness of -0.271 (SE = 0.204)
and kurtosis of -0.290 (SE = 0.406). Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was not significant (p =
0.065). Physical activity-specific self-efficacy also showed a normal distribution with skewness of
-0.397 (SE = 0.205) and kurtosis of -0.349 (SE = 0.407). Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was
distribution with skewness of 0.465 (SE = 0.203) and kurtosis of -0.208 (SE = 0.403). Shapiro-
Exertion showed a normal distribution with skewness of -0.246 (SE = 0.203) and kurtosis
of 0.414 (SE = 0.403). Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was not significant (p = 0.186). Intrinsic
motivation also showed a normal distribution with skewness of -0.270 (SE = 0.203) and kurtosis
of -0.083 (SE = 0.403). Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was not significant (p = 0.058).
The differences between participants’ goals and their exercise outcomes were used to test
normality for pushups, planks, jumping jacks, and the balancing exercise since these differences
were used in the later Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) analyses. Pushups showed a non-normal
distribution with skewness of 0.871 (SE = 0.206) and kurtosis of 3.472 (SE = 0.410), with
Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality significant (p < 0.001). Log (base 10) and square root
(Kruskal-Wallis) was later used for this variable. For planks, skewness of -0.226 (SE = 0.209) and
kurtosis of 1.379 (SE = 0.414) signified a slightly non-normal distribution, but Shapiro-Wilk’s test
of normality was not significant (p = 0.011), indicating normality. After visually observing
normality (Figure 1), this variable was used in a later ANOVA analysis. For jumping jacks,
skewness of -0.701 (SE = 0.204) and kurtosis of 0.728 (SE = 0.406) also signified a slightly non-
normal distribution, but once again Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was not significant (p =
26
0.002), indicating normality. After visually observing normality (Figure 2), this variable was also
used in a later ANOVA analysis. Lastly, the balancing exercise showed a non-normal distribution
with skewness of 0.941 (SE = 0.206) and kurtosis of 1.562 (SE = 0.408), with Shapiro-Wilk’s test
of normality significant (p < 0.001). A log (base 10) transformation was applied and normality
was produced, with this normal distribution having a skewness of 0.276 (SE = 0.206) and kurtosis
of 0.984 (SE = 0.408). Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality was not significant (p = 0.009).
Before analyzing our dependent variables, each potential covariate was tested to confirm
that they did not vary significantly by condition. None of the potential covariates, including
support differed significantly by condition. Because of this, these variables were not controlled for
in the major (ANOVA) analyses, although they were used in later correlation analyses to test if
they were significantly related to any dependent variables (these findings can be found in Results
subsection titled “correlation analyses”). Forty-six participants were randomly assigned to have
private goals, 49 to have their goals acknowledged, and 48 to have their goals go unacknowledged
when acknowledgment was possible. Goal averages, performance averages, and goal attainment
varied by condition due to this variable’s non-normal distribution. The average difference from
goal to result for each condition was used to measure performance. Data from three participants
were dropped due to either not setting a goal for pushups or the variation they specified on their
goal sheet did not match the variation they performed. When participants did not set a goal for an
exercise, this was due to them not feeling comfortable making an estimation of their performance
27
because of their unfamiliarity with the exercise. For pushups, participants with private goals
performed an average of 29.5 consecutive repetitions (SD = 15.7) with a +1.9 average difference
from goal to result (meaning they outperformed their goal by 1.9 repetitions), 27.9 repetitions (SD
= 12.3) for participants with acknowledged goals with a +1.8 average difference, and 30.2
repetitions (SD = 16.0) for participants with unacknowledged goals with a +2.0 average
difference. The analysis showed that pushup performance did not vary by condition, H(2, 138) =
1.426, p = 0.490. A chi-square test of independence was then conducted to see if goal attainment
differed by condition. The chi-square test showed that goal attainment differed significantly by
condition, X2 (3, N = 140) = 7.14, p = 0.028. Participants with private goals and acknowledged
goals had significantly higher goal attainment rates (90.7% and 89.8%, respectively) than did
Next, an ANOVA was used to test if plank performance varied by condition. Again, the
average difference from goal to result for each condition was used to measure performance. Data
from six participants were dropped due to either not setting a goal for planks or the variation they
specified on their goal sheet did not match the variation they performed. For planks, participants
with private goals held this exercise for an average of 79.0 seconds (SD = 35.3) with a +9.3
average difference from goal to result, 88.2 seconds (SD = 30.3) for participants with
acknowledged goals with a +20.1 average difference, and 73.4 seconds (SD = 22.4) for
participants with unacknowledged goals with a +7.5 average difference. The ANOVA showed that
plank performance was superior for participants with acknowledged goals, F(2, 135) = 3.32, p =
0.039. Effect sizes were then calculated for the mean differences between conditions in standard
deviation units. The largest effect size was between participants with acknowledged goals and
participants with goals that went unacknowledged (d = 0.486). The next largest was between
28
participants with acknowledged goals and participants with private goals (d = 0.418). Lastly, the
smallest effect size was between participants with private goals and participants with goals that
went unacknowledged (d = 0.075). A chi-square test of independence was then conducted to see if
goal attainment differed by condition. The chi-square test showed that goal attainment differed by
condition, X2 (3, N = 137) = 6.16, p = 0.046. Participants were most likely to accomplish their
plank goal if they had their goals acknowledged (87.5%) rather than having private goals (76.2%)
Next, an ANOVA was used to test if jumping jack performance varied by condition. Once
again, the average difference from goal to result for each condition was used to measure
performance. Data from two participants were dropped due to not setting a goal for jumping jacks.
