Thanks to visit codestin.com
Credit goes to www.scribd.com

0% found this document useful (0 votes)
192 views29 pages

Pilot Centrifuge Mechanism

This document discusses a method called COHAM (Coherent Alignment Method) that aims to reduce Coriolis effects in centrifuge simulators by allowing small mismatches in the alignment of the G-vector. Simulations show that as long as these mismatches remain within a region where humans perceive the G-vector as coherent, cabin rotations and angular accelerations can be reduced. The method was experimentally tested on a high-G maneuver task and found to reduce sickness, dizziness, and discomfort for pilots, making centrifuge sessions more tolerable. Further improvements to the filter design and additional testing with fighter pilots are recommended.

Uploaded by

Gourav Das
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
192 views29 pages

Pilot Centrifuge Mechanism

This document discusses a method called COHAM (Coherent Alignment Method) that aims to reduce Coriolis effects in centrifuge simulators by allowing small mismatches in the alignment of the G-vector. Simulations show that as long as these mismatches remain within a region where humans perceive the G-vector as coherent, cabin rotations and angular accelerations can be reduced. The method was experimentally tested on a high-G maneuver task and found to reduce sickness, dizziness, and discomfort for pilots, making centrifuge sessions more tolerable. Further improvements to the filter design and additional testing with fighter pilots are recommended.

Uploaded by

Gourav Das
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 29

Mitigating Coriolis Effects in Centrifuge Simulators

Through Allowing Small, Unperceived G-Vector Misalignments

Tigran Mkhoyan,∗ Mark Wentink,† Bernd de Graaf‡


Desdemona B. V., Kampweg 5, 3769 DE Soesterberg, The Netherlands.

M. M. (René) van Paassen§ and Max Mulder¶


TU Delft, Faculty of Aerospace Engineering, Control and Simulation, The Netherlands

When coupled with additional degrees of freedom, centrifuge-based motion platforms can
arXiv:2202.02677v1 [eess.SY] 6 Feb 2022

combine the agility of hexapod-based platforms with the ability to sustain higher G-levels
and an extended motion space, required for simulating extreme maneuvers. However, the
false and often nauseating sensations of rotation, by Coriolis effects induced by the centrifuge
rotation in combination with rotations of the centrifuge cabin or the pilot’s head, are a major
disadvantage. This paper discusses the development of a motion filter, the Coherent Alignment
Method (COHAM), which aims at reducing Coriolis effects by allowing small mismatches in
the G-vector alignment, reducing cabin rotations. Simulations show that as long as these
mismatches remain within a region where humans perceive the G-vector as ‘coherent’, the
Coherent Alignment Zone (CAZ), the cabin angular accelerations can indeed be reduced.
COHAM was tested in a high G-maneuver task with a fixed CAZ threshold obtained in a
previous study. It was experimentally compared to an existing motion filter, using metrics such
as sickness, comfort and false cues. Results show that sickness, dizziness and discomfort are
reduced, making the centrifuge sessions more bearable. It is recommended to further improve
the filter design and tuning, and test it with more fighter pilots.

Acronyms

ALAC Accumulated Level of Alignment Coherency of the G-cueing


AMS Alignment Mismatch Score
AWS Accumulated Well-being Score
CAZ Coherent Alignment Zone
COHAM Coherent Alignment Method
DFS Dynamic Flight Simulators
DOF Degrees-of-Freedom
FOV Field of View
GIA Gravito Inertial Acceleration
JND just noticeable difference
LOC-I loss of control in-flight
MISC MIsery SCale
PBWS Preference-Based Well-being Score
SCC Semi Circular Canals
TTPF Two-Tailed Peak Flattening
UPRT Upset Prevention and Recovery Training
∗ Ph.D. student, [email protected]
† Director technology, Desdemona B. V., [email protected]
‡ Director, Desdemona B. V., [email protected]
§ Associate Professor, [email protected]
¶ Professor, [email protected]

1
Nomenclature

A, B = entry point and climb-out point of trajectory (MISC evaluation)


ℎ 𝛼 ,ℎ 𝛼 ,ℎ 𝛼
𝑥 𝑦 𝑧 = angular accelerations in head-centric axes 𝑥,𝑦,𝑧, ◦ /s2
𝑎 𝑡 , 𝑎𝑟 = linear tangential and radial accelerations during centrifugation, m/s2
C1, C2 = motion condition in Desdemona, Rocket Man, COHAM filter
𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 = instantaneous simulator G-level, g-units
𝐺 𝑏𝑎𝑠𝑒𝑙𝑖𝑛𝑒 = baseline G-level equal to 1.4 G, g-units
𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 = instantaneous G-vector predicted by the COHAM, g-units
𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥 = maximum unfiltered G-level, g-units
𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓 = maximum unfiltered G-level, g-units
𝐺 𝑥, 𝐺𝑦, 𝐺𝑧 = gravitational accelerations in 𝑥, 𝑦 and 𝑧, m/s2
𝐺˜ 𝑚𝑎𝑥 = maximum filtered G-level, g-units
𝑔 = gravitational acceleration, m/s2
𝑁 𝑥 , 𝑁𝑦 , 𝑁𝑧 = normal accelerations in 𝑥, 𝑦 and 𝑧, m/s2
N1-N4 = experiment evaluation days
𝑅𝑐 = centrifuge arm, m
S1-S3 = experiment subjects
𝑡 𝐴, 𝑡 𝐵 = time point before and after the onset peak in TTPF, s
𝑈𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 = COHAM cabin angle deviation, ◦
𝑢 𝑝𝑖𝑡 𝑐ℎ , 𝑢𝑟 𝑜𝑙𝑙 = stick signal in pitch and roll, lbf
𝑋¯ 𝑑𝑎𝑡 = table input mapping vector G-level, g-units
𝑌¯𝑑𝑎𝑡 = table output mapping vector alignment angle mapping, ◦
𝜃 = rotation around the cabin or the head,◦
Ω𝑐 = centrifuge main axis yaw rate,◦ /s
𝜔𝑐 = cabin rotation rate,◦ /s

Subscripts

true = true cabin alignment deviation or angle


COHAM = COHAM cabin alignment deviation or angle
min, max = minimum, maximum
lower, upper = lower and upper threshold of cabin alignment angle
𝑠𝑠 = steady state value of velocity

I. Introduction
assive G-training has been the main application of centrifuge-based simulators. In this type of training, pilots are
P required to sustain a prescribed G-plateau (up to 9G for fighter pilots) for a fixed duration of time (10 seconds),
without losing consciousness (known as G-lock [1]). Fig. 1a shows an example of a typical human centrifuge used
for passive training. Active G-training, in contrast, would allow to simulate more realistic dynamic flight scenarios
where pilots have active control over the Degrees-of-Freedom (DOF). Dynamic Flight Simulators (DFS) have been in
development for the past four decades [2–5]. An example of such a dynamic flight simulator is Desdemona, Fig. 1b,
which has 6-DOF and allows pilots to control three or more DOFs in a simulation [6].

2
Fighter pilots are must be capable of carrying out mentally demanding tasks, while simultaneously enduring
significant impact from G-forces on their bodies during maneuvering. The strain resulting from the high G-levels has
shown to deteriorate pilot performance and decision-making [7, 8]. Significant training is required to build resistance
and the ability to perform in these conditions [9]. Active G-training has the potential to provide a realistic platform
where pilots can safely practice dangerous scenarios at a much lower cost [8, 10]; G-suits or G-seats are an alternative
[11, 12]. Ref. [13] provides a comprehensive review of research carried out in Dynamic Flight Simulators (DFS) and its
usefulness in various simulation scenarios.
Upset Prevention and Recovery Training (UPRT) is one example of such scenario [14], where adequate simulation
training can help reduce loss of control in-flight (LOC-I), a significant contributor to fatal accidents [15, 16]. Various
efforts have been undertaken to reduce LOC-I, such as predictive cueing [17], haptic feedback [18] and adaptive flight
control systems [19, 20]. In the European research project ‘Simulation of Upset Recovery in Aviation’ (SUPRA),
centrifuge-based simulation has shown to significantly improve pilot’s ability to recover from adverse flight attitudes
[21–23]. Apart from enabling psycho-physical studies on human perception [24, 25], the artificial gravity generated by
short-radius centrifuges is a promising countermeasure to halt the deterioration of astronauts’ health in space [26–28].

Conventional 2-DOF

Cabin

Rc Yaw Axis

𝜽roll
Roll Axis

(a) 2-DOF centrifuge platform (b) 6-DOF Desdemona platform

Figure 1 Conventional and Desdemona centrifuge platforms.

The main limitation to the application of centrifuges is the Coriolis cross-coupling – also referred to as Coriolis
effects – induced at the pilot’s head, during simultaneous rotations of the cabin (or gondola) and the centrifuge’s central
yaw rotation. This cross-coupling can trigger various sensory reactions such as eye movements, illusions, disorientation
and motion sickness [29–32]. While adaptation in training has been demonstrated to the vestibulo-ocular reflex [33] and
the Corolis-inducing head movements [34], (severe) motion sickness symptoms remain a limitation to the tolerably of
centrifuges [8, 35].
Coriolis rotations can be induced passively and actively. In ‘passive Coriolis’, the pilot’s head is fixed relative to
the cabin and the cross-coupling is caused by the cabin motion required to align the G-vector. In ‘active Coriolis’, the
cross-coupling is caused by the pilot rotating the head. Whereas active Coriolis cannot be controlled without stringent
pilot instruction or adaptation training [33], passive Coriolis can, this is the subject of this paper.
In most practical flight scenarios, for example pulling sharp turns in an F-16, one aims to replicate the G-forces
experienced in reality as well as possible in the centrifuge (within the allowable limits, 3G for Desdemona), Fig. 2.
For this purpose, the subject must be aligned with the resultant G-vector along the vertical axis, through rotating the
cabin towards the centrifuge center. The sharp pull-up maneuver can peak as high as 9G (maximum for F-16), which
corresponds to 3G and a cabin roll or pitch angle (here 𝜃 𝑠𝑖𝑚 ) in the order of 70 degrees.
The magnitude of the G-level is dictated by the rotation rate Ω𝑐 of the centrifuge central yaw-axis. For each change
in G-level, a transient referred to as “G-onset”, the cabin must rotate accordingly to maintain a correct alignment of the
resulting G-vector. This combination of cabin alignment rotation and the centrifuge’s yaw rotation causes the passive
Coriolis effect introduced above. The experienced Coriolis rotation is proportional to both the central yaw and the cabin
alignment rotations: the higher these rotations, the stronger the Coriolis effect. Hence, to mitigate the passive Coriolis
effect, either one or both rotations would need to be made smaller.

3
Reality Simulator

trajectory

𝜽ac 𝜽sim
9G
Rac Rc 3G
center of rotation

Ωc

Figure 2 G-cueing during F-16 high-G maneuver in Desdemona.

