Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and
Retrospective Theses and Dissertations
Dissertations
1993
Three papers on homosexual sexual scripts
Brian Magruder
Iowa State University
Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd
Part of the Family, Life Course, and Society Commons
Recommended Citation
Magruder, Brian, "Three papers on homosexual sexual scripts " (1993). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 10841.
https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/10841
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University
Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University
Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected].
U-M-I
MICROFILMED 1994
INFORMATION TO USERS
This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI
films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some
thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may
be from any type of computer printer.
The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the
copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality
illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins,
and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete
manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if
unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate
the deletion.
Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by
sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and
continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each
original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in
reduced form at the back of the book.
Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced
xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white
photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations
appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly
to order.
University Microfilms International
A Bell & Howell Information Company
300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA
313/761-4700 800/521-0600
Order Number 9414002
Three papers on homosexual sexual scripts
Magruder, Brian, Ph.D.
Iowa State University, 1993
UMI
300 N. ZeebRd.
Ann Arbor, MI 48106
Three papers on homosexual
sexual scripts
by
Brian Magruder
A Dissertation Submitted to the
Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of the
Requirements for the Degree of
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
Department; Sociology
Major; Sociology
Approved;
Signature was redacted for privacy.
In Charge of Major Work
Signature was redacted for privacy.
For the Major Department
Signature was redacted for privacy.
Fop/^e Graduate College
Iowa State University
Ames, Iowa
1993
ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
GENERAL INTRODUCTION AND THEORY 1
Introduction 1
The Sexual Script Perspective 5
Organization of Dissertation 28
PAPER 1. ADOLESCENT DISCLOSURE OF HOMOSEXUALITY
TO THEIR FAMILIES 31
ABSTRACT 32
INTRODUCTION 33
Gay and lesbian youths 35
Disclosure and family dynamics 36
THEORETICAL ANALYSIS 40
METHODS 45
Sample 45
Measurement 46
Procedures 49
RESULTS 51
DISCUSSION 55
CONCLUSION 58
BIBLIOGRAPHY 60
iii
PAPER 2. THE DEFINITION OF ATTRACTIVENESS AND
SENSUALITY: A SCHEMATIC COMPARISON
BETWEEN HOMOSEXUALS AND HETEROSEXUALS 67
ABSTRACT 68
INTRODUCTION 69
REVIEW OF LITERATURE 71
Objective versus subjective definitions of
attractiveness 71
Objective criteria of attractiveness 72
Gender differences 75
A SCHEMATIC ANALYSES 77
Schematics 78
Hypotheses 81
METHODS 83
Homosexual sample 83
Heterosexual sample 85
Measurement 85
Cautions and limitations 89
RESULTS 90
Definitions of attractiveness 90
Definitions of sensuality 92
Definitional differences between attractiveness
and sensuality 94
Group comparisons 95
DISCUSSION 99
BIBLIOGRAPHY 103
iv
PAPER 3. THE VIOLENT EFFECTS OF DISCRIMINATION
ON GAY AND LESBIAN RELATIONSHIPS 118
ABSTRACT 119
INTRODUCTION 120
MODELING VIOLENCE IN GAY/LESBIAN RELATIONSHIPS 122
Cultural scenario scripts 123
Interrelational scripts 127
Intrapsychic scripts 128
Hypothesized models 130
METHODS 132
Measurement 132
Procedures 137
RESULTS 139
DISCUSSION 150
BIBLIOGRAPHY 154
GENERAL SUMMARY 163
Summary of Results 163
Limitations and Future Research 182
Conclusion 191
BIBLIOGRAPHY 195
APPENDIX 199
1
GENERAL INTRODUCTION AND THEORY
Introduction
Homosexuality is either a feeling of sexual desires for
persons of one's own sex or sexual relations with a member of
one's own sex, or both (Thio, 1983). The past forty years
have witnessed several shifts in sociological research
concerning the subject of homosexuality. At the onset of the
late forties and up to the early seventies, research on sexual
orientation was a waxing field of study. As research began to
wane on approximating the parameters of the homosexual
population, inquiry into causal development began to dominate
the scientific investigation of homosexuality. This research
trend spanned the early sixties to the early eighties. The
late eighties brought a psychological shift in the sexual
orientation research, focusing primarily on the social
processes of homosexuals, including concepts such as
homosexual identity development. The nineties has undertaken
the scientific study of homosexual relationships, denoting
differences and similarities between heterosexual
relationships. This dissertation will continue with this
trend by exploring homosexual relationships as extrapolated
from a sexual script perspective.