For jumping jacks, participants with private goals had an average of 65.9 repetitions (SD = 11.2)
in one minute (+17.2 average difference from goal to result), 68.2 repetitions (SD = 6.5) for
participants with acknowledged goals (+16.1), and 66.7 repetitions (SD = 9.3) for participants
with unacknowledged goals (+16.0). The ANOVA showed that there was no significant difference
in performance by condition, F(2, 139) = 0.087, p = 0.916. A chi-square test of independence was
then conducted to see if goal attainment differed by condition. The chi-square test showed that
goal attainment did not differ by condition, X2 (3, N = 141) = 0.02, p = 0.990. Participants were
equally likely to accomplish their jumping jack goal if their goals were private (86.7%),
Another ANOVA was then used to test if balancing performance varied by condition.
Again, the average difference from goal to result for each condition was used to measure
performance. Data from four participants were dropped due to not setting a goal for the balancing
exercise. For balancing, participants with private goals held this exercise for an average of 36.3
29
seconds (SD = 34.7) with a -3.9 average difference from goal to result. Participants with
acknowledged goals averaged 31.9 seconds (SD = 30.7) with a -4.7 average difference. Finally,
participants with unacknowledged goals averaged 35.0 seconds (SD = 32.6) with a +4.6
difference. As showed by the ANOVA (using the transformed data), there was no significant
independence was then conducted to see if goal attainment differed by condition. The chi-square
test showed that goal attainment did not differ by condition, X2 (3, N = 139) = 0.18, p = 0.916.
Participants were equally likely to accomplish their balancing goal if they had their goals were
Two ANOVAs also showed that intrinsic motivation and average exertion did not
significantly differ by condition. Data from one participant was dropped in these analyses due to
missing data. Participants with acknowledged goals had the highest intrinsic motivation scores (M
= 36.3, SD = 6.2), followed by those with private goals (M = 35.8, SD = 7.1), and those with
unacknowledged goals (M = 34.9, SD = 5.9), although the ANOVA showed that these differences
were not significant, F(2, 140) = 0.604, p = 0.548. For average exertion, participants with
unacknowledged goals had the highest average exertion score (M = 12.8, SD = 1.6), followed by
those with acknowledged goals (M = 12.6, SD = 1.3), and those with private goals (M = 12.2, SD
= 1.8). Again, an ANOVA showed that these differences were not significant, F(2, 140) = 1.569, p
= 0.212.
Correlation Analyses
Correlation analyses were then conducted to observe the relationship between our
activity-specific social support) and our dependent variables (performance measures, intrinsic
30
motivation, and exertion). A correlation matrix can be seen in Table 3 showing the relationship
between each of these variables. Both physical activity-specific self-efficacy and physical activity-
specific social support had moderate size correlations with multiple dependent variables. First,
physical activity-specific self-efficacy had significant and positive correlations with intrinsic
motivation (r(138) = 0.220, p = 0.009), pushup repetitions (r(137) = 0.252, p = 0.003), and plank
seconds (r(138) = 0.310, p < 0.001), while physical-activity specific social support had significant
and positive correlations with pushup repetitions (r(140) = 0.177, p = 0.035), jumping jacks
(r(141) = 0.176, p = 0.035), and seconds balancing (r(141) = 0.238, p = 0.004). Rejection
sensitivity was the weakest predictor and did not have significant correlations with any of the
dependent variables.