Since the central yaw rotation rate is controlled to properly simulate the required G-level, the only degree of freedom
that remains in mitigating the Coriolis effect is the cabin alignment rotation. In this paper, it is hypothesized that small
deviations from the proper cabin alignment angle are acceptable, as long as these deviations are not perceived by pilots.
The concept of manipulating the cabin rotation angle and rotation rate will lead to a novel motion cueing filter, the
Coherent Alignment Method, or COHAM.
The principle of COHAM is to align the centrifuge cabin with a minimum amount of rotation, while not allowing the
pilot to notice the mismatch in the G-alignment during each G-onset. The positive (upward) and negative (downward)
limits of the mismatch are determined by the hypothetical Coherent Alignment Zone (CAZ), defined as a region where
the deviation from the correct cabin angle is still perceived as coherent and consistent by pilots. Details on how this
CAZ zone is determined are discussed in Mkhoyan et al. [36], only the main results will be discussed here.
COHAM advances the state-of-the-art in centrifuge simulator motion cueing [37–39], a problem even more
challenging than motion cueing for common 6-DOF Stewart platforms which are typically based on classical washout
[40–42], robust control [43], tuned using insights from pilot models or behavior [44–46], and modal analysis [47].
This paper describes the development and experimental evaluation of the COHAM filter and is structured as follows.
In Section II some background information is provided on centrifuge cueing and Coriolis cross-couplings. The concept
of the CAZ coherence zone and results from a first experiment to determine the mismatch limits are summarized in
Section III. The COHAM motion cueing algorithm rationale is discussed in Section IV. A second experiment has
been performed to test COHAM, Section V, the results of which are discussed in Section VI. The paper ends with a
discussion, Section VII and conclusions in Section VIII.

II. Background

A. Centrifuge Platforms
To attain sustained G-levels, the centrifuge base needs to spin along the central yaw axis, Fig. 3a. This yaw velocity,
denoted as Ω𝑐 can be as high as 150 degrees/s [48] in Desdemona. The desired constant G-level, usually referred to as
the magnitude of the resultant Gravito Inertial Acceleration (GIA) corresponds to a steady-state value Ω𝑐𝑠𝑠 . From the
perspective of the pilot situated in the cabin, the GIA coincides with the gravitational acceleration z-direction 𝐺 𝑧 for
most practical simulation scenarios. A free body diagram of accelerations is shown in Fig. 3b for the case of constant
centrifugation at a 2G level.
The resultant G-vector, also referred to as GIA, can be expressed as follows:
√︃
𝐺 𝐼 𝐴 = 𝑎 𝑡 2 + 𝑎 𝑅 2 + 𝑔2 , (1)

4
Su
Ve bj.
rti
cal

Ωc

(b) Free Body Diagram of linear accelera-


tions at constant centrifuge yaw rota-
(a) Schematic overview of the subject’s orientation tion of 2.06 rad/s

Figure 3 Schematics of subject’s orientation in centrifuge cueing in the Desdemona simulator (here configured for the
‘Rocket Man’ cueing, i.e., alignment of the cabin in pitch).

where 𝑎 𝑡 and 𝑎 𝑅 are the tangential and radial linear accelerations resulting from centrifugation, respectively, and 𝑔 the
gravitational constant. Here, 𝑎 𝑅 can be expressed as:

𝑎 𝑅 = Ω2𝑐 · 𝑅𝑐 , (2)

with 𝑅𝑐 the distance from the cabin to the centrifuge central axis (4 m in Desdemona).
During the centrifugation phase, the cabin needs to ‘swing out’ to keep the true orientation of the human-vertical
axis 𝐺 𝑧 concerning the resultant G-vector, see Fig. 3b. For a constant centrifuge rotation the correct steady-state cabin
angle required for alignment, 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 , can be computed as:
   2 
𝑎𝑅 Ω𝑐 𝑅 𝑐
𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (𝑡) = arctan = arctan (3)
𝑔 𝑔

Higher G-levels require increasingly higher yaw-rates Ω𝑐 , which in turn require larger cabin rotation angles 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 . Both
relations are quadratic in nature.

B. Cabin Rotation Options


Over the years, two G-force alignment cueing solutions were developed for the Desdemona motion simulator [49],
see Fig. 4. The first is a conventional solution where the pilot faces tangential to the rotation and where 𝐺 𝑧 is aligned
through rotating the cabin in roll, Fig. 4a. The second is a solution where the pilot faces inward towards the center of
rotation and where 𝐺 𝑧 is aligned through rotating the cabin forward in pitch, Fig. 4b. This latter solution, nicknamed the
“Rocket Man”, or RM for short, is reported by pilots to be more comfortable and appears to cause less motion sickness
for centrifuge-based UPRT [22].

C. Coriolis Cross-coupling
When the cabin is allowed to have a DOF of angular rotation (roll or pitch), in an axis other than that of the main
yaw-axis, simultaneous rotations will induce a Coriolis effect [50]. From the discussion above it is clear that, since cabin
rotations are required to align the subjective vertical, or G-vector, this cross-coupling is inevitable. For pilots situated
inside the cabin, this cross-coupling can be experienced as a sensation of tumbling, nausea and or dizziness [51].
Although the perceived Coriolis effect is related to the (inaccurate) vestibular responses of the Semi Circular Canals
(SCC) – and therefore varies between subjects – its kinematic cause can be computed. Considering that the pitch, roll
and yaw SCC canals have a specific orientation∗ with respect to the horizontal plane, the angular acceleration arriving at
∗ The exact orientation differs from person to person. The yaw-axis is, in fact, slightly tilted downward to accommodate for our default orientation

of the head (slightly looking downward).

5
Su Su
Ve bj. Ve bj.
rti rti
cal cal
aR aR

GIA GIA
Ωc g Ωc g
θ θ
Rc Rc
(a) Conventional cueing (alignment in roll) (b) ‘Rocket Man’ cueing (alignment in pitch)

Figure 4 Illustration of the two cueing orientations used in Desdemona.

Figure 5 Standard coordinates and unit vectors for specification of head angular motion (from [52]).

the pilot’s head can be decomposed into three axes as shown in Fig. 5 [52]. The resulting angular accelerations are the
head-centric, ℎ 𝛼𝑋 , ℎ 𝛼𝑌 and ℎ 𝛼 𝑍 , accelerations expressed by Holly [52–54]:
ℎ 𝛼 𝑥  
   𝜃¥ 

ℎ    ¤ sin(𝜃) 
𝜶 = ℎ 𝛼 𝑦  =  Ω𝜃¤ cos(𝜃) + Ω (4)
 𝛼𝑧  −Ω𝜃¤ sin(𝜃) + Ω ¤ cos(𝜃) 
ℎ   
   
This set of equations describe a typical case of head tilt, rotation around the head-centric-axis, in the presence of
simultaneous whole-body rotation. The latter represents the default, ‘rotating chair’, configuration for fundamental
Coriolis-related experiments [54, 55]. Here, 𝜃 represents the rotation around the head-𝑥-axis, 𝜃¤ the angular head rotation
rate, 𝜃¥ the head rotational acceleration; Ω represents the angular rate of the rotating reference frame (the chair, or, in our
case, the centrifuge) and, Ω, ¤ the angular acceleration of the rotating frame.
Fig. 6a exemplifies Coriolis cross-coupling, where the subject initiates 90 degrees of head tilt in roll (𝑥-axis) while
the centrifuge is rotating with a constant yaw rate Ω𝑐 . Due to the rotation around the 𝑥-axis, the subject’s 𝑦 and 𝑧 axes
are tilted into the centrifugal rotation plane and components of Ω𝑐 appear in both axes. The resulting cross-coupled
stimulus is sensed by the SCC in these two axes, causing a sensation of tumbling, nausea and dizziness [51]. This form
of active Coriolis can be mitigated by instructing subjects to keep their head straight-up relative to the cabin.
But even in case the subject’s head is aligned with the cabin, the G-force alignment requires the cabin to rotate,
causing passive Coriolis. A similar situation as Fig. 6a occurs in the conventional roll rotation of the cabin. This cabin
alignment rotation causes a coupling in the two other axes, the magnitude of which depends on the product of the
cabin rotation (in the form of angular position and angular rate) and the centrifuge yaw rotation (in the form of angular
velocity and angular acceleration), Eq. 4.
The passive Coriolis effect can thus be minimized by reducing the centrifuge rotation, the cabin rotation, or both.
Given that the centrifuge yaw rotation rate Ω𝑐 is required to create the G-level required for the simulation, Eqs. 1-2,

6
Ωc

ẏ (pitch)[°/s2] ż (yaw)[°/s2]
Ωc
Start: t=0 Mid: t=.7 tend End: t=tend
z0 k j z0
z
z0 z
z y
yaw i y

yaw
pitch yaw z y0

ẋ (roll)[°/s2]
y0 y0
pitch
y roll
x roll x pitch x
roll 0
y

x rotation [°]
x = 90°

Ωc Ωc Ωc
t0 t.7 tend

(a) Orientation of the subject’ head under simultaneous head and centrifuge (b) Responses of the subject 𝑥, 𝑦 and 𝑧 SCC channels re-
rotation sulting from head rotation in a centrifuge

Figure 6 Illustration of the Coriolis cross-coupling effect in a human centrifuge.

the cabin rotation, its rate and acceleration are the primary means to mitigate passive Coriolis. Then, what would be
the effects of rotating the cabin with a (slightly) different angle than 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 on the perceived subjective vertical? When
subjects would not be able to perceive the G-force alignment perfectly, which seems a reasonable assumption given
the limitations in human self-motion perception, this opens up a possibility to exploit this perceptual inaccuracy, and
manipulate the cabin rotation such to reduce passive Coriolis effects. The next section describes an experiment which
investigated perceptual thresholds in the human perception of G-force alignment.

III. Coherent Alignment Zone: Concept and Evaluation

A. Coherence Zone Concept


The coherence zone indicates the range where visual and vestibular motion, even if not identical, are perceived as
coherent, as studied in [56, 57]. Beyond the limits of the coherence zone, humans perceive the simulated motion to be
incorrect. Results of these studies have been applied in flight and driving simulator motion cueing tuning [57, 58].
Regarding the centrifuge’s G-vector, its magnitude and direction (alignment) can be manipulated in the centrifuge,
and ideally the motion cueing algorithm presents the pilots with a G-vector that is equal to the real one. But if the pilot
would not notice small deviations from this G-vector, in its magnitude and alignment, this allows harmful Coriolis
effects to be mitigated. Gracio et al. [59] studied the just noticeable difference (JND) in the perception of G-load
magnitude and found that this JND is related to the G-load intensity. In the alignment of the centrifuge’s G-vector, it is
likely that small deviations from the true alignment angle, 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 , will also not be perceived by pilots. A region of small
alignment angles around 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 may be defined within which the pilot would still perceive the alignment of the G-vector
to be ‘correct’, or coherent. Hence, it is hypothesized that a ‘Coherent Alignment Zone’ (CAZ) exists, illustrated in
Fig. 7 for the RM cueing condition.
Fig. 7 shows the typical quadratic relationship between the cabin rotation angle 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (horizontal axis) and the
desired G-level (vertical axis), which in turn requires a centrifuge rotation rate Ω𝑐 . A constant 1.4G G-level means that
the cabin needs to be rotated forward with 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 ≈ 44 degrees. The CAZ indicates the set of possible cabin rotation
angles 𝜃 which yield a coherent perception of the magnitude and angle of the G-vector alignment. In this hypothetical
example, rotating the cabin between 𝜃 𝑙𝑜𝑤 𝑒𝑟 ≈ 30 deg and 𝜃 𝑢 𝑝 𝑝𝑒𝑟 ≈ 57 deg would yield the same perceived subjective
vertical by the pilot. An experiment has been conducted to measure this zone, discussed next.