Kinsey and others pioneered the first extensive study of
homosexuality in 1948. Their research established the extent
of participation in homosexual activities by white males
(Kinsey, Pomeroy, and Martin, 1948). Further research has
2
attempted to approximate the parameters - boundaries - of the
homosexual population by replicating Kinsey et al.'s initial
study. Such researchers have concluded that between ten and
twelve percent of any given population to be of a same-sex
orientation (Athanasiou, Shaver, and Travis, 1970; Hayes and
Oziel, 1976; Hunt, 1974). Applying these percentages to the
current population of the United States would roughly be
equivalent to over thirty million homosexuals residing in the
United States.
With regard to causal development, the origin of
homosexuality and heterosexuality remain a mystery. However,
it seems apparent both biological and psychosocial
(sociological and psychological) factors are implicated in
homosexuality. The extensive work of Money (1980) on genetic
and hormonal factors influencing sexual drives compounded with
the recent research being conducted on the organic dimensions
of the brain provide a convincing argument for the biological
premise of a homosexual orientation. However, other
researchers indicate no clear genetic differences between
homosexuals and heterosexuals (Gagnon, 1977; Masters and
Johnson, 1979).
Sexual identities, perceptions of self as heterosexual,
homosexual, or bisexual in relation to sexual, romantic, and
broader cultural arrangements are constructed through various
forces. The homosexual identity is one of several identities
incorporated into an individual's concept of self. According
3
to Cass (1983) depending on the context, the homosexual
identity may function as a self-identity, a perceived
identity, a presented identity, or all three^ The homosexual
identity is a self-identity when people see themselves as
homosexual in relation to romantic or sexual encounters and
engagements. It is a perceived identity in situations where
people think or know that others view them as homosexual. It
is a presented identity when people present or announce
themselves as homosexual in concrete social settings, i.e.,
occupational environments. Troiden (1984/1985) expands on
Cass's research by asserting homosexual identity is firmly
established when an individual has developed a positive gay
self-image.
During the past decade, several researchers have proposed
theoretical models, attempting to explain the formation and
continuance of homosexual identities (Cass, 1979, 1983; Lee,
1977; Ponse, 1978; Shafer, 1976; Troiden, 1977, 1979;
Weinberg, 1977, 1978). Although the various models suggest
different stages in the formation of homosexual identities,
striking similarities emerge as transitional growth periods
for homosexual identity development.
The social stigma and sanctioning surrounding
homosexuality in the United States affects both the formation
and expression of homosexuality. Therefore, homosexual
identities are described as developing over a protracted
period of time and involving a number of transitional stages.
4
In short, homosexual identity formation entails increasing
acceptance of the label "homosexual" as applied to the self in
spite of possible resistance or sanctioning from society. The
homosexual individual, accepting this stigmatized identity,
initiates disclosure of their identity, and finally commits to
the identity through love relationships or membership in the
gay subculture (Cass, 1983; Coleman, 1982; Lee, 1977).
Although a substantial amount of empirical attention has
been given to the formation of homosexuality, its causes and
consequences; little sociological theorizing has been
submitted regarding scripted intimate relationships among
homosexuals. The role of social structure, culture, and
various systems have been neglected in the literature through
the sole analysis of the gay/lesbian relationship as an entity
within itself, apart from cultural context. Not only must
researchers carefully analyze the dynamics of relationships,
and the psyche of those involved; consequently, researchers
must also study the phenomenon under the context of the
structural and cultural arrangements which frame the
relationship. Therefore, a sexual script perspective, which
entails all three dimensions (the individual, the
relationship, and culture), demonstrates a strong utility for
a sociological inquiry into gay/lesbian relationships.
5
The Sexual Script Perspective
In general, sexual scripts are understood as general
guidelines, procedural diagrams for interpreting sexually
relevant expressions and behaviors (Simon and Gagnon, 1984).
In short, sexual scripts are typologized by three distinct,
yet interconnected, levels of analysis. Cultural scenario
scripts dictate the normatively accepted or shared values
attached to sexuality. Interrelational scripts guide the
contexts of relational sexuality. Intrapsychic scripts define
the motivations, fantasies, and desires for individual
sexuality.