31
Chapter 4 - Discussion
received from another person. Participants were asked to set goals regarding four exercises they
would subsequently perform. A third of participants were assigned to keep their goals for
themselves and not show them to the experimenter. Another third gave their goals to the
experimenter immediately after writing them and received positive acknowledgment of their
goals. The last third of participants were also assigned to give their goals to the experimenter, but
in their case the experimenter did not acknowledge their goals and instead immediately placed
them, face down, on a large stack of papers. The results from this experiment support some
hypotheses while providing no evidence for others – when one may receive acknowledgment,
there seems to be some positive effects of having goals acknowledged and some negative effects
of not having goals acknowledged. But, this effect was not seen across all dependent variables as
hypothesized.
Pushups were the first exercise participants engaged in, and there was a significant effect
of condition on goal attainment. Here, since participants with unacknowledged goals had a
significantly lower goal attainment rate than both participants with private goals and participants
who had their goals acknowledged, there seemed to be a negative effect in having goals go
unacknowledged when acknowledgment was possible. Since we did not see acknowledgment
enhance goal attainment rates over those who had private goals, this explanation of a negative
Planks were the next exercise participants engaged in and once again there was a
significant effect of condition on goal attainment. For planks though there was also a significant
32
effect of condition on goal-to-result averages. Participants who had their goals acknowledged had
significantly higher goal-to-result averages (meaning they outperformed their goals by the greatest
margin) than participants in the other two conditions. For goal-to-result averages, there was no
difference between participants with private goals and participants who had their goals go
acknowledged rather than goal determent from having goals go unacknowledged – the opposite of
what was seen in pushup goal attainment. But, this conclusion is based off the assumption that
participants who had private goals act as a “control” condition, which is not necessarily the case.
Since these participants knew they would be keeping their goals for themselves (although they
were asked for them back during the debriefing), this creates a second variable that is different
from the other two conditions in addition to acknowledgment. Not only did they not get their
goals acknowledged, they also would have never expected acknowledgment. For this reason, it
may be more valid to only make conclusions about whether having goals be acknowledged is
conditions two and three), although from interpreting the results across all four exercises it does
seem as if private goals are acting as a control since participants in this condition had relatively
average performance numbers – meaning that when there were significant differences between
conditions, participants with private goals had results in the middle of the three conditions. For
example, for goal attainment rates in planks, the group that reached their goals was the highest in
those who had their goals acknowledged, followed by lower rates by those with private goals, and
finally followed by even lower rates by those who had their goals go unacknowledged when
acknowledgment was possible. Here private goals seem to be acting as a control since those with
33
acknowledged goals saw significantly higher goal attainment rates, while those with
There were no significant differences by condition for jumping jacks, the third exercise
conducted, or the balancing exercise, which was the fourth and last exercise completed. There
may be reasons for this lack of differences between conditions in these exercises: first, there may
have been a ceiling effect for jumping jacks. This could be due to the fact that it would require
extraordinary effort to fit a significantly higher amount of jumping jacks than average into one
minute. Or, it is possible that differences in effort and physical fitness would not be expressed
until two or three minutes of jumping jacks have taken place and fatigue has significantly set in. In
addition, this exercise had the lowest goals being set for participants relative to actual
performance, meaning that the vast amount of participants overall were exceeding their goals by a
large margin, possibly further lowering their motivation to work extra hard to fit a lot of additional
jumping jacks into one minute. The low standard deviations across all conditions for jumping
jacks seems to indicate that their simply is not much variability to be expected in jumping jacks
As for the balancing exercise, there were very high standard deviations across all
conditions, likely reflecting participants’ lack of experience with the challenge. This lack of
experience may have made it difficult for participants to make goals and subsequently have a
decent chance at meeting them, as reflected in the low rates of goal attainment across conditions
relative to the other three exercises. This difficulty may have decreased the chance that the
manipulation would have an effect on results, but there are other reasons to consider as well.
The results across all four exercises suggest that the effect of goal-acknowledgment may
be more related to physical challenges relating to one’s persistence rather than physical attributes
34
such as speed or coordination. Concerning activities such as speed (jumping jacks) and balance
(single-leg balancing), it becomes harder to see the role that determination or persistence plays.
On the other hand, pushups and planks in the context of this experiment seem to be exercises
related to persistence. Participants often have to be willing to push past a pain threshold when
conducting pushups and a plank to failure rather than stop when it gets difficult. This is especially
true for those who set difficult goals but have a chance to reach them. Goal-acknowledgment may
make people more willing to push through a pain threshold when they perhaps know they could
do more but may not want to. With jumping jacks, it is unlikely that the pain threshold would be
reached unless participants were asked to do them for a much longer period of time than one
minute. In the same manner, for balancing it would be unlikely that persisting through a pain
barrier would play a role in performance unless participants were asked to hold a balance pose that
is perhaps easier, but more uncomfortable, enabling them to hold the pose for much longer than
our participants could hold an eyes-closed balancing pose. Further research can explore this
acknowledgment and persistence relationship to understand not only the strength of its
relationship, but also what other exercises it may extend to and in what contexts.