7
Figure 7 Coherent Alignment Zone.

B. Experiment
The objective of the experiment, discussed in detail in Ref. [36], was to establish the body tilt perception thresholds
under elevated G-levels (1-1.4 G) in both pitch (RM cueing) and roll (conventional cueing) axes. Twelve naive subjects
(without piloting experience) were introduced to sub-threshold pitch and roll cabin tilt motion (0.4 deg/s and a maximum
of 20 degrees) in random order and asked to indicate the perceived direction of tilt with respect to the perfectly upright
sitting position. The motion conditions that were tested, were: 1G (no elevated gravity), 1G with central yaw rotation
and no elevated gravity, and 1.4 G in an elevated G-environment.
The hypothesis was that the pitch axis cabin rotation (RM cueing) would result in higher tilt thresholds, because of
differences in the body somatosensory pressure interfaces stimulated with pitch or roll tilt. That is, it was hypothesized
that subjects ‘measured’ roll misalignment using a differential pressure interface (left and right-hand side of the body
along the symmetry axis), providing a more ‘immediate’ comparison between pressure differences. Pitch misalignment
was expected to be measured using absolute (time-dependent) somatosensory cues, where the pressure cues were
sampled from a dissipating pressure region (backside of the head and back), and compared against an absolute reference
point (headrest at start of the simulation) for the duration of the tilting motion. The latter was believed to lead to higher
ambiguity in the pitch axis, making it more difficult to ‘perceive’ the tilt. These are explained in detail in Ref. [36].
20

18

16

14

12

10

0
Roll Pitch

Figure 8 Comparison of group median thresholds for axis, including and excluding ‘errors’, Ref. [36].

8
20

18

16

14

12

10

0
Roll Pitch

Figure 9 Average thresholds in two axes (roll and pitch) for Conditions 1G, 1G yaw and 1.4 G, see Ref. [36].

Experimental results, shown in Fig. 8, support the hypothesis that cabin pitch rotation has a larger ambiguity, leading
to slightly higher thresholds and also larger variations in pitch (average ≈ 5 degrees) than roll (average ≈ 4 degrees). No
differences were found in the direction of the pitch (down, up) or roll (left, right) rotations. The ‘errors’ included in
the figure refer to subject ratings that were erroneous in either the direction (roll or pitch) or sign of the cabin tilt, or
both, see Ref. [36] for details. The (slightly) higher thresholds in pitch suggest that the RM cueing condition allows for
more room to manipulate the cabin alignment angle without the subject inside the cabin noticing it. This corroborates
experience in the Desdemona simulator that the RM cueing is preferred by pilots. In the following, the Rocket Man
cueing will therefore be selected for further study of the COHAM motion cueing development.
Fig. 9 shows the threshold values for the three motion conditions separately. No clear trend can be observed from
this plot as regarding whether the G-level has an effect on the reported threshold. Preliminary experiments at higher
G-levels had to be aborted because of a lack of subjects, and also because the subjects involved quickly experienced
motion discomfort. Although this could indicate the CAZ thresholds to become smaller when G-level increases, more
experiments are needed to find more evidence and to exclude potential confounds.
Concluding the results of the CAZ experiment of Ref. [36]: although a dynamic, i.e., G-level -dependent threshold
may exist, a fixed 5-degree CAZ will be used for the development of the novel cueing method here. We concentrate
on pitch, as there the thresholds were found to be (slightly) larger, and only study the ‘Rocket Man’ condition. The
rationale and operation of the resulting COHAM filter will be discussed in the next section.

IV. Cueing Method: COHAM

A. Coherent Alignment Rationale


The presence of a perceptual coherence zone and the fact that minimizing cabin alignment rotations reduces Coriolis
effects led to the development of the Coherent Alignment Method (COHAM). Its main purpose is to align the simulator
cabin with the least amount of motion possible, without the pilot noticing a mismatch in the G-alignment. COHAM
operates within the maximum allowable mismatch defined by the Coherent Alignment Zone (CAZ).
The principle of COHAM can be explained using Fig. 10, which shows a typical G-onset from 1G to a steady-state
level of 1.4 G (top), and a hypothetical CAZ boundary of fifteen degrees pitch (bottom). The figure illustrates three
possible alignments of the cabin angle: (i) using the nominal rotation (blue) 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 which follows from Eq. (3), (ii)
a leading mechanism (magenta) that starts rotating earlier than the nominal rotation, and (iii) a lagging mechanism
(yellow) that rotates the cabin slightly later than the nominal rotation. The pilot icons (center plot) represent the subject’s
orientation in the cabin for the three cases of rotation. As long as the cabin alignment trajectories lie in-between the
dashed lines of the top plot, showing the hypothesized 15 degree CAZ boundary around 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 , the cabin misalignment
(the absolute value of which is shown in the bottom plot) will be unnoticed by pilots.
Fig. 11 illustrates the response characteristics for these three cabin rotation trajectories (true, leading, lagging),
in terms of the achieved cabin rotation angle (top), rotation rate (center) and rotation acceleration (bottom). The top

9
A1 CAZ lead phase shift A1
amplitude gain A1
A2 CAZ lag phase shift A1
amplitude gain A1 θtrue
θlead

cab
θlag
θthresh

1 2

Coherent Alignment Zone θ1


1 2 θ2
θthresh
mismatch

Figure 10 Illustration of the COHAM for a single cabin alignment in pitch from 1G to a baseline level of 1.4G [60]

60

40

20

0
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

30

20

10

2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Figure 11 Response characteristics of nominal (blue), leading (magenta) and lagging (yellow) cabin rotation pitch motions.

plot shows the CAZ limits using dashed lines. The center and bottom plots also show, respectively, the centrifuge
¤ 𝑐 using dash-dot lines. From these latter plots, it is clear that the cabin rotation rate
rotation rate Ω𝑐 and acceleration Ω
and acceleration are significantly reduced in both the leading and lagging mechanisms. In addition, the onset of the
cabin angular acceleration peaks is shifted to a region with lower centrifuge yaw rate magnitude, further decreasing the
product of two angular rotations, Eq. (4), reducing kinematic cross-couplings. A significant reduction is achieved in
cabin rotation rate and acceleration with respect to the nominal case. Both leading and lagging mechanisms sweep
through the CAZ region, introduce a mismatch, but as long as this mismatch does not exceed the CAZ threshold, the
motion is perceived to be coherent.

10
B. COHAM Building Blocks
The analysis so far was performed while having full knowledge of the to-be-simulated G-onset, and with that
the cabin rotation angle. For any practical application of these findings in an on-line motion filter the cabin must be
coordinated within the CAZ from a baseline G-level (typically for Desdemona 1.4 G, corresponding with ≈ 44.5 degrees
of cabin rotation) to any G-level initiated by pilot control actions, which are unknown beforehand. The main challenge
therefore is to predict the required simulator G-level and dynamically adapt the cabin rotation to always remain within
the CAZ.
To address this challenge, COHAM has three components. First, COHAM applies a lead-lag filter, the Two-Tailed
Peak Flattening (TTPF). Second, it includes an algorithm to predict the G-onset (and with that the required centrifuge
rotation rate) from pilot control actions. Third, it applies a Dynamic Lookup Table method [61] to guarantee that the
mismatch always remains within the CAZ limits. We briefly discuss these components in the following.

1. Lead/lag filter: the TTPF


Leading or lagging mechanisms applied to the cabin rotation can reduce the angular velocity and acceleration
associated with cabin alignment. One could argue that both mechanisms characterize a ‘one-tailed’ use of the allowed
mismatch. For example, using the lead mechanism the angular velocity and acceleration are increased up to the G
response peak approaching from the left (positive mismatch), whereas for the lag mechanism this goes from the peak
towards the steady-state value (negative mismatch), see the bottom plot of Fig. 10. Both mechanisms feature a reversal
of mismatch while traversing through the CAZ, and act mainly in one half of the onset curve, hence, one-tailed.

Lead/Lag Mechanism Two-Tailed Peak


Flattening Mechanism
G-level

G-level

2G 2G

1.4G 1.4G

t t
Pitch angle

Pitch angle

60o 60o
tA tB tA tB

44.4o 44.4o
t t
CAZ of the lead CAZ of the TTPF
mechanism mechanism
Mismatch
Mismatch

+U +U
tA tB tA tB
0 0

-U -U
t t

Figure 12 Comparison of lead or lag mechanism (left) versus the Two-Tailed Peak Flattening Mechanism (right) for a
hypothetical mismatch ±𝑈 in cabin pitch alignment.

To fully exploit the CAZ boundaries, however, a combination of both lead and lag mechanisms would be desired, as
this could result in ‘flattening’ and ‘spreading’ out of the response over the two tails of the peak. Ideally, this would
allow for a twice as wide spread over time and a further reduction of cabin alignment angular velocity and acceleration.
This concept is referred to as Two-Tailed Peak Flattening (TTPF), and can be characterized as follows (see Fig. 12):
• the TTPF rotation leads the true cabin rotation prior to the onset peak 𝑡 < 𝑡 𝐴 in Fig. 12;
• the TTPF rotation has a lower magnitude than the true rotation (𝑡 𝐴 < 𝑡 < 𝑡 𝐵 ); and
• the TTPF rotation lags the true cabin rotation after the onset peak 𝑡 > 𝑡 𝐵 .
The second characteristic of the TTPF, that the cabin angle rotation can be lower than the true rotation at the peak of the
G simulation, alludes to another opportunity of using the CAZ, by having the cabin not return to the nominal rotation
after every onset, but actually return to a higher angle. Again, as long as the cabin alignment mismatch remains within

11
the CAZ, this will not be perceived by pilots. The TTPF filter therefore applies a certain offset from the baseline value
(here: 5 degrees), allowing the controller to lead and transition to lag over a longer time span, and rotate the cabin even
less. The result is a lazy but punctual control mechanism, which is in phase with the predicted onset peak but always
operates within the specified CAZ limits.
75

70

65

60

55

50

45

40
280 300 320 340 360 380 400

0
280 300 320 340 360 380 400

Figure 13 Time traces of nominal and COHAM cabin alignment (top), and the mismatch between the two (bottom).

The result is illustrated in Fig. 13, which shows the cabin rotation angle (top) and mismatch (bottom) for a recorded
sequence of G-pulls in a Desdemona test run. At the baseline level of 1.4G, the cabin is tilted 5 degrees more than
required, in anticipation of a pilot pulling larger G-forces than 1.4G. Once the pilot pulls a G-force, the cabin pitches
forward to align the pilot onto the 𝐺 𝑧 vector, but at the maximum G-level ideally remains 5 degrees short of the true
alignment angle. After the G-pull, the simulator cabin returns to a tilt angle that is 5 degrees more than nominal. The
mismatch is always smaller than the 5 degrees limit of the CAZ. In this way, the smallest pitch rotation is required to
align the cabin without the pilot noticing the difference.