The sexual script perspective actually emerged from
schema theory. The term script, under a schematic definition,
refers to the dynamic knowledge surrounding particular events
and phenomenon; consequently, schema is defined as the static
knowledge unique to a given situation (Schwartz and Reisberg,
1991). In relation to sexual scripts, scripted and schematic
information is geared to the static and dynamic processes
relevant to sexual attitudes, behaviors, and life styles.
Sexual schema and scripts are means for cognitively ordering
normative or redundant and dynamic or interactive processes
associated with sexuality.
The tenets of schema theory are generally centered around
the concepts of active perception and interpretation. As a
microlevel theory, a schema reflects a generalized perception
based on redundant situations, phenomenon, and events (Schank,
6
1982). Therefore, a schema consists of familiar
understandings about people, places, norms, and actions.
Schema theory does not necessarily explain why a certain
phenomenon may occur, as much as it focuses on the
interpretation of the phenomenon by individuals.
In short, schema theory is a learning-oriented theory.
Schematas are maintained and modified by repetition and other
various learning models (i.e., reinforcement, etc.). The
assumptions represented by schema theory suggest the level of
analysis is the individual. However, schema theory does not
necessarily ignore the social context of the individual.
Socially constructed norms and values are embedded within the
frameworks of schemata, thus establishing certain parameters
for acceptable social behavior.
Not unlike symbolic interactionism, schema theory
emphasizes decoding and encoding processes in the
interpretation of phenomenon. For example, Bartlett (1932)
focused on the effect of relevant schematic patterns,
hypothesizing that a phenomenon which is not relevant or
particularly meaningful to an active schema will not receive
detailed attention. And phenomenon, which is pertinent to an
individual's schema, will be encoded.
The decoding process entails the act of interpretation,
categorizing phenomenon into compartmentalized schematas
resulting from perceptual filtering (Anderson and Pichert,
1978). The filtering process entails decoding mechanisms in
7
order to "filter out" meaningless information, and cognitiveiy
order relevant information about a given phenomenon. The
filtering process is subject to various schematic mechanisms
which may alter the perception and meaning of the phenomenon,
resulting in source confusion and schematic errors (Spiro,
1977).
The sexual script perspective borrows several key
elements and assumptions from schema theory to explain
sexuality. Again, the focus of a sexual script perspective is
on the formation and maintenance of those schemata which are
sexual in nature. Encoding, decoding, schematic errors, and
the general cognitive ordering of sexual phenomenon and
terminology are incorporated in the sexual script perspective.
However, the two theories diverge on the point of analysis.
Where schema theory tends to view the individual in isolation,
centering only on the cognitions of the individual; sexual
scripts tends to partition schemata into environmental
contexts and effects. In short, schema theory maintains the
individual is the centralized element of interpretation. The
sexual script perspective contends the individual is an active
component, but must be understood in terms of the social
context of the phenomenon.
The domain assumption of the sexual script perspective
maintains humans are primarily social in their learning of
sexuality (Gagnon, 1977). At the beginning of life, humans
are defined as being polymorphously perverse, in other words.
8
a "blank slate" of sexuality. During childhood and
adolescence, men and women begin to learn and incorporate
general norms and values regarding sexuality via their
socialization agents. By early adulthood, men and women have
developed their sexual scripts concerning sexual behaviors,
attitudes, relationships, desires, and sanctions. Sexual
scripts define the affective and cognitive boundaries for
appropriate and inappropriate sexuality. Scripts designate
acceptable and unacceptable partners, behaviors, fantasies,
techniques, and emotions. Although, sexuality does maintain
an imperative biological element, the sexual script
perspective facilitates attention to important social forces
which guide and shape sexuality. In many cases, these scripts
are congruent with previous gender-role socialization (Levine,
1987). For example, males tend to view sexuality in
instrumental, active means; consequently, females perceive
sexuality in expressive, process-oriented methods (Laws and
Schwartz, 1977).
Cultural scenario scripts reflect the broader cultural
and structural arrangements which represent the official and
public means to legitimate or illegitimate sexual behaviors,
and life styles. Cultural scenario scripts can be
characterized by three distinct categories; procreative,
recreational, and relational (Gagnon and Simon, 1977).
Procreative scripts maintain sexuality is only legitimated for
reproductive purposes. Recreational scripts propose sexuality
9
is for enjoyment and pleasure, regardless of the relational or
reproductive facets. Relational scripts dictate the context
for appropriate sexual relations. If a culture warrants a
relational script, members in the culture adopt the belief
patterns that sexuality is only permitted and legitimate in
committed, affective relationships.