It is also important to consider that all participants did these exercises in the same order,
and pushups and planks were the exercises closest to the act of positive acknowledgment of goals
or no acknowledgment of goals. It is possible that the effect of this acknowledgment had worn off
by the time the participants began jumping jacks, which was the third exercise conducted.
This order effect may have had an effect on results, but there are also reasons to believe it
either did not have an effect or that the effect was minimal. First, the manipulation had more
pervasive effects on planks than it did pushups (reflected in the significant effects of goal
attainment and goal-to-result averages for planks), even though pushups were the first exercise
35
conducted and done immediately after the manipulation. Also, when goals were acknowledged
they were placed face up in a visible place, while when goals were not acknowledged (but
acknowledgment was possible) they were placed face down on a stack of papers in a visible place.
The goal document was visible for the participant throughout the experiment and likely made the
manipulation more salient for the participant throughout the experiment. Even with these reasons,
future research would be wise to randomize the order of exercises to confirm that order does not
The intrinsic motivation scale was assessed directly after participants were done with their
exercise session, and the questions pertained to the activity they had just participated in (the
exercise session). Although there were no significant differences by condition for intrinsic
motivation, we did see expected differences in the hypothesized direction and may have simply
needed more power to detect smaller effects. Future research that randomizes the order of
exercises, or places pushups and planks at the end of the exercise session, may see a stronger
result for intrinsic motivation due to the fact that the exercises done most recently are more salient
in the participants’ minds when completing the intrinsic motivation scale. Since the jumping jacks
and balancing exercise were done at the end of the exercise session and there were no significant
differences between conditions in these two exercises, having these two exercises be more salient
than pushups and planks for participants when completing the intrinsic motivation scale may have
Exertion also did not differ by condition. This may have been due to some confusion in the
scale for participants. Although the scale, and its validated instructions that were read to every
participant, put an emphasis on physical exertion and effort, many participants treated this scale as
a rating of difficulty. For example, many participants reported high exertion scores after the
36
balancing exercise, even when they had performed the exercise for a little amount of time. There
is no known reason to think that balancing would be more physically exhaustive than exercises
such as pushups and planks, but it was definitely more difficult for most participants as not only
had most of them not conducted it before, but it can be very difficult to do even with experience.
Again, since the manipulation seemed to have an effect on persistence more than any other
exercise variable, there is little reason why it would be expected to see differences in perceptions
of the difficulty of tasks across conditions. So although persistence in the face of discomfort
implies that more exertion would be needed, no differences may have been seen due to the fact
that participants misinterpreted this scale to ask for something other than actual exertion.
questionnaire at the end of the participants’ sessions, ended up being good predictors of dependent
variables. Physical activity-specific social support and self-efficacy both had significant
correlations with multiple dependent variables. This is not only intuitive (people who have more
support and are more confident in an area are likely to perform better in that area than those who
do not have support and are not confident), but it is also not surprising in light of Ryan and Deci’s
and relatedness (closely related to social support) are essential for motivation and behavior to
excel.
In addition to Ryan and Deci’s (2000) work on self-determination theory, the results from
this experiment coincide well with that of Todd and O’Connor’s (2005) research. They argued
that providing feedback in the short term not only increases motivation, but also the commitment
level to achieve the goal. They also found that feedback was more effective in a context with
positive language. Murcia et al. (2008) showed that a peer motivational climate of social support
37
predicts both motivation and enjoyment of exercise. When thinking of acknowledgment of goals
as a type of social support, the results from this experiment clearly strengthen Murcia et al.’s
(2008) claim regarding a positive effect of a peer motivational climate when engaging in physical
activity.
This experiment, and its results, also closely mirrored that of Vallerand and Reid (1984),
with a couple of important differences. Their study had participants conduct a motor task by doing
a balancing challenge multiple times in 20-second intervals with 20 seconds of rest between
intervals. The researchers only used participants that had at least a moderate level of intrinsic
motivation for the balancing task. There were three conditions in their experiment: in one,
participants received positive feedback during their balancing trials, such as being told they had a
natural ability at the task. Another condition had participants hear negative feedback, such as their
improvement between balancing trials was relatively slow. The last group did not get any
feedback. Participants in the positive feedback condition reported the highest levels of intrinsic
motivation for the activity, followed by those with no feedback, and then finally those who
received negative feedback – and these differences between each group in intrinsic motivation
were significant. But, the researchers did not measure actual performance in the balancing task,
which would be interesting to see and compare to the results from this experiment. Also, it is
important to realize that this positive and negative feedback was happening during the task of
interest instead of beforehand. Despite this, the differences between groups in their main
dependent variable – intrinsic motivation – varied in the same way that intrinsic motivation in this
study varied (although the differences were non-significant in this study). Also, the differences
they found in intrinsic motivation varied in the same way goal attainment differed for planks in
this study.