2. Predicting the G-onset


The ability to predict the G-load pulled by the pilot is crucial for the COHAM filter. Whereas adding 5 degrees of
cabin pitch to the baseline 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 is trivial, arriving 5 degrees short at the maximum of the G-maneuver is not. This
requires prediction of the G-load exerted on the aircraft model, and consequently a leading response to the instantaneous
simulator G-level, 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 .
COHAM uses the pilot stick input as a means to predict the G-load resulting from this input. This stems from the
fact that any aircraft response can be characterized as ‘low-pass filtering’ the pilot input, causing a lag. In the F-16
fighter aircraft, the stick input controls normal accelerations (𝑁 𝑥 , 𝑁 𝑦 , 𝑁 𝑧 ) in all flight conditions apart from landing.
The stick input therefore always leads the 𝑁 𝑧 , and therefore the requested G-load.
Fig. 14 illustrates the COHAM prediction mechanism. Starting in the top-left, the pilot exerts forces on the stick,
leading to stick inputs in roll and pitch which enter the F-16 dynamics (block F-16 Plant). COHAM uses the pitch
input 𝑢 𝑝𝑖𝑡 𝑐ℎ to directly compute 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 (block “Calc G-lead”), whereas the simulator software computes the actual F-16
G force using the model (block “Calc Gact”). This latter block also includes a G-onset timing mechanism based on
the maximum G-value that ensures realistic onset even at reduced G-levels, given that the Desdemona simulator has a
maximum of 3G and the F-16 model a maximum of 9G. That is, the 𝑁 𝑧 from the aircraft model (1-9G), is mapped
to an instantaneous simulator G-level 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 (1-3G) within the simulator performance limits [48]. The conventional
cueing, illustrated in the top-right, then uses 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 to compute the true cabin angle rotation 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (block “Calc True
Coordination”).
COHAM, on the other hand, as illustrated in the bottom left of Fig. 14, takes the maximum of two signals, the actual

12
G-level of the F-16 model 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 and the leading G-level 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 obtained from the pilot input, and a baseline level of
1.8G. Typical lead times observed in 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 vs. 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 are in the order of 1 second. Hence, the maximum G-level will be
taken at any time based on the F-16 simulation and pilot inputs, but not lower than 1.8G. This latter minimum will
take care of the fact that in case of no or small maneuvers, the cabin rotates back to a higher – but not perceived to
be higher – pitch angle than would correspond to the baseline 1.4G. The resulting output signal 𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓 is low-pass
filtered (second order filter, damping 1 and natural frequency 1 rad/s), to obtain the maximum required G-level at that
instance, 𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥 , which will enter the “Dynamic Lookup Table” block (bottom right), discussed next.
Fig. 15 illustrates the same maneuver as Fig. 13. The middle figure shows the signal 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 (yellow) clearly leading
the actual G-level 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 (blue) used in the conventional cueing. The effect of taking the maximum of these two signals
and then prohibiting the resulting signal to get below 1.8 G, as well as the relatively high frequency content in 𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓
(magenta) are clear from Fig. 15. Filtering this signal and applying it to the dynamic look-up table (described below)
eventually results in the cabin rotation 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 shown in the top figure.

Calc True
𝛿𝑟 𝑜𝑙𝑙 𝑢𝑟 𝑜𝑙𝑙 Coordination
states 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 (𝑡)
Pilot 𝛿 𝑝𝑖𝑡 𝑐ℎ Stick 𝑢 𝑝𝑖𝑡 𝑐ℎ F16-Plant Calc 𝑮 𝒂𝒄𝒕

𝑢 𝑝𝑖𝑡 𝑐ℎ
Calc 𝑮 𝒍𝒆𝒂𝒅
𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (𝑡)
𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 (𝑡) 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 (𝑡) 𝜽 𝒕𝒓𝒖𝒆 + 𝑼𝑪𝑶𝑯 𝑨𝑴

1.8𝑮 𝒎𝒂𝒙{𝒖 1 , 𝒖 2 , 𝒖 3 } 𝑈𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 (𝑡)


𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 (𝑡)

𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓 (𝑡) Dynamic Lookup


𝑋¯ 𝑑𝑎𝑡 table
𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥 (𝑡) 𝑿¯ 𝒅𝒂𝒕 =
LP2
[1.4𝑮 𝑮 𝒎𝒂𝒙 (𝒕)]
𝑌¯𝑑𝑎𝑡
𝒀¯𝒅𝒂𝒕 = [5◦ − 5◦ ]

Figure 14 Schematics of the pitch mismatch coordination by COHAM, illustrating the G-onset prediction and use of a
Dynamic Lookup Table.

80

70

60

50

40
280 300 320 340 360 380 400

2.5

1.5

280 300 320 340 360 380 400

2.5

1.5

280 300 320 340 360 380 400

Figure 15 Time traces of G-coordination for the test run with the COHAM using the predicted signal.

13
There are two additional noteworthy comments on the filtering. First, because of the substantial lead, 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 arrives
at the estimated maximum G-load (approximately 1 second) earlier than the actual response of the aircraft 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 . It is
used to predict what the actual maximum G-level will be at any time to maintain 5 degrees less cabin pitch after passing
𝑡 = 𝑡 𝐴 in Fig. 12. Second, given that the F-16 has a (velocity-dependent) approximately proportional relationship
between pilot pitch inputs and 𝑁 𝑧 , the 𝐺 𝑙𝑒𝑎𝑑 signal shape roughly corresponds to the pilot pitch stick input, explaining
the relatively high frequency content and the need to filter 𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓 .

3. Dynamic Lookup Table


To guarantee a smooth transition from “5 degrees additional pitch at baseline” to “5 degrees less at max. G”, a
Dynamic Lookup Table is used, illustrated in the bottom right of Fig. 14. This function block first creates a dynamic,
required G-level interval, from a fixed 1.4G lower bound to a maximum 𝐺˜ 𝑚𝑎𝑥 , i.e., [1.4G 𝐺˜ 𝑚𝑎𝑥 ] ( 𝑋¯ 𝑑𝑎𝑡 in Fig. 14), and
maps it to the static minimum and maximum cabin angle rotation CAZ thresholds [+5 -5] degrees (𝑌¯𝑑𝑎𝑡 in Fig. 14). It
then uses the actual aircraft G-level 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 to compute the actual cabin pitch angle deviation 𝑈𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 to be added to the
true cabin pitch angle 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 from the conventional simulator cueing. In other words, the dynamic look-up table adapts
the required cabin rotation 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 such that the discrepancy never deviates outside the CAZ, while allowing the COHAM
TTPF and predictive blocks to work as designed, i.e., be lazy but punctual.
The inputs to the Dynamic Lookup Table are illustrated in the bottom figure of Fig. 15. The unfiltered 𝐺 𝑚𝑎𝑥𝑢𝑛 𝑓
signal (red) is filtered by the second order low-pass filter (LP2) yielding the yellow signal 𝐺˜ 𝑚𝑎𝑥 which is used as the
maximum value of the 𝑋¯ 𝑑𝑎𝑡 vector, 𝑋𝑚𝑎𝑥 . The minimum value 𝑋𝑚𝑖𝑛 is set at 1.4 G. Based on the actual G-level, 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 ,
the look-up table dynamically assigns an allowable cabin angle rotation discrepancy ranging between +5 degrees (𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡
lower than 1.4G, so tilting too much forward) and −5 degrees (𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 higher than the maximum predicted G-level at that
time, so tilting less than needed).

C. Recapitulation and Main Results


Summarizing, the essence of the COHAM dynamic mapping mechanism lies in the following:
1. Since the saturation value of 1.8G is higher than the baseline 1.4G, for low values of 𝐺 𝑎𝑐𝑡 the cabin mismatch
will always be 5 degrees above the baseline as shown in the top subplot in Fig. 15. The result is a lazy pitch
coordination that always leads prior to a G-onset: 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 >𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (𝑡) before 𝑡 𝐴.
2. Since the maximum G-level is estimated ahead of time, the mismatch flips to a negative mismatch as the G-level
rises. The result is a lower coordination onset at the peak: 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 <𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (𝑡) between 𝑡 𝐴 and 𝑡 𝐵 .
3. When the G-level drops back to the baseline, the dynamic input value 𝐺˜ 𝑚𝑎𝑥 (the last element of the 𝑋¯ 𝑑𝑎𝑡 vector)
drops back to 1.8 G, ensuring the cabin moves back to the baseline mismatch: 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 >𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (𝑡) after 𝑡 𝐵 .

20

10

-10

-20
280 300 320 340 360 380 400

Figure 16 Comparison of true cabin pitch rate and COHAM cabin pitch rate for the same recorded test run.

Fig. 16, corresponding to the time traces of Fig. 13, shows the cabin rotation rates with or without the COHAM
filter. With COHAM, the magnitude at the peaks is reduced up to a factor of two, significantly reducing the Coriolis
effect, without the pilot noticing. The COHAM cueing is evaluated experimentally and discussed next.

14
V. COHAM Evaluation: Method

A. Goal and Description


A small experiment was performed which compared the Desdemona “Rocket Man” motion cueing (RM) to COHAM,
in terms of the experienced Coriolis effect, comfort and false cues. The main goal was to evaluate whether COHAM can
mitigate Coriolis effects, while still providing a realistic G-cueing experience in high-G piloting tasks. Constraints on
the available number of fighter pilots (three) and simulator hours (one week) were severe, so the experiment is a first
qualitative evaluation, no statistical analyses were performed; results should be interpreted with caution.
The evaluation lasted four days. During the first three days, pilots were trained to fly an F-16 along a prescribed
trajectory with a given airspeed, resulting in a sequence of high G-pulls. Two simulators were used for training,
Desdemona and the Dome, both facilitated by the Desdemona B.V. [49]. Training was important for pilots to produce
comparable G-levels and allow for a fair comparison between RM and COHAM during the fourth evaluation day.

B. METHOD

1. Apparatus

(a) Desdemona (b) Dome

Figure 17 Apparatus used in the evaluation (based in Soesterberg, The Netherlands).

The apparatus used in the experiment were the 6-degrees of freedom, centrifuge-based simulator Desdemona [49],
and the fixed-based F-16 BARCO Dome simulator [62], see Fig. 17. Both simulators were equipped with an F-16
cockpit configuration with nearly identical controls, but a more realistic F-16 cockpit interior for the Dome. The Dome
features a 240 x 270 degrees visual Field of View (FOV); Desdemona has a 120 x 30 degrees FOV.
The same simulation environment (Grand Canyon scenery) and trajectory were used in both simulators.

2. Piloting Task
The piloting task was to follow a trajectory of approximately 52 miles (≈ 84𝑘𝑚) over and along the Grand Canyon,
see Fig. 18. A run, or ‘round’ in the following, started with taking-off from Grand Canyon West Airport runway 35,
climbing to 3,000 ft, after which pilots entered the Grand Canyon (Dive-In) at entry point (A) and followed its contours
at 100 feet AGL until they left the canyon (Climb-Out) at exit point (B). The round was considered completed when the
aircraft returned safely to the airport waypoint, flying at 3,000 ft again. For every subsequent round, subjects were asked
to proceed with the trajectory (no landing), fly comfortably to the entry point (A) and re-enter the valley for another
round. Fig. 19 illustrates the pilot outside view at three positions during a round.
The trajectory consisted of a high-G phase with a sequence of high pulls (indicated by the red circles in Fig. 18) and
a low-G cruise phase (blue circles) allowing subjects sufficient time to recover from the high-G strain in-between rounds.
The G-peaks of the high-G phase of the trajectory varied between 3 G to 5 G with two extremes at 7 G.