Interrelational (or interpersonal) scripts serve to
mediate between intrapsychic and cultural scenario scripts.
Interrelational scripts facilitate the norms and values
surrounding sexuality, from the broader social forces, in
preexisting and future relationships with others. However,
interrelational scripts also expedite personal fantasies and
sexual motivations into the relationship. Interrelational
scripts further incorporate the fantasies and sexual
curiosities of the significant other. In short,
interrelational scripts bridge the gap between general
cultural forces and personal desires. The concept of sexual
identity, whether heterosexual or homosexual, emerges at this
particular level. Sexual identity, from an interrelational
script, consists of expectations and the social roles embedded
in sexuality with others, primarily significant others.
Interrelational scripts are constantly being modified
throughout the relationship, and with each new relationship
which follows. Modification of interrelational scripts may
occur when sexual partners or significant others present new,
different, or variant sexual expectations. When opposing
10
interrelational scripts converge, individuals negotiate new
interrelational scripts for accommodating the disparity, under
the constraints of the prevailing cultural scenario scripts.
Intrapsychic scripts serve as the latent features of
individual sexuality. Intrapsychic scripts are the internal
desires, motivations, fantasies, those characteristics which
foster sexual arousal and excitement. In short, intrapsychic
scripts are responsible for stimulating and maintaining sexual
arousal. Intrapsychic scripts also incorporate those
characteristics which are personality-oriented, such as self-
esteem, body image, feelings of sexual adequacy, and the like.
Such personality features serve to facilitate sexual and
affective motivations.
Sexual scripts can be divided into two categories,
homosexual and heterosexual sexual scripts. Although both are
not mutually exclusive in content, homosexual scripts do
possess unique characteristics which deviate from the dominant
heterosexual script. One might suggest, all participants in a
society are socialized with heterosexual scripts; however,
only integrated homosexuals (maintaining a positive homosexual
V
self-image or identity) are privy to homosexual scripts.
Homosexual sexual scripts incorporate the cultural,
heterosexist norms governing acceptance of homosexuality; and,
in turn, homosexual scripts incorporate the homosexual
subcultural characteristics which reflect those norms and
values specific to the gay subculture. Homosexual scripts
11
differ from heterosexual scripts in one facet due to the vast
stigma attached to homosexuality. In other words, homosexual
scripts must consequently deal with a divisive stigma attached
to their entire sexual life style. Heterosexual structural
and cultural influences impact on definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality, homosexual identity formation
and development, and relational dynamics as affected by
discriminatory stress, the themes of this dissertation.
As cultural scenario scripts influence the context or
domain in which homosexuality transpires, interrelational
scripts, for homosexuals, represent the dimensions of their
relationships which are disparate from heterosexual
relationships, perhaps due to stigmatizing or isolating
structural influences. Intrapsychic scripts reflect the
internalized dimensions of cultural and interrelational
scripts via motives, and desires relevant to homosexual
expressions, identity salience, and sexual symbolism. All
three levels of scripting are interdynamic processes,
constantly modified throughout the life course by interchanges
with various structural and relational influences.
Theory or perspective?
The sexual script perspective builds its theoretical
interpretation on many sociological theories, borrowing
particular concepts and propositions which place the context
of homosexuality in the eye of the culture or system (cultural
12
scenario scripts). Questions of sexuality are cultural
constructions, specific to time and place: what sexuality is,
the purposes it serves, its manner of expression, and what it
means to be sexual. Lesbianism and male homosexuality are
similarly constructed and culture bound. Sexual behavior and
responsiveness span the spectrum from exclusive
heterosexuality to exclusive homosexuality; the positions that
people occupy on this spectrum result from social learning in
specific environments.
According to Niklas Luhmann (Turner, 1991), society is
composed of three dynamic "social systems": interaction
systems, organization systems, and societal systems. The
"interaction system" revolves around the presence of the
individual, and those in contact with the individual.
Organizational systems "coordinate the actions of individuals
with respect to specific conditions" (Turner, 1991: 96). The
societal system establishes the boundaries and guidelines for
interactions between individuals and culture. Therefore,
cultural scenario scripts view homosexual identity as grounded
in specific social contexts or interaction systems. Normative
structures prevailing in the societal system govern activity
in specific contexts that determines which roles and
identities may be expressed legitimately. And organizational
systems place limits on the identities and roles that may be
enacted appropriately.