38
An obvious difference from these past studies described in comparison to this study is the
variable of feedback. Of course, acknowledgment of goals before the task of interest is different
from verbal feedback during the task of interest. From seeing similar results though in studies
looking at feedback during the task of interest, it seems as if acknowledgment of goals and
feedback may work in similar ways. To study this, future research using our experimental
paradigm could have separate conditions where the experimenter either provides positive
acknowledgment of goals before the task of interest or provides positive feedback during the task
of interest. The same could be done for no acknowledgment when it is possible and negative
feedback. If similar results were found between acknowledgment and feedback, there would be
further evidence that these two variables have similar effects on performance. Due to more
research having been done of feedback rather than goal-acknowledgment, this finding could be
More recent research may help to further understand this type of earlier feedback.
Oettingen, Marquardt, and Gollwitzer (2012) showed that more positive feedback results in
this feedback occurs before the task of interest. The researchers in this study manipulated
feedback of creative potential – they included a manipulation check that confirmed that those who
received strong positive feedback (e.g., told they are in 90 th percentile) believed they had better
creative potential for the experimental tasks (creative insight tasks) than those who received
moderate positive feedback (e.g., told they are in 60th percentile). Those who received the
strongest positive feedback and were using mental contrasting in their creative tasks had
significantly better performance than those who received less positive feedback and also used
mental contrasting. These results are especially noteworthy because this manipulation of feedback
39
occurred before undergoing the task of interest – which is similar to the manipulation of
Lastly, this experiment ended with important similarities to Ariely, Kamenica, and Prelec’s
(2008) work in behavioral economics. In both this study and their study, acknowledgment, or lack
behavior of interest relative to both experiments. Further, since the behavior in this study was
different than that in Ariely et al.’s (2008) research, there is now more evidence that the
There were other strengths of this research as well. The true experimental design allows
for causal conclusions that correlational research would not be able to make. This creates a good
springboard for other studies that are looking at similar effects. Could this detrimental effect of
lack of acknowledgment extend to other behaviors? How pervasive could this effect be? Does this
effect accentuate with multiple “rejections” of acknowledgment over a long period of time? How
much does it matter who the person is that is or is not acknowledging the goals?
Another strength of this research is that it focuses on a section of goal-related behavior that
is understudied: and that is what happens “after” one makes a goal. There is substantial research
on how to make a goal with a lack of understanding regarding what one should do after they set
the goal. Due to this lack of understanding, further research in this area has the potential to
drastically improve goal attainment. This study can serve as a starting point for experimental
There were some limitations with this research as well that are important to address. The
were Caucasian, with almost all participants between the ages of 18 and 22. In addition, the
40
majority of participants were of healthy weight. This was likely due to the recruitment description
of the experiment: participants were aware that they would be exercising, so only those who were
comfortable with this would likely sign up. Having a homogeneous sample has its setbacks. It
would be inappropriate to extrapolate these results to a general adult population due to the
sample’s narrow demographic diversity, and more research would need to be done on a more
diverse population if conclusions could be made that this effect of acknowledgment is widespread.
Additionally, the setting where this experiment took place could also be seen as not very
naturalistic since it was in a laboratory and not in a gym, athletic field, or other facility designated
for physical exercise – although having someone else watch (such as the experimenter is this
paradigm) could be likened to an exercise session in a gym where others are nearby. Additionally,
the paradigm conducted in this experiment could be seen as very similar to an exerciser working
with a personal trainer. It could also be argued that the experimental realism in this study was
high, meaning that the participants were highly engaged in the task they were assigned to
(exercising). Overall, intrinsic motivation scores across all conditions were very high. The
intrinsic motivation scale was framed to have the participant reflect directly on the exercise
session they had just engaged in (meaning that they were reflecting on the experiment), including
questions asking about the enjoyment of the activity and also how much the activity held their
attention. Seven questions were asked regarding intrinsic motivation, with each question on a
seven point Likert scale. In each condition the average response was at a level of five out of seven
Another possible limitation of this research is that the experimenter was not a close peer to
the participant, and they were also not an actual personal trainer. This limits how much the results
from this study can be directly applied to the real world, but it is also very possible that the effect
41
of acknowledgment could be strengthened if the person receiving the goals was either a trusted
peer or an actual personal trainer – this could be due to the close peer who is receiving the goals
possibly being someone the exerciser deeply respects and cares about (therefore taking seriously
their opinion or the way they react to their goals), or simply a personal trainer being an individual
with expertise, and therefore their opinion or reaction to physical activity goals would likely be
taken seriously. In these cases it is plausible to think that these important others acknowledging
goals would mean more to the goal-setter than an experimenter acknowledging goals, and
Finally, another limitation of this research is its short-term nature. The effects that were
found from acknowledgment, or the lack of acknowledgment, were only a few minutes removed
from the manipulation in this experiment. But, it is important to consider that this short-term
effect is all that was hoped to be found in this experiment, but now that it has been shown that this
effect can occur within physical-activity goals, there is merit to study this effect further. Future
studies would be wise to see if this effect could have a long-term impact – possibly from repeated
possible. Finding a long-term effect would only strengthen the argument that acknowledgment of
exercise goals has the potential to play a large role in goal attainment and exercise performance.