15
The target trajectory was shown on the outside visual as a three-dimensional contour built up by red, green and blue
lines, corresponding to the traces of the aircraft left wing tip, right wing tip and body axis, respectively. The red and
green lines served to control the required amount of bank, thus the amount of G-force pulled at the given airspeed, see
Fig. 19. Airspeed was kept constant by subjects at approximately 400 kt, and they were trained to accurately reproduce
the desired pattern of G-pulls during the high G-phase.

Figure 18 Map of the Grand Canyon and the trajectory flown.

3. Independent Variable
The independent variable was the Desdemona centrifuge simulator motion cueing condition, with two levels: Rocket
Man and COHAM. In the following these are referred to as C1 and C2, respectively.

4. Control Variables
All runs featured the same trajectory. The COHAM filter was set to accommodate a maximum of 5 degrees mismatch
in cabin pitch rotation. The cockpit was kept in constant F-16 configuration and 30-degree seat inclination (from the
vertical). Subjects were outfitted with the same equipment: headphones with two-way communication. Lastly, the
briefing, questionnaire, experimental procedure and protocol were the same for all subjects.

5. Participants and Instructions


The three participants (all males, aged between 22 and 28 years) were Dutch air-force freshmen, in the midst of
completing their elementary military pilot training. All had piloting experience with light trainer aircraft (PC-7) and
limited (<2 hours) centrifuge experience (conventional centrifuge and Desdemona).
Prior to and during the training phase (days N1-N3), subjects were told that they participated in a study of long term
G-training; they were not made aware of the existence of two motion filters. On the evaluation day (N4), subjects were
told that two motion filters were being compared and got familiarized with the experiment questionnaire. They were
given no specific details about the type of motion filter, nor were they aware which of the two was active at any given
time. Each subject performed an assessment and two comparisons (referred to as R1 and R2) of the two motion filters,
using a set of criteria including comfort, dizziness, sickness and G-alignment, all discussed below.

6. Experiment Set-up and Procedure


Experiment Set-up Table 4 illustrates the training and evaluation schedule for the three subjects S1-S3. In
this table, labels D and C refer to simulation in the Dome and Desdemona, respectively, with the number of runs in
parentheses. For the Desdemona runs, ‘1’ and ‘2’ refer to the RM and COHAM motion cueing, respectively. In each
row, a vertical bar indicates the transition from the Dome to the Desdemona simulator. The first day of the training
phase (N1) consisted of five rounds in the Dome and one round in the Desdemona simulator with Rocket Man cueing C1.
This was done to get subjects accustomed to the controls and the trajectory. Days 2 and 3 were used to also familiarize

16
(a) S1 just before Dive-In (A). (b) S2 mid-trajectory. (c) S3 just before Climb-Out (B).

Figure 19 Outside view of F-16 and cockpit camera, showing subjects flying the F-16 through the Grand Canyon.

the pilots to the COHAM motion and train their trajectory-following skills.

Table 4 Training and evaluation schedule.

Subject 1 (S1) Subject 2 (S2) Subject 3 (S3)


Training days
Day 1 (N1) D(×5) | C1 D(×5) | C1 D(×5) | C1
Day 2 (N2) D(×1) | C12121 D(×1) | C21212 D(×1) | C12121
Day 3 (N3) D(×1) | C12121 D(×1) | C21212 D(×1) | C12121
Evaluation day

Day 4 (N4) D(×1) | C12 , C21 D(×1) | C21 , C12 D(×1) | C12 , C21
|{z} |{z} |{z} |{z} |{z} |{z}
R1 R2 R1 R2 R1 R2

Procedure In the final evaluation session on day 4, the R1 and R2 refer to the two evaluations each subject
performed of the two motion conditions in Desdemona, RM and COHAM, applied in a different order. The evaluation
was done by means of a questionnaire. To reduce simulator time and exposure to motion, the questionnaire was evaluated
over the intercom, during the time subjects flew from the canyon exit (point B) back to the canyon entry (A).
During the whole experiment, subjects were asked to report their well-being scores, using the MIsery SCale (MISC)
score (0-10). MISC is developed by Wertheim et al. [63–65], see Table 5. Subjects provided their MISC score twice
per round: just before the initial dive into the canyon (point A) and right after the climb-out (point B). Any training or
evaluation session would be aborted whenever a MISC score of 5 or higher was given.

7. Questionnaire
The questionnaire was completed twice (R1 and R2) after flying two rounds, with the motion cueing filters RM
and COHAM, in different orders, see Table 4. It consisted of two types of assessment: the subject well-being, and
the experienced G-alignment. All assessments must be regarded as a ‘weighted’ score, as they are all based on the
‘accumulated experience over time’ when flying with both filters. Further, note that for all scores, low is better.
Well-being Scores and Preference Assessment Subjects were asked to rate both motion filters using two metrics.
First, in the Accumulated Well-being Score (AWS), Table 6, subjects assessed the experienced motion sickness,
dizziness and bearability, using a scale similar to the Griffin and Newman Scale [66].
Second, subjects were asked to indicate which of the two cueing settings they preferred in the Preference-Based
Well-being Score (PBWS), using the same three categories as in the AWS (sickness, dizziness, bearability) extended
with a fourth category, comfort. In the PBWS, subjects simply had to choose which of the two cueing settings they
preferred, or neither. Because subjects did not know what cueing setting was active in what round, they indicated it
using ‘first filter’, or ‘second filter’, in the questionnaire. These were then coupled to C1 or C2 by the experimenter.
G-alignment coherency metrics The effects of the motion filter on the perceived G-alignment were assessed
using two metrics. First, in the Alignment Mismatch Score (AMS), subjects were asked to indicate whether during
G-onsets they experienced any mismatch in G-alignment relative to the true G-vector. AMS is expressed in mismatch

17
Table 5 MIsery SCale, the MISC score.

Symptom Degree Score


No problems 0
Uneasiness (no typical symptoms) 1

Vague 2
Dizziness, warmth, headache, Slight 3
stomach awareness, sweating Fairly 4
Severe 5

Slight 6
Fairly 7
Nausea Severe 8
Retching 9
Vomiting 10

Table 6 Accumulated Well-being Scale (AWS).

Definition Score
no symptoms experienced 0
slight symptoms experienced (not always recognizable) 1
mild effect experienced (recognizable) 2
moderate to mildly experienced 3
moderate but was able to continue 4
unbearable wanted to stop 5

direction (sideways left, sideways right, forward and backward) and (relative) magnitude (range of angular mismatch in
degrees: 0𝑜 , 0 − 5𝑜 , 5 − 10𝑜 , > 10𝑜 ).
Second, the Accumulated Level of Alignment Coherency of the G-cueing (ALAC) was defined for the entire
centrifuge session, see Table 7. ALAC has a numeric scale ranging from 0 to 5, and represents the accumulated level of
alignment coherency for a given motion condition; here 0 stands for motion cueing that is absolutely coherent throughout
the session (G-vector is ‘spot-on’) and 5 for cueing that is not coherent (G-vector always too far titled).

Table 7 Accumulated Level of Alignment Coherency of the G-cueing (ALAC).

Level of Coherency Alignment G-vector ability to Score


detect deviation
absolutely coherent G-vector spot on impossible 0
almost always coherent G-vector almost always spot on very hard 1
fairly coherent G-vector is true most of the time sometimes too far tilted hard 2
moderately coherent G-vector is true most of the time sometimes too far tilted moderate 3
fairly incoherent G-vector is not true most of the time easy 4
not coherent G-vector always too far tilted very easy 5

Overall Preference Verdict As a final metric, at the end of each comparison subjects were asked their overall
preference (first filter, second filter, no preference). These were labeled C1 or C2 by the experimenter.

8. Dependent Measures
A number of dependent measures were obtained throughout the whole experiment: the MISC scores introduced
above, the F-16 model G-levels, the simulator G-levels, and the cabin rotations and rotation rates. The F-16 model

18
G-levels higher than 2 G (G-peaks) were processed to obtain their probability distributions.
All other dependent measures were obtained only in the evaluation phase (N4) in the four rounds flown in Desdemona,
using the questionnaire. These were the AWS, PWBS, AMS and ALAC scores defined above, and the overall preference.

C. Hypotheses
The first hypothesis is that the COHAM cueing is preferred by our subjects. The lower cabin G-alignment rotations
and rotation rates with COHAM mitigate the Coriolis cross-couplings, yielding lower MISC scores, lower AWS scores,
higher PBWS scores and an overall preference for COHAM. The second hypothesis is that COHAM will be perceived
as coherent within 5 degrees of CAZ, as indicated by low AMS and ALAC scores.
The third hypothesis is that the considerable time for training (three days) will lead to comparable G-levels in the
Dome and Desdemona simulators, despite the high G-loads in Desdemona. The latter is because earlier experience with
training pilots on Desdemona has shown that pilots learn to cope with the G-environment quickly. Our fourth hypothesis
is that this will also be the case in this experiment, that the prolonged exposure to elevated G-levels will increase pilot
endurance. It means that we expect the well-being ratings to decrease during the experimental days, perhaps only to
increase again when many runs in Desdemona are done on the same day.

VI. COHAM Evaluation: Results


Results of the first evaluation of COHAM are presented, using the subjective and objective measures defined above.

A. Subjective Measurements

1. MISC Scores
Table 8 lists all MISC scores given during the Desdemona sessions conducted in the last three days (N2-N4) of the
experiment. Recall that the MISC scores were collected twice for each round, right after the ‘Dive-In’ maneuver at the
canyon entry point (A), and right after the ‘Climb-Out’ maneuver at the canyon exit point (B).

Table 8 All MISC scores given (note: ★ means that no rating was given).

N2 N3 N4
C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2
S1 A 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 1
B 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 1
S2 A 1 6 ★ ★ ★ 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 1
B 3 5 ★ ★ ★ 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 2 1
S3 A 2 3 4 5 ★ 1 2 2 2 2 1 2 2 3
B 2 3 4 4 ★ 1 2 2 3 2 1 2 2 3
R1
R2

Several trends can be read from this table. First, the MISC scores are largest in the second day (N2) and become
much smaller at the last day (N4). On the second day, a few runs had to be aborted for subjects S2 and S3 because of
increasing motion sickness. Second, the MISC scores are generally a little higher after the exit point (B), which can
be expected as between (A) and (B) the canyon was followed requiring several high G maneuvers. Third, in the two
comparisons (R1 and R2) Subjects 1 and 2 reported the lowest MISC score for almost all rounds, whereas Subject S3
reported slightly lower MISC scores for the RM cueing in R1, then higher MISC scores for RM cueing in R2.
Adding all scores (all subjects, days, points (A) and (B)) yields totals of 72 and 62 for RM (38 rounds) and COHAM
(36 rounds), respectively. Adding all scores for the final day (N4) yields 17 and 16 for RM (12 rounds) and COHAM
(12 rounds), respectively, (R1: 6 for RM, 8 for COHAM; R2: 11 for RM, 8 for COHAM). Note that adding MISC
scores is a questionable step and merely yields a qualitative, first indication.
These MISC scores are evidence that subjects became accustomed with the centrifuge-based G cueing; a small
difference emerged in favor for the COHAM cueing.