13
The sexual script perspective treats the homosexual
identity as a construct embedded within an interrelational
script and an intrapsychic script. Further, a homosexual
identity will only be mobilized or set forth in situations
which norms permit it to be presented-an important function
provided by the various subcultures and scenes that constitute
the homosexual subculture. A homosexual intrapsychic script,
and perhaps a homosexual interrelational script, is manifested
only in systems that honor homosexual conduct and allow him or
her to claim the identity or express the corresponding role,
although the experience of homosexual identity may be
triggered when homosexual elements are introduced in
heterosexual systems (Troiden, 1984/1985). The homosexual
identity is suppressed, and becomes latent, when an individual
leaves homosexual systems for more conventional environments.
In other words, the homosexual intrapsychic script is
confounded outside of a cultural scenario script tolerant of
homosexuality.
The most extreme statement of the sexual script
perspective is expressed by sociologist Edward Sagarin (1973,
1975, 1976, 1979). He points out that many people have
homosexual desires, but that relatively few individuals
translate these desires into behavior; fewer yet define
themselves as homosexual in the sense of an intrapsychic
script. He argues that people commit the fallacy of
reification when they define themselves as "being" what they
14
are "doing." In other words, discontinuity between a
homosexual intrapsychic script and a heterosexual cultural
scenario script results in fragmentation or
compartmentalization in the interrelational script. In order
for a continuous homosexual identity to emerge, from a sexual
script perspective, all three scripts (cultural,
interrelational, and intrapsychic) must be congruous, despite
the cultural resistance provided by the dominant heterosexual
cultural scenario scripts. Therefore, it is imperative to
examine the role of cultural and structural forces which shape
the sexual scripting among homosexuals.
Again, the influence of culture or social systems plays
an integral component in the manifestation of the various
sexual scripts. For example, several patterns of formal and
informal lesbian relationships emerge in nonclass societies
where women have greater autonomy. Among the polygynous
Azande of Africa, for example, where each wife had her own
dwelling and her own plot of land, and controlled whatever
profits from her work she made through trade, some women
established lesbian relationships with their co-wives within
the formal, organizational structure of polygynous marriage.
Husbands could not forbid these arrangements, but the wives
kept them secret to avoid threatening their husbands
(Blackwood, 1985). Lesbian relationships are also reported to
exist between co-wives of the Nyakyusa, another polygynous
African group (Adam, 1985).
15
In class societies, where women lack autonomy and power,
formal lesbian relationships, if they exist at all, occupy a
marginal status in relation to the dominant culture or the
societal system. In the Near East, for example, lesbian
behavior among women in harems and within the Muslim
institution of purdah was informal. It should be noted that
homosexual behavior is outlawed under Islamic law, and
adultery is punishable by divorce or death (Blackwood, 1985).
Therefore, concepts such as stratification guide the normative
context, ensuing the development of a cultural scenario script
governing homosexual relations.
The ethnographic evidence also suggests that age-
structured homosexuality among unmarried male youths is
extremely common in societies where "bachelorhood" is a
transitional status between childhood and adulthood.
Standards, associated with age appropriate sexual behavior, is
but one of the many aspects enveloped under cultural scenario
scripts. Among the Nyakyusa, for example, young males between
the ages of ten and or eleven and twenty-five separate from
their families to form peer groups that set up entirely new
villages for themselves. "It is generally accepted that these
youths engage in reciprocal homosexual relations" (Adam, 1985:
21).
Ritualized homosexual relations between older males and
younger males are even more common and provide a contrast to
the peer-oriented models described above. An older married
16
male enters into a role-structured pedagogic/sexual
relationship with a young man. The older man socializes the
youth to male culture and gender functions. A role
differentiation consistent with this "one-way" socialization
emerges: The older man is the provider and the younger man is
the recipient, and this differentiation "structures anal and
oral intercourse" (Adam, 1985; 25). The social categories of
heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual are meaningless and
irrelevant. The fundamental social division in these
societies is between the relatively "free, adult male citizen
and all others. Sexuality is merely one of many rights
exercised by the free male over women, youth, and slaves"
(Adam, 1985: 22). This example portrays the means in which
the cultural scenario scripts, of a given society, influences
the structure of not only gay and lesbian relationships, but
also heterosexual engagements.
For other societies, older bachelors enter into role-
structured sexual relationships with younger boys. "Gender
ideology plays the primary role in determining sexuality among
males" (Adam, 1985; 22). Homosexuality is associated with
gender differentiation (masculinization of the male) rather
than gender mixing (effiminization of the male), a view that
prevails in the contemporary United States. Sexuality is only
one of the many ways in which males set themselves apart from
women and perpetuate the male group.