Conclusion
Regular physical activity has been shown to have substantial physical and mental benefits,
ranging from a protection against obesity to a greater quality of life (Harvard School of Public
Health, 2014; Faulkner & Taylor, 2005). Yet, a low percentage of people in the United States
meet recommended levels of physical activity (Troiano et al., 2008). Additionally, goal setting has
been shown to be an effective way to improve behavior (Locke & Latham, 1990; Latham &
42
Budworth, 2006), but clearly has room to be more effective for exercise with low rates of overall
physical activity. While an abundance of research exists that investigates how one should set a
goal, there is a lack of research, especially in physical activity, concerning what one should do
with that goal after setting it. The research described throughout this paper addresses that
experiment, the role of goal-acknowledgment has been shown to have an effect on exercise
performance and has merit to be studied further in different contexts. A deeper understanding of
goal-acknowledgment has the potential to form a clearer picture of what one should do after
setting a physical activity-goal, possibly leading to higher levels of goal attainment and exercise
performance.
43
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. Springer Berlin
Heidelberg. 11-39.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision
Ariely, D., Kamenica, E., & Prelec, D. (2008). Man’s search for meaning: The case of Legos.
Baranowski, T. (1988). Validity and reliability of self report measures of physical activity: an
314-327.
Borg, G. (1998). Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign, IL, US: Human Kinetics.
Borg, G. & Ohlsson, M. (1975). A study of two variants of a simple run-test for determining
physical working capacity. Reports from the Institute of Applied Psychology (61).
Borg, G. & Ottoson, D. (1986). The perception of exertion in physical work. London:
Macmillan.
Bouchard, C., Blair, S.N., & Haskell, W.L.E. (2007). Physical activity and health. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Caspersen, C.J., Powell, K.E., & Christenson, G.M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and
physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health
Ceci, R., & Hassmen, P. (1991). Self-monitored exercise at three different RPE-intensities in
treadmill versus field running. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 23, 732-
44
738.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013). The health effects of overweight and
ob245
Cohen, S. & Wills, T.A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis.
Danaei, G., Ding, E.L., Mozaffarian, D., Taylor, B., Rehm, J., Murray, C.J.L., & Ezzati, M.
(2009). The preventable causes of death in the United States: comparative risk
assessment of dietary, lifestyle, and metabolic risk factors. PLos Medicine 6(4):e100058
Downey, G. & Feldman, S.I. (1996). Implications of Rejection Sensitivity for Intimate
Durstine, J.L., Gordon, B., Wang, Z., & Luo, X. (2013). Chronic disease and the link to physical
Eheman, C., Henley, S.J., Ballard-Barbash, R., Jacobs, E.J., Schymura, M.J., Noone, A.M, Pan,
L., Anderson, R.N., Fulton, J.E., Kohler, B.A., Jemal, A., Ward, E., Plescia, M., Ries,
L.A.G., & Edwards, B.K. (2012). Annual report to the nation on the status of cancer.
Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis
Eston, R.G. & Williams, J.G. (1998). Reliability of ratings of perceived effort for regulation of
Faulker, G.E.J. & Taylor, A.H. (2005). Exercise, health and mental health: Emerging
45
Garrow, J.S. & Webster, J. (1985). Quetelet’s index (W/H2) as a measure of fatness.
Greaves, C.J., Sheppard, K.E., Abraham, C., Hardeman, W., Roden, M., Evans, P.H., & Schwarz,
increased effectiveness in dietary and physical activity interventions. BMC Public Health,
11:119.
Hagger, M.S., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D., & Biddle, S.J.H. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the
theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity
and the contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
24(1), 3-32.