19
2. Accumulated Well-being Score (AWS)
The AWS scores for metrics sickness, dizziness and bearability are summarized in Table 9. Recall that these scores
were given by pilots once for each session (R1, R2) flown with both two motion conditions (C1 and C2).

Table 9 Accumulated Well-being Scores (AWS).

R1 R2 “best”
subject C1 C2 C1 C2 R1 R2
sickness S1 1 1 1 0 - C2
S2 1 1 1 2 - C1
S3 1 2 3 2 C1 C2
dizziness S1 0 0 1 0 - C2
S2 1 1 1 2 - C1
S3 1 1 3 2 - C2
bearability S1 1 1 2 1 - C2
S2 2 3 1 2 C1 C1
S3 2 2 3 2 - C2
10 12 16 13 C1 C2
22 29

Each row in the table shows the score given for C1 and C2 in R1, C1 and C2 in R2, and based on a comparison of
these scores the “best” cueing was distilled by the authors for each of the two comparisons R1 and R2 (last two columns).
Note that subjects were also asked explicitly for their preference; these scores are discussed in the next subsection.
For all scores, it can be seen that in the first comparison R1, pilots gave quite similar ratings, with perhaps a slight
preference for C1 (added score 10 relative to 12 for C2). In the second comparison, R2, the scores were slightly higher
(which means worse, 29 in total for R2 versus 22 for R1), especially for dizziness, and two of the three pilots gave lower
(=better) ratings for motion condition C2 (added score 13 relative to 16 for C1). Note that adding AWS scores is a
questionable step and merely yields a qualitative, first indication.
The lower scores for COHAM in the last session R2 is evidence that pilots can longer sustain the G training, which
is supported by verbal comments (not shown). However, the number of pilots in this exploratory experiment is too low
to draw definite conclusions.

3. Preference-Based Well-being Scores (PBWS)

Table 10 Preference-Based Well-being Scores (PBWS).

R1 R2
C1 C2 neither C1 C2 neither
sickness 3 1 1 1
dizziness 1 1 1 1 2
bearability 3 1 2
comfort 2 1 1 2
9 2 1 4 7 1

The PBWS scores for metrics sickness, dizziness, bearability and comfort are summarized in Table 10. The PBWS
scores show a similar trend as the AWS ratings. In the first comparison the RM cueing (C1) is preferred in 9 out of
12 possible preferences, but in the second comparison COHAM cueing (C2) is preferred in 7 out of 12. This is some
evidence that whereas the default Desdemona RM tuning worked quite well when experienced for a shorter time, the
COHAM cueing was more bearable and could be dealt with by our subjects for a longer time.
In their verbal and written comments, subjects reported that their comments and ratings on sickness and dizziness
were mainly based on their experiences during high G-onsets, that is in all maneuvers that required G(un)-loading. The

20
Dive-In (canyon entry, A) and Climb-Out (canyon exit, B) maneuvers were of less importance for their ratings. No
differences were reported in this respect between the two motion filters.

4. G-Alignment Scores
Assessment of the G-alignment, using ALAC and AMS, is shown in Table 11 and Table 12, respectively.

Table 11 Accumulated G-ALignment Coherency Scores (ALAC).

R1 R2 “best”
subject C1 C2 C1 C2 R1 R2
ALAC S1 0 0 0 0 - -
S2 1 1 1 3 - C1
S3 3 4 3 4 C1 C1
4 5 4 7

Table 12 G-Alignment Mismatch Scores (AMS).

Body Tilt
subject evaluation experienced? Forward Backward Left Right
S1 R1 - - - - -
R2 C1 - - - 0 − 5𝑜
S2 R1 C1 & C2 0 − 5𝑜 0 − 5𝑜 - -
R2 C2 0 − 5𝑜 0 − 5𝑜 - -
S3 R1 C2 - 5 − 10𝑜 - -
R2 C1 & C2 0 − 5𝑜 0 − 5𝑜 - -

The ALAC scores table has the same structure as the AWS scores. It can be seen that, apart from Subject 1 who
judged the G-alignment in both motion conditions to be ‘spot-on’, Subjects 2 and 3 experienced some issues with C1
(total score 8) but especially C2 (total score 12). Adding ALAC scores is a questionable step and merely yields a
qualitative, first indication. Nonetheless, COHAM was not always perceived to be coherent in terms of the G-vector
alignment. Subject 3 rated the RM cueing to be ‘too far tilted’ (ALAC level 3) while the G-alignment was in fact correct
all the time. He rated the COHAM cueing to be ‘not true most of the time’ (ALAC level 4). This indicates the difficulty
to assess ALAC well.
The lower coherence with COHAM is corroborated by the AMS scores, summarized in Table 12. Subject 1 was the
only one to report some slight (0-5 degrees) mismatch to his right, with C1. Subjects 2 and 3 reported slight forward and
backward misalignments in C1, but especially in C2, with a 5-10𝑜 backward misalignment for C2 as maximum score.

5. Overall preference
All subjects reported the necessity of the second session (R2) for their definitive verdict. All subjects had an initial
slight preference for the Rocket Man cueing, in R1. Subjects 1 and 3 switched their preference to the COHAM cueing in
R2, mainly because they found this cueing to cause less sickness and dizziness and also being smoother at G-onsets
during sharp turns. Recall that our subjects did not know what cueing was given in what round. The trend in the
overall preference verdict corroborates the findings discussed in the AWS and PBWS scores. Apparently, the more often
perceived misalignment of the G-vector with COHAM (in ALAC and AMS) did not lead subjects to prefer the RM
cueing. Tentatively, they accepted the misalignments because the overall G environment was more bearable.

6. Verbal comments
Subject S2 reported a higher G-load experienced during a straight and level flight (1.4 G baseline) for the sessions
with COHAM. Apparently, he was able to sense the offset in the cabin angle 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 introduced by COHAM, discussed
in Section IV.B.1, and he interpreted the higher cabin angle rotation (≈ 49 versus 44 degrees) as a higher G-load,
whereas the G-load was in fact correct. Clearly, for this subject, the CAZ threshold of 5 degrees was too large, and

21
the perceived ‘additional tilt’ relative to the RM cueing was interpreted as ‘higher G’, which makes sense as when
simulating higher G-levels the centrifuge cabin needs to be rotated more inwards to properly align the gravity vector.

B. Objective Measurements

1. F-16 Model and Simulator G-levels


The G-levels of the F-16 model were collected in both the stationary Dome and the centrifuge Desdemona simulators
across all training (N1–N3) and evaluation (N4) days. To investigate potential differences between the simulators, these
G-levels were processed first. Because the subjects spent most of their time at the baseline G-level, we focused our
analysis on their ‘willingness’ to venture into higher G-regions. That is, all data below 2 G were discarded, and we
analyzed the resulting ‘G-peak’ data using scatter-histograms, illustrated in Fig. 20.

(a) Subject S1, days N1-N4 (b) Subject S2, days N1-N4

(c) Subject S3, days N1-N4 (d) All subjects, all days

Figure 20 Comparison Dome versus Desdemona with Scatter-histograms of F-16 model G-peaks.

These G-peaks reflect the high G-levels pulled during sharp turns, the Dive-In and Climb-Out instances. Fig. 20
shows a comparison of the G-peaks measured in the Dome (horizontal-axis) versus the Desdemona (vertical-axis) per
subject. Whereas Figures 20a-20c show the distributions for the three subjects S1-S3 averaged per experimental day
(N1-N4), Fig. 20d shows the data averaged over all days for the three subjects.†
Examining the distributions in Fig. 20d, and the boxplots of the data obtained on the evaluation day N4, Fig. 21, the
differences between the Dome and Desdemona simulators are small, suggesting that the G-profiles flown by our subjects
† The numbers next to the legends indicate the number of data points (Dome, Desdemona) for that day, e.g. N1-(17,1.7)·e+5 in the top left.

22
were indeed comparable. The high actual G-levels in the centrifuge did not lead the three pilots to reduce the intensity
of their maneuvering. For both simulators the distributions are skewed, their modes occur around 3 - 3.5 G; recall that
these are the G-levels of the simulated F-16 model, not the actual G-levels in Desdemona.

Figure 21 Boxplots of F-16 model G-peaks (Dome versus Desdemona, all subjects, day N4).

The actual G-levels in Desdemona were also recorded. Fig. 22 illustrates these G-levels for the RM and COHAM
filters for all three subjects. Unfortunately, the data for Subject S1 in the first round (RM) were lost. These boxplots
show that the G-levels were comparable in both cueing settings.

missing data
Rocket Man
COHAM
outliers

Figure 22 Boxplots of simulator G-peaks in Desdemona (RM versus COHAM, all subjects, day N4).

2. Cabin Rotations
Fig. 23 shows time histories of the true cabin rotation angle 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 and 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 for subject S2, in the order C2
C1 C1 and C2, see Table 4. Similar to the cabin rotation, also the rotation rates 𝜔 (not shown, but see Fig. 16) are
smaller with COHAM. Fig. 24 shows boxplots of the cabin rotation (left) and rotation rates (right) for all three subjects,
measured on the evaluation day (N4) for RM and COHAM. These clearly show that COHAM considerably reduces the
cabin rotations, which in turn (see Eq. (4)) reduces the Coriolis effects (not shown). This reduction indeed yielded lower
sickness and dizziness in our subjects, especially when time progressed, however, the extent of mismatch coordination
needs to be further investigated as two subjects reported issues related to the G-misalignment.

VII. Discussion
A G-cueing filter was developed, the Coherent Alignment method (COHAM), to reduce Coriolis effects in human
centrifuges. A four-day evaluation campaign with three fighter pilots was conducted to test its effectiveness. Pilots

23
80

70

60

50

40
0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000 3500

-5
0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000 3500

Figure 23 Cabin coordination angles 𝜃 𝐶𝑂𝐻 𝐴𝑀 versus 𝜃 𝑡𝑟 𝑢𝑒 (Subject S2, day N4) (top), and the mismatch (bottom), when
the COHAM filter was active, or not.

Rocket Man Rocket Man


COHAM COHAM

(a) Rotation (b) Rotation rate

Figure 24 Boxplots of cabin rotation and rotation rate.

performed a high G-maneuver F-16 control task in the centrifuge-based motion simulator Desdemona, preceded by a
training phase in the fixed-based Dome simulator. In the following, we will reflect on the experimental results using the
four hypotheses stated in Section V.C and then discuss some recommendations for future work.