17
The anthropological case studies reveal structural
components in the evolution of homosexual sexual scripts.
Homosexuality is manifested in its behaviors concurrent with
interaction, organization, and societal systems. However,
Luhmann's neofunctionalism is not adequately adept in
describing or explaining the social conflict between the
homosexual role and institutions, for example, in the United
States. Luhmann's analysis also does not easily lend itself
toward explaining why individuals chose expressions of their
social-roles continuously over settings. For a further
explanation of the social-role approach, the essay will now
turn toward Randall Collins' exchange conflict theory, Peter
Blau's structural exchange theory, Claude Levi-Strauss's
structuralism, and Karl Marx's theory of exchange and conflict
to increase the depth of theoretical analysis regarding the
homosexual sexual script perspective.
The tenets of exchange theory are numerous, depending on
the theoretical variation in question. However, the basic
principles underlying all variations of exchange theory can be
reduced to several key axioms. One of the propositions
includes the rationality principle. According to Peter Blau
(Turner, 1991), rationality constitutes the ability to expect
and perceive a profit from one another through a particular
activity. And the more profitable an activity is, the more
likely the activity will be repeated. A second principle is
reciprocity. Reciprocity represents any obligation which
18
arises when people have exchanged a reward or rewards with one
another. The justice principle asserts established exchange
relations and are likely to be governed by norms of equality
and fairness. The imbalance principle proposes the more
unequal the exchanges between social groups (individuals), the
more likely future exchanges will be unequal and unstable.
These general tenets comprise our understanding of structural
exchange theory from the perspective of both Claude Levi-
Strauss and Peter Blau.
Randall Collins and Karl Marx possess slightly different
theoretical perspectives, guiding their interpretation of
exchange relations. Both theorists view the exchange network
as embedded with conflict. In general, both Marx and Collins
contend exchange relations are based on the availability and
value of particular resources (Turner, 1991). Conflict and
organization arise when those who possess scarce and valued
resources control (suppress) those who do not have access to
such resources.
The rationality of homosexuality precludes the
heterosexual dogma of many structures and institutions in
heterosexist societies, such as the United States. To
understand the conflict and exchange principles of homosexual
identity acquisition, an understanding of the social
structures impairing homosexuality must be emphasized. The
analysis of innate costs and rewards attached to homosexuality
19
by social structures broadens the sociological insight to
homosexual sexual scripts.
According to Barry Adam (1978), the socio-structural
relationships between heterosexual and homosexual groups are
framed by patterns of domination and subordination. Dominant
groups produce and maintain social order because they control
the bulk of the socially valued resources. According to
Turner's interpretation of Marx's theory of exchange and
conflict, "Those who controlled valued resources have power
over those who do not" (Turner, 1991: 299). Control over
resources enables these groups to create and control social
institutions, which are structured to maximize the life
chances of dominant groups by minimizing those of subordinate
groups. Inferiorization, then, refers to the creation of
social inequality.
The inferiorization of homosexuals is evident at all
levels of society and in all major institutions. Dominant
institutions inferiorize subordinate groups by constructing
the characteristics that allegedly set them apart (e.g.,
effeminacy, same-sex attraction) and that justify their
exclusion from the hierarchy of access. Peter Blau's vision
of exchange systems and macrostructure reiterates a similar
proposition: "the significance of 'shared values' increases,
for it is through such values that indirect exchanges ... are
mediated" (Turner, 1991: 340). "Shared values" could connote
heterosexism, the belief that all individuals are and should
20
be heterosexual. If institutions are comprised of these
"shared values", then "as people and various forms of
collective organizations become dependent upon particular
networks of indirect exchanges for expected rewards, pressures
for formalizing exchange networks through explicit norms
increase" (Turner, 1991: 342). Institutions of social
control-police and the courts, for example-assume attitudes of
official blindness and neglect toward acts of aggression
leveled against inferiorized groups (e.g., hate crimes, or
discrimination) and "at the same time, the inferiorized
themselves are more frequently subject to arrest, police
harassment, and conviction by courts" (Adam, 1978: 28).
Further, "institutions exert a kind of external constraint on
individuals and various types of collective units, bending
exchange processes to fit their prescriptions and
proscriptions" (Turner, 1991: 342). Blau envisioned
institutions as possessing normative guidelines contingent
with society's "shared values." Institutions project
authority in order to establish social objectives based on
"shared values" (Turner, 1991). In the case of homosexuals,
the mode of sexual expression itself has been criminalized
under many state statutes.