Harvard School of Public Health. (2014). Obesity Prevention Source. Retrieved from http://www
.hsph.harvard.edu/obesity-prevetion-source/obesity-causes/physical-activity-and-obesity
Hordern, M.D., Dunstan, D.W., Prins, J.B., Baker, M.K., Fiatarone Singh, M.A., & Coombes,
J.S. (2012). Exercise prescription for patients with type 2 diabetes and pre-diabetes.
Katzmarkzyk, P.T., Gledhill, N., & Shephard, R.J. (2000). The economic burden of physical
Kim, H.-Y. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution using
Kramer, A.F., Erickson, K.I., & Colcombe, J. (2006). Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain.
Lamb, K.L. (1995). Children’s ratings of effort during cycle ergometry: An examination of two
46
Latham, G.P. & Budworth, M. (2006). The effect of training in verbal self-guidance on the self-
efficacy and performance of Native North Americans in the selection interview. Journal
Locke, E.A. & Latham, G.P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood
Locke, E.A. & Latham, G.P. (2013). New developments in goal setting and task performance.
Routledge.
Martin, S.B., Morrow, J.R., Jackson, A.W., & Dunn, A.L. (2000). Variables related to meeting
the ACSM/CDC physical activity guidelines. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise,
32, 2087-2092.
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V.V. (1989). Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic
McDonnel, M.N., Hillier, S.L., Roth, D.L., Judd, S.E., Haley, W.E, Esterman, A.J., & Howard,
V.J. (2015). Abstract W P176: Self-reported pre-stroke physical activity levels influence
Mei, Z., Grummer-Strawn, L.M., Pietrobelli, A., Goulding, A., Goran, M.I., & Dietz, W.H.
(2002). Validity of body mass index compared with other body-composition screening
indexes for the assessment of body fatness in children and adolescents. American Journal
Mokdad, A.H., Marks, J.S., Stroup, D.F., & Gerberding, J.L. (2004). Actual causes of death in the
United States, 2000. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 291(10),
1238-1245.
47
Murcia, J., San Roman, M., Galindo, C., Alonso, N., & Gonzalez-Cutre, D. (2008). Peers’
National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. (2005). Behavioral Risk
Oettingen, G., Marquardt, M.K., & Gollwitzer, P.M. (2012). Mental contrasting turns positive
Ogden, C.L., Carroll, M.D., Kit, B.K., & Flegal, K.M. (2014). Prevalence of childhood and adult
obesity in the United States, 2011–2012. Journal of the American Medical Association
Pate, R.R., Pratt, M., Blair, S.N., Haskell, W.L., Macera, C.A., Bouchard, C.,… Wilmore, J.A.
(1995). Physical activity and public health: A recommendation from the Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. Journal of
Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. (2008). Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory
Committee Report, 2008. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services.
Rodgers, W.M. & Brawley, L.R. (1991). The role of outcome expectations in participation
Ryan, R.M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of
cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450-461.
Ryan, R.M. & Deci, E.L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
48
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68-78.
Sallis, J.F., Grossman, R.M., Pinski, R.B., Patterson, T.L., & Nader, P.R. (1987). The
Sallis, J.F., Pinski, R.B., Grossman, R.M., Patterson, T.L., & Nader, P.R. (1988). The
Sattelmair, J., Pertman, J., Ding, E.L., Kohl III, H.W., Haskell, W., Lee, I. (2011). Dose response
between physical activity and risk of Coronary Heart Disease. Circulation, 124, 789-795.
Shilts, M.K., Horowitz, M., & Townsend, M. (2004). Goal setting as a strategy for dietary and
19(2), 81-93.
Swezey, R.W., Meltzer, A.L., & Salas, E. (1994). Some issues involved in motivating teams.
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2007). Experimental designs using ANOVA.
Thomson/Brooks/Cole.
Thorp, A.A., Owen, N., Neuhaus, M., & Dunstan, D.W. (2011). Sedentary behaviors and
207-215.
Todd, C. & O’Connor, J. (2005). Clinical Supervision. In N. Skinner, A.M. Roche, J. O’Connor,
Y. Pollard, & C. Todd (Eds.), Workforce Development TIPS (Theory Into Practice
49
Strategies): A Resource Kit for the Alcohol and Other Drugs Field. National Centre for
Troiano, R.P., Berrigan, D., Dodd, K.W., Masse, L.C., Tilert, T., & McDowell, M. (2008).
Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Medicine & Science in
Tsigilis, N. & Theodosiou, A. (2003). Temporal stability of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory.
Vallerand, R.J. & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of perceived competence on intrinsic
Wang, G., Pratt, M., Macera, C.A., Zheng, Z., & Heath, G. (2004). Physical activity,
Warnecke, R.B., Johnson, T.P., Chavez, N., Sudman, S., O'Rourke, D.P., Lacey, L., & Horm, J.