A. Reflection on Experiment and Hypotheses


The experiment, albeit performed with only three subjects, provided some fair evidence for supporting the first
hypothesis. The lower rotations and rotation rates with COHAM led our subjects to ultimately prefer the COHAM
cueing, while achieving comparable G-levels as with the conventional cueing. Although our subjects quickly became
accustomed to the high G-levels, as witnessed by the steep decline of the MISC ratings over the three days, these ratings
were smaller for COHAM until the last runs, comparison R2. The AWS, PBWS and Overall Preference scores were
all in favor of COHAM after R2. The AWS scores illustrate that sickness and especially dizziness increase less with
COHAM as compared to the conventional RM cueing. Overall, COHAM was considered more ‘bearable’.
Our second hypothesis, that COHAM is coherent, is rejected. The G-alignment scores (ALAC, AMS) all show that
COHAM performed worse than RM, and two of our subjects reported the cabin misalignments with COHAM. This lack
of coherence was interpreted differently, however, as the slightly higher cabin rotation led one of our subjects to believe
that the simulator G loads were higher. The other subject also reported misalignments for RM, whereas there were none.
Apparently, it is difficult to perceive the G-vector alignment in dynamic G maneuvering tasks. Despite the perceived

24
misalignments with COHAM, it was still the preferred cueing setting.
The distribution of G-levels was used as an objective measure to assess whether the centrifuge would affect the
subjects’ performance. The distributions of G-peaks do not show differences between the Dome and centrifuge G-levels,
despite the fact that the pilots were operating under considerable G-strain in Desdemona and high yaw rates up to (150
deg/s). This leads us to accept the third hypothesis, which has important implications for training, as apparently the
fixed base Dome simulator did prepare the subjects well enough to produce a set of prescribed G-maneuvers.
For the given setup and subject profile, a systematic centrifuge training of approximately one hour a day over the
course of three days has shown to produce nearly ‘exemplary’ subjects that are able to cope with high frequency and
magnitude of G-onsets, and thus large Coriolis effects, for a considerably long piloting task. The substantial decrease of
the MISC ratings for all subjects over the three days shows that they became better capable of enduring almost a full hour
of centrifuge training. Pilots indeed learn to cope with the G-environment relatively quickly, supporting hypothesis four.
In this respect, the trends in almost all ratings are similar, and show that the COHAM cueing indeed is more bearable
than the conventional cueing. The effectiveness of COHAM becomes more salient for longer centrifuge sessions.
The recovery of the MISC ratings, clearly seen over the progression of the runs from day N2 to N4, suggests that
pilots have a buffer to sustain certain levels of instantaneous G-onsets. The increase of MISC at the end of the sessions
suggests that this buffer will eventually deplete, and subjects become less able to recover from a rush of nausea. While
these observations are supportive of the positive adaptation effect in training [33, 34], the coping mechanism can also be
attributed in part to adjustments in head movements. As minimal head movements reduce the active Coriolis effect, this
negatively impacts the pilot’s natural head-scanning behavior.
The setup of the experiment allowed us to investigate the effects of centrifuge training on our subjects’ ability to
adapt to high G-levels. The novel metrics (PBWS, AWS, ALAC, AMS) proved to be useful to characterize our subjects’
well-being, endurance and witness their growing capacity to deal with the high G-onsets. For instance, the AWS allowed
to test the motion cueing on different levels of well-being metrics, such as accumulated dizziness, sickness, comfort and
bearability, that would otherwise remain implicit in the MISC.

B. Recommendations
An obvious recommendation is to continue investigating the tuning of COHAM, but with more pilots. Although
some clear trends were present in the data, inter-subject differences exist, and the number of participants needs to be
considerably higher to draw any firm conclusions.
Regarding the Coherence Alignment Zone (CAZ), a number of recommendations can be made. First, in this work
the CAZ thresholds were obtained using naive subjects, not using experienced fighter pilots. The reported mismatches
by two of our three subjects suggests that their CAZ thresholds may be considerably lower. Second, the COHAM
mismatch of 5 degrees represents a static CAZ threshold, which might depend on the experienced G-level, and we only
measured it for relatively low G levels. Perhaps a more dynamic, i.e., G-level-dependent threshold could be determined
and used in COHAM. Our evaluation suggests that the effects of a prolonged exposure to a static cabin mismatch,
and the resulting perceived G-level, needs to be further investigated. That is, while maneuvering, do pilots obtain
information to ‘calibrate’ the alignment, and with that start to notice any misalignments?
Regarding COHAM, the fixed tilt offset which was noticed by one subject is not a real requirement of the filter, it
was only implemented to further limit the cabin rotations. COHAM can be easily adapted to maintain true coordination
in the baseline, which would, however, render it less effective. An alternative, although more complex, could be to
develop a mechanism that detects ‘prolonged phases of steady flight’, and during these periods bring the cabin alignment
back to the baseline. For simulation training sessions where the trajectory to be flown, or the tasks to be conducted, are
to some extent predictable, this could be a viable option.
In this respect, for high-performance maneuvering with repetitive trajectory pursuit tasks, (i.e., where subjects are
asked to track or produce a specific G-profile) our study suggests that systematic training can help pilots to achieve
consistent performance in the centrifuge. However, for tasks with an unpredictable outcome (e.g., chase, dogfight)
the differences may be larger. In addition, when pilots are instructed to carry out concentration demanding tasks, or
simultaneously make difficult decisions, their performance might be affected under high G-load [7]. It is recommended
to further investigate this and study how the increased endurance with COHAM can be better exploited.
Finally, the correspondence of our subjects’ verbal reports with their AWS scores suggests that for future studies,
these scales could indeed provide a convenient and simple way of assessing the subject’s accumulated well-being in
centrifuge training. Various coping mechanisms can be at work here, and it is recommended to investigate ways to
clarify what mechanisms work best, whether they can be related to subjective ratings, and how they can be trained.

25
VIII. Conclusions
A novel motion cueing filter for centrifuge-based simulators is presented, COHAM, which aims to mitigate adverse
Coriolis effects through reducing cabin rotations. Results show that for prolonged centrifuge sessions, in particular
featuring high G-maneuvers, the filter was found to reduce sickness and dizziness, and increased the subjects’ bearability
and comfort. Objective performance metrics, such as average G-levels, did not change relative to a fixed base simulation.
The novel filter may allow for longer exposure of pilots to elevated G-levels in centrifuge-based simulators. Future
research focuses on whether dynamic thresholds, changing with actual simulated G-levels, can lead to improvements.

References
[1] Burton, R. R., “G-induced Loss of Consciousness: Definition, History, Current Status,” Aviation, space, and environmental
medicine, Vol. 59, No. 1, 1988, pp. 2–5.

[2] Bischoff, D. E., and Poole, D. A., “Flight Simulation Fidelity in a Total G-Force Environment,” Proc. of the AIAA Flight Simulation
Technologies Conference, AIAA, St. Louis (MO), 1985, pp. 29–36. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.1985-1741.

[3] Eyth Jr., J., and Heffner, P. L., “Design and performance of the centrifuge-based Dynamic Flight Simulator ,” Proc. of the
AIAA Flight Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Hilton Head Island (SC), 1992, pp. 183–190. https://doi.org/https:
//doi.org/10.2514/6.1992-4156.

[4] Kiefer, D. A., and Calvert, J. F., “Developmental evaluation of a centrifuge flight simulator as an enhanced maneuverability
flying qualities tool,” Proc. of the AIAA Flight Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Hilton Head Island (SC), 1992, pp.
191–198. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.1992-4157.

[5] Vasiletz, V. M., and Yakimenko, O. A., “Semi-full-scale dynamic simulation complex on the basis of centrifuge,” Proc.
of the AIAA Flight Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Monterey (CA), 1993, pp. 187–194. https://doi.org/https:
//doi.org/10.2514/6.1993-3577.

[6] Bles, W., Hosman, R. J. A. W., and de Graaf, B., “Desdemona - Advanced disorientation trainer and (sustained-G) flight
simulator ,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Denver (CO), 2000, pp. 1–8.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2000-4176.

[7] Creer, B. Y., Smedal, H. A., and Wingrove, R. C., “Centrifuge Study of Pilot Tolerance To Acceleration and the Effects of
Acceleration on Pilot Performance,” Tech. Rep. Technical Note D-337, NASA, 1960.

[8] Masica, R. M., “A Study to Evaluate the Suitability of a Centrifuge as a Dynamic Flight Simulator for F/A-18 Strike Fighter
Mission Training,” MSc. Thesis, University of Tennessee, 2009. https://doi.org/https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes/543.

[9] Bell, H. H., and Waag, W. L., “Evaluating the Effectiveness of Flight Simulators for Training Combat Skills: A Review,”
International Journal o Aviation Psychology, Vol. 8, No. 3, 1997, pp. 223–242. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15327108ijap0803_4.

[10] Spenny, C. H., Liebst, B. S., Chelette, T. L., Folescu, C., and Sigda, J., “Development of a sustainable-g dynamic flight
simulator ,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Denver (CO), 2000, pp. 1–8.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2000-4075.

[11] Sabourin, M.-O., and Sondermeyer, V., “G-Cueing for Figher Simulation Training,” Proc. of the AIAA Modelling and Simulation
Technologies Conference, AIAA, Denver (CO), 2000, pp. 1–8. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2000-4073.

[12] Ng, C., Sondermeyer, V., and Lewis, M., “G-Cueing System Tuning Optimization,” Proc. of the AIAA Modelling and Simulation
Technologies Conference, AIAA, Montreal, Canada, 2001, pp. 1–8. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2001-4306.

[13] Vidakovic, J., Lazarevic, M., Kvrgic, V., Vasovic Maksimovic, I., and Rakic, A., “Flight Simulation Training Devices:
Application, Classification, and Research,” International Journal of Aeronautical and Space Sciences, 2021. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s42405-021-00358-y, URL https://doi.org/10.1007/s42405-021-00358-y.

[14] Comtois, P. W., and Glaser, S. T., “Effectiveness of Sustained G Simulation In Loss of Control and Upset Recovery
Training,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Toronto, Canada, 2010, pp. 1–8.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2010-7798.

[15] IATA, “Loss of Control In-Flight Accident Analysis Report Edition 2019 Guidance Material and Best Practices,” Tech. rep.,
IATA, 2019. URL https://www.iata.org/contentassets/b6eb2adc248c484192101edd1ed36015/loc-i_2019.pdf.

26
[16] Belcastro, C. M., Foster, J. V., Shah, G. H., Gregory, I. M., Cox, D. E., Crider, D. A., Groff, L., Newman, R. L., and Klyde,
D. H., “Aircraft loss of control problem analysis and research toward a holistic solution,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and
Dynamics, Vol. 40, No. 4, 2017, pp. 733–775. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G002815, URL www.aiaa.org/randp.

[17] Stepanyan, V., Krishnakumar, K., Dorais, G., Reardon, S., Barlow, J., Lampton, A., and Hardy, G., “Loss-of-control
mitigation via predictive cuing,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 40, No. 4, 2017, pp. 831–846.
https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G001731, URL http://arc.aiaa.org.

[18] Van Baelen, D., Ellerbroek, J., van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Design of a haptic feedback system for flight envelope
protection,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 43, No. 4, 2020, pp. 700–714. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G004596,
URL https://arc.aiaa.org/doi/abs/10.2514/1.G004596.

[19] Klyde, D. H., Lampton, A. K., Richards, N. D., and Cogan, B., “Flight-test evaluation of a loss-of-control mitigation system,”
Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 40, No. 4, 2017, pp. 981–997. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G001486, URL
http://arc.aiaa.org.

[20] Smaili, M. H., Breeman, J., Lombaerts, T. J., Mulder, J. A., Chu, Q. P., and Stroosma, O., “Intelligent flight control systems
evaluation for loss-of-control recovery and prevention,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 40, No. 4, 2017, pp.
890–904. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G001756, URL http://arc.aiaa.org.

[21] Ledegang, W. D., Groen, E. L., and Wentink, M., “Pilot performance in centrifuge-based simulation of unusual attitude recovery,”
Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 49, No. 4, 2012, pp. 1161–1167. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.C031629, URL http://arc.aiaa.org.