Legal institutions condone inferiorization explicitly by
defending the constitutionality of discriminatory practices
(e.g., state sodomy statutes, physical harassment, etc.); they
routinize inferiorization implicitly by not outlawing
21
discrimination in employment and place of residence on the
basis of sexual orientation. Economic institutions maintain
inferiorization through discriminatory hiring and barring
homosexuals from certain occupations (e.g., the clergy,
national security), trade unions (e.g., firefighters), and
voluntary associations (e.g., Big Brothers). Political
institutions also remain, for the most part, the monopoly of
noninferiorized people.
Institutions that transmit culture-schools, churches, the
publishing industry, and the mass media-define the personal
and social characteristics of inferiorized people: "The
systematic selection of attributes of inferiorized peoples for
public presentation constructs an image which rationalizes
inferiorized status for both privileged and inferiorized
groups. Inferiorized people discover their 'objective'
identity lives beyond their control; the image of self,
institutionalized by cultural agents, exists alien to their
own experience and self-expression." (Adam, 1978: 30-31).
Inferiorized people tend to be described (if acknowledged at
all) in disrespectable contexts: crime, physical disease,
immorality, mental illness. Homosexuals, for example, have
been variously portrayed as child molesters, mentally ill, and
sinful.
Medical institutions also contribute to inferiorization
by presenting the life patterns of inferiorized people as
unnatural (e.g., the presumption that heterosexuality is the
22
biological norm), as reflecting disease (e.g., homosexuality
is caused by pathological fears of the opposite sex), as
invalid or unauthentic (e.g., homosexual relations are not
meant to be gratifying, they are simply sexually confused
situations). The maintenance of the social order, as
currently constituted, depends on the continuing
inferiorization of lesbians and gay men. The general practice
of inferiorization and the more specific pattern of stigma
shape the experience and the life chances of homosexuals. The
social-role of homosexuality from the institutional
perspective has been equally confused with the concept of
"being" and "doing." Institutions seek to influence the role
of homosexuals, yet subordinate the "being" (identity) of
homosexuality. In other words, institutions create obstacles
for homosexual identity formation, and the manifestation of
integrated homosexual sexual scripts.
Why then does the social-role of homosexuality pervade in
societies constructed by heterosexist institutions? The
decision to participate in homosexual roles is based on the
perceived rewards inherent in the organizational structure.
Sexual behaviors are one of the most valued experiences for
human beings as a species and for most people as individuals.
Its valuation varies from those who are indifferent, to those
that are relatively addicted to sexual gratification (Burgess
and Wallin, 1953; Cuber and Haroff, 1965). Sexual
gratification, feelings of intimacy, and innate desires
23
(intrapsychic scripts) are individual responses to the
socially constructed rewards and costs attached to variant
sexualities. According to exchange theory, conceptualization
of homosexual identities are expressed at a cost/benefit
ratio. Those behaviors or identities which have the greatest
rewards or benefits will increase the salience or expression
of a particular behavior or identity. Those identities or
behaviors which entail the greatest costs will decrease the
salience of identity constructs or the probability of
retaining certain behaviors or beliefs.
Benefits and costs are unevenly skewed toward
heterosexual relations in the United States. Religion,
political ideologies, family structures tend to reinforce
those behaviors which are heterosexual in nature, and sanction
behaviors linked to homosexuality. Thus, development of
homosexual identities procure perceiving homosexual relations
as a benefit as opposed to a cost. Such a process may be
indefinite for some individuals. However, others are capable
of solidifying a homosexual identity at relatively young ages.
From an exchange perspective, establishing a homosexual
identity involves structural arrangements which enable the
homosexual to perceive a greater benefit than cost ratio in
expressing and developing homosexual identities.
Davenport (1965) has described the sexual scripts of the
Melanesians in the Southwest Pacific. Premarital intercourse
is strictly forbidden in this culture, both males and females
24
are encouraged to masturbate. In addition, all unmarried
males engage in homosexual relations with the full knowledge
and acceptance of the community. This cross-cultural case
study validates how perceived rewards and costs provide
opportunities for salient homosexual identity development.
Given the rewards in the United States are not those of the
Melanesians, it is presumed the perceived costs outweigh the
perceived rewards associated with a presented homosexual
identity.
The realization of costs associated with homosexual
identities forces gays and lesbians to develop resources which
enhance the benefits of their unique identity. As Randall
Collins suggests "the chains of interactions develop a
structure, whose profile depends on the respective levels and
types of resources possessed by participants" (Turner, 1991:
248). Such resources may include friendship networks
supportive of homosexuality, or participation in homosexual-
oriented organizations. Given the validity and relativity of
perceptions, gay love relationships may also procure salience
in homosexual identity development.
From an exchange perspective, several voluntarily and
involuntarily imposed factors explain the stability of
homosexual sexual scripts. Plummer (1979) lists ease,
pleasure, and secondary gain as factors that individuals
impose voluntarily upon themselves. Involuntarily imposed
25
factors include problems of access, lack of "in-group"
support, and public labeling as homosexual.
The ease of remaining committed to familiar patterns of
behavior and the difficulties posed by adopting new lines of
action encourage people to retain the various sexual scripts
associated with homosexuality. Randall Collins describes the
familiarity with the social role as "rituals provide payoffs,
per se, especially those critical resources revolving around
group membership" (Turner, 1991; 238). Once individuals
become stabilized in homosexual roles, they may come to view
personal costs involved in taking on bisexual or heterosexual
roles (e.g., time, energy, anxiety, and diminished sexual
arousal) as outweighing the benefits of occupying a more
conventional social status. Comfort and familiarity with
homosexual identities and roles may foster the idea that it is
"easier, more attractive, less costly to remain homosexual"
(Plummer, 1975: 50). Individuals may also chose to retain
their homosexual identities and roles because they are more
pleasurable. Lesbian and gay males have learned that the
"acts of falling, making, and being 'in love' with a member of
the same sex can be both pleasurable and satisfying" (Plummer,
1975: 151). Homosexual experience comes to be sought as an
end in itself.
Secondary gains may also lead individuals to retain
homosexual roles and identities. Clear advantages accrue to
homosexual lifestyles. Lesbian and gay male sexuality is
26
nonprocreative. Worries about pregnancy are nonexistent.
Homosexuals are also more tolerant of others labeled
"deviant," perhaps as a consequence of their own experience
with stigma (Corbett, Troiden, and Dodder, 1974). Greater
social mobility and a higher standard of living (at least for
gay males) have also been cited as advantages (Plummer, 1975).
Similar advantages, however, are open increasingly to
heterosexual cohabitators and child-free, married couples.
Factors over which an individual has little or no control
may also maintain the stability of homosexual roles and
identities over time. One involuntarily imposed factor is the
problem of access. As lesbians and gay males become older and
more entrenched in homosexual experience, they become
increasingly alienated from heterosexual experience, finding
it: "difficult to make or maintain heterosexual contacts and
increasingly disturbing to contemplate the idea of
heterosexual activity. Earlier problems of access and
identity may re-emerge in reverse if [they] should contemplate
departure from the homosexual role: the secure world is nor
the 'deviant' world, and the problematic world becomes the
'straight' world" (Plummer, 1975: 152).
The lack of in-group social support is a second
involuntarily imposed factor. Plummer (1975) contends that
homosexuals who attempt to reenter the heterosexual world
receive little or no support from homosexual friends and
acquaintances. More often than not, those who try to "go
27
straight"-that is, attempt to incorporate heterosexual sexual
scripts-are ridiculed, not taken seriously, or rejected. A
person's homosexual friends are likely to see his or her
desires as unrealistic, self-deceptive, faddish, or a form of
fence sitting. The individual is often accused of denying his
or her "true" sexual or romantic nature.
Public labeling also fosters an involuntary commitment to
the homosexual script. Public labeling and denouncement as
homosexual by official social-control agents, such as the
police or courts, although relatively infrequent, may lead to
role imprisonment; "To come before a court in a blaze of
public scandal is to be publicly ushered into a deviant role,
with very few chances of receiving official declarations of
exit" (Plummer, 1975; 152).
In summary, social, cultural, and historical forces,
rather than inherent traits or essences, shape or construct
the characteristics thought to correspond with homosexual
sexual scripts. The term homosexual describes a particular
script, an expected pattern of behavior that flows along lines
shaped by age, gender conceptions, economic arrangements,
kinships, and other concepts. It is a shorthand summary of
how a given culture at a given historical point expects
homosexually experienced people to be cognitively,
emotionally, and behavioral.
The balance of rewards and costs as extracted by
heterosexist institutions and fostered by the presences of