50
Appendix A
Goals
I want to do _____ repetitions of pushups doing variation
# ____.
Variation 1: On knees using table
variation # ____.
pushup)
____ seconds.
51
Appendix B
“While exercising we want you to rate your perception of exertion, i.e., how heavy and strenuous
the exercise feels to you. The perception of exertion depends mainly on the strain and fatigue in
your muscles and on your feeling of breathlessness or aches in the chest. Look at this rating scale;
we want you to use this scale from 6 to 20, where 6 means ‘no exertion at all’ and 20 means
‘maximal exertion.’ Try to appraise your feeling of exertion as honestly as possible, without
thinking about what the actual physical load is. Don’t underestimate it, but don’t overestimate it
either. It’s your own feeling of effort and exertion that’s important, not how it compares to other
people’s. What other people think is not important either. Look at the scale and the expressions
and then give a number. Any Questions?”
6 No exertion at all
8 Extremely light
9 Very light
10
11 Light
12
13 Somewhat hard
14
15 Hard (heavy)
16
17 Very hard
18
19 Extremely hard
20 Maximal exertion
52
Appendix C
Subset of Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) that Measures Intrinsic Motivation Post-
Experimentally
“For each of the following statements, please indicate how true it is for you, using the following
scale:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. While I was doing this activity, I was thinking about how much I enjoyed it
53
Appendix D
Rejection Sensitivity
“Each of the items below describes things college students sometimes ask of other people. Please
imagine that you are in each situation. You will be asked to answer the following questions:
1) How concerned or anxious would you be about how the other person would respond?
1 2 3 4 5 6
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. You ask your parents for help in deciding what programs to apply to.
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your parents would want to help
you?
2. You approach a close friend to talk after doing or saying something that seriously upset
him/her.
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your friend would want to talk with
you?
I would expect that he/she would want to talk with me to try to work things out.
3. After graduation, you can’t find a job and ask your parents if you can live at home for a
while.
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your parents would want you to
come home?
4. You call your boyfriend/girlfriend after a bitter argument and tell him/her you want to
see him/her.
54
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your boyfriend/girlfriend would
want to see you?
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your parents would want to come?
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your friend would do this favor?
I would expect that he/she would willingly do this favor for me.
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not your boyfriend/girlfriend would say
yes?
8. You go to a party and notice someone on the other side of the room and then you ask
them to dance.
How concerned or anxious would you be over whether or not the person would want to dance
with you?
55
Appendix E
Exercise-Specific Self-Efficacy
“Below is a list of things people might do while trying to increase or continue regular exercise.
We are interested in exercises like running, swimming, brisk walking, bicycle riding, or aerobics
classes. Whether you exercise or not, please rate how confident you are that you could really
motivate yourself to do things like these consistently, for at least six months. Please circle one
number for each question. How sure are you that you can do these things?:
1 2 3 4 5
4. Set aside time for a physical activity program; that is, walking, jogging, swimming, biking,
5. Continue to exercise with others even though they seem too fast or too slow for you.
6. Stick to your exercise program when undergoing a stressful life change (e.g. divorce, death
8. Stick to your exercise program when your family is demanding more time from you.
9. Stick to your exercise program when you have household chores to attend to.
10. Stick to your exercise program even when you have excessive demands at work.
11. Stick to your exercise program when social obligations are very time consuming.
56
Appendix F
“Below is a list of things people might do or say to someone who is trying to exercise regularly. If
you are not trying to exercise, then some of the questions may not apply to you, but please read
and give an answer to every question. Please rate each question twice. Under family, rate how
often anyone living in your household has said or done what is described during the last three
months. Under friends, rate how often your friends, acquaintances, or coworkers have said or done
what is described during the last three months. Please write one number from the following rating
scale in each space:
1 2 3 4 5
During the past three months, my family (or members of my household) or friends:”
57
Appendix G
Table 1
Females
Males
58
Table 2
Results summary for goal averages, performance averages, and goal attainment percentages. Pushups
and jumping jacks are represented in repetitions while planks and balancing are represented in seconds.
Goal Averages
Performance Averages
Goal Attainment %
Note: within column values with different superscripts are significantly different at p<0.05
59
Table 3
SS ----
RS .02 ----
SE .27** .21* ----
IM .13 .01 .22** ----
E .06 .01 .03 .19* ----
Pushups .18* .03 .25** .10 .14 ----
Planks .09 .08 .31*** .28** .26** .42*** ----
JJs .18* .06 .13 .44*** .16 .26** .34*** ----
Balancing .24** .11 .05 .15 .06 .19* .30*** .09
60
Appendix H
61
Figure 2. Jumping jack scores
62