[22] Nooij, S. A. E., Wentink, M., Smaili, H., Zaichik, L., and Groen, E. L., “Motion simulation of transport aircraft in extended
envelopes: Test pilot assessment,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 40, No. 4, 2017, pp. 776–788.
https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G001790, URL http://arc.aiaa.orghttps://arc.aiaa.org/doi/abs/10.2514/1.G001790.

[23] Abramov, N. B., Goman, M. G., Khrabrov, A. N., and Soemarwoto, B. I., “Aerodynamic modeling for poststall flight simulation
of a transport airplane,” Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 56, No. 4, 2019, pp. 1427–1440. https://doi.org/10.2514/1.C034790, URL
https://arc.aiaa.org/doi/abs/10.2514/1.C034790.

[24] Tribukait, A., and Eiken, O., “Roll-Tilt Perception During Gondola Centrifugation: Influence of Steady-State Acceleration (G)
Level,” Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, Vol. 77, No. 7, 2006, pp. 695–703.

[25] Tribukait, A., Bergsten, E., and Eiken, O., “Variability in Perceived Tilt During a Roll Plane Canal-Otolith Conflict
in a Gondola Centrifuge,” Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, Vol. 84, No. 11, 2013, pp. 1131–1139. https:
//doi.org/10.3357/ASEM.3655.2013.

[26] Sides, M. B., Vernikos, J., Convertino, V. A., Stepanek, J., Tripp, L. D., Draeger, J., Hargens, A. R., Kourtidou-Papadeli, C.,
Pavy-LeTraon, A., Russomano, T., Wong, J. Y., Buccello, R. R., Lee, P. H., Nangalia, V., and Saary, M. J., “The Bellagio report:
Cardiovascular risks of spaceflight: Implications for the future of space travel,” Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine,
Vol. 76, No. 9, 2005, pp. 877–895. URL https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/asma/asem/2005/00000076/00000009/
art00010.

[27] Pavy-Le Traon, A., Heer, M., Narici, M. V., Rittweger, J., and Vernikos, J., “From space to Earth: Advances in human
physiology from 20 years of bed rest studies (1986-2006),” European Journal of Applied Physiology, Vol. 101, No. 2, 2007, pp.
143–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-007-0474-z.

[28] Mahmoodi, A., and Kazemi, I., “Optimal Motion Cueing Algorithm for Accelerating Phase of Manned Spacecraft in Human
Centrifuge,” Chinese Journal of Aeronautics, Vol. 33, No. 7, 2020, pp. 1991–2001.

[29] Stewart, J. D., and Clark, B., “Coriolis Effects During Pitch and Roll Maneuvers in a Piloted Flight Simulator,” Aerospace
Medicine, Vol. 36, No. 2, 1965, pp. 105–112.

[30] Reason, J., and Brand, J. J., Motion sickness / J. T. Reason, J. J. Brand, Academic Press London ; New York, 1975.

[31] Glaser, S. T., and Newman, M. C., “Quantification of Coriolis Cross-Coupling Intensity during Centrifugation and Sustained-G
Flight Training,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Boston (MA), 2013, pp. 1–8.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2013-4977.

[32] Bertolini, G., and Straumann, D., “Moving in a moving world: A review on vestibular motion sickness,” , feb 2016.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2016.00014, URL www.frontiersin.org.

27
[33] Young, L. R., Sienko, K. H., Lyne, L. E., Hecht, H., and Natapoff, A., “Adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex, subjective tilt,
and motion sickness to head movements during short-radius centrifugation,” Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium
and Orientation, Vol. 13, No. 2-3, 2003, pp. 65–77. URL https://content.iospress.com/articles/journal-of-vestibular-research/
ves00161.

[34] Newman, M. C., McCarthy, G. W., Glaser, S. T., Bonato, F., and Bubka, A., “Motion sickness adaptation to coriolis-inducing
head movements in a sustained G flight simulator,” Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, Vol. 84, No. 2, 2013, pp.
104–109. https://doi.org/10.3357/ASEM.3170.2013.

[35] Frett, T., Green, D. A., Arz, M., Noppe, A., Petrat, G., Kramer, A., Kuemmel, J., Tegtbur, U., and Jordan, J., “Motion sickness
symptoms during jumping exercise on a short-arm centrifuge,” PLoS ONE, Vol. 15, No. 6, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0234361.

[36] Mkhoyan, T., Wentink, M., van Paassen, M. M., Mulder, M., and de Graaf, B., “Mitigating the Coriolis Effect in Human
Centrifuges by Coherent G-misalignment,” AIAA Scitech 2019 Forum, 2019, pp. 1–22. https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2019-0714.

[37] Crosbie, R. J., and Kiefer, D. A., “Controlling the human centrifuge as a force and motion platform for the dynamic
flight simulator,” Proc. of the AIAA Flight Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, St. Louis (MO), 1985, pp. 37–44.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.1985-1742.

[38] Wentink, M., Bles, W., Hosman, R. J. A. W., and Mayrhofer, M., “Design and Evaluation of Spherical Washout Algorithm for
Desdemona Simulator ,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, San Francisco (CA),
2005, pp. 1–8. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2005-6501.

[39] Glaser, S. T., and Newman, M. C., “G-Pointing: Articulated Centrifuge for Real-Time G Flight Simulation ,” Proc. of
the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA, Portland (OR), 2011, pp. 1–8. https://doi.org/https:
//doi.org/10.2514/6.2011-6496.

[40] Sturgeon, W. R., “Controllers for Aircraft Motion Simulators,” Journal of Guidance and Control, Vol. 4, No. 2, 1981, pp.
184–194. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/3.56070.

[41] Nahon, M. A., and Reid, L. D., “Simulator motion-drive algorithms - A designer’s perspective,” Journal of Guidance and
Control, Vol. 13, No. 2, 1990, pp. 438–448. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/3.20557.

[42] Grant, P. R., and Reid, L. D., “PROTEST: An Expert System for Tuning Simulator Washout Filters,” Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 34,
No. 2, 1997, pp. 152–159. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/2.2166.

[43] Beccera-Vargas, M., and Belo, E. M., “Robust Control of Flight Simulator Motion Base,” Journal of Guidance, Control, and
Dynamics, Vol. 34, No. 5, 2012, pp. 1519–1529. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/1.53301.

[44] Schroeder, J. A., and Chung, W. W. Y., “Simulator Platform Motion Effects on Pilot-Induced Oscillation Prediction,” Journal of
Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 23, No. 3, 2000, pp. 438–448. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/2.4578.

[45] Hosman, R. J. A. W., and Advani, S. K., “Integrated Design of the Motion Cueing System for a Wright Flyer Simulator,”
Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 28, No. 1, 2005, pp. 43–52.

[46] Hess, R. A., and Marchesi, F., “Analytical Assessment of Flight Simulator Fidelity Using Pilot Models,” Journal of Guidance,
Control, and Dynamics, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2009, pp. 760–770. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/1.40645.

[47] Miletović, I., Pavel, M. D., Pool, D. M., Stroosma, O., Van Paassen, M. M., Mulder, M., and Wentink, M., “Eigenmode
Distortion: A New Perspective on Motion Cueing Fidelity in Flight Simulation,” Journal of Guidance, Control & Dynamics,
Vol. 44, No. 7, 2021, pp. 1314–1326. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/1.G005596.

[48] Roza, M. Z., Wentink, M., and Feenstra, P., “Performance Testing of the Desdemona Motion System,” Collection of
Technical Papers - 2007 AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, Vol. 1, 2007, pp. 275–288. https:
//doi.org/10.2514/6.2007-6472.

[49] Desdemona, “Welkom to Desdemona.eu - Desdemona,” \url{http://www.desdemona.eu/}, 2010. URL http://www.desdemona.


eu/desdemona.html.

[50] Schubert, G., Die physiologischen Auswirkungen der Coriolisbeschleunigungen bei Flugzeugsteuerung, J. Springer, 1932.

[51] Guedry, F. E., “Psychophysics of Vestibular Sensation,” Vestibular System Part 2: Psychophysics, Applied Aspects and General
Interpretations, Springer, 1974, pp. 3–154. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-65920-1_1.

28
[52] Holly, J. E., “Subject-Coincident Coordinate Systems and Sustained Motions,” International Journal of Theoretical Physics,
Vol. 35, No. 2, 1996, pp. 445–473. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02083826.

[53] Holly, J. E., Vrublevskis, A., and Carlson, L. E., “Whole-Motion Model of Perception During Forward- and Backward-Facing
Centrifuge Runs,” Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium and Orientation, Vol. 18, No. 4, 2008, pp. 171–186.

[54] Holly, J. E., “Vestibular Coriolis Effect Differences Modeled with three-Dimensional Linear-Angular Interactions,” Journal of
Vestibular Research: Equilibrium and Orientation, Vol. 14, No. 6, 2004, pp. 443–460.

[55] Guedry, F. E., and Benson, A. J., “Coriolis Cross-Coupling Effects: Disorienting and Nauseogenic or Not?” Aviation Space and
Environmental Medicine, Vol. 49, No. 1 I, 1978, pp. 29–35.

[56] Van der Steen, F. A. M., “Self-motion perception,” Ph.D. thesis, Delft University of Technology, 1998.

[57] Valente Pais, A. R., Van Paassen, M. M., Mulder, M., and Wentink, M., “Perception Coherence Zones in Flight Simulation,”
Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 47, No. 6, 2010, pp. 2039–2048.

[58] Correia Grácio, B. J., Pais, A. R., Van Paassen, M. M., Mulder, M., Kelly, L. C., and Houck, J. A., “Optimal and
Coherence Zone Comparison Within and Between Flight Simulators,” Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 50, No. 2, 2013, pp. 493–507.
https://doi.org/10.2514/1.C031870.

[59] Correia-Gracio, B. J., Wentink, M., Groen, E., and Bles, W., “Subjective Estimates of G-load in Centrifuge-Based Simulation
and Applications for G-cueing in Desdemona,” Proc. of the AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference, AIAA,
Chicago (IL), 2009, pp. 1–8. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2514/6.2009-5919.

[60] Mkhoyan, T., “Minimisation of Coriolis Effect in Human Centrifuges: Preliminary Study,” Tech. rep., Delft University of
Technology, 2017.

[61] Matlab, “Dynamic Lookup Table Mathworks,” \url{https://nl.mathworks.com/help/simulink/slref/lookuptabledynamic.html/},


2014.

[62] Barco, “Barco Dome,” \url{http://www.image-society.org/IMG/1302.html/}, 2010.

[63] Bos, J. E., and Krul, A. J., Predicting Motion Induced Vomiting From Subjective Misery ( MISC ) Ratings Obtained in 12
Experimental Studies, TNO Human Factors, 2001.

[64] Stanney, K. M., Handbook of Virtual Environments: Design, Implementation, and Applications, CRC Press, 2002.

[65] Bos, J. E., de Vries, S. C., van Emmerik, M. L., and Groen, E. L., “The effect of Internal and External Fields of View on
Visually Induced Motion Sickness,” Applied Ergonomics, Vol. 41, No. 4, 2010, pp. 516–521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.
2009.11.007.

[66] Griffin, M. J., and Newman, M. M., “Visual Field Effects on Motion Sickness in Cars,” Aviation Space and Environmental
Medicine, Vol. 75, No. 9, 2004, pp. 739–748.

29

You might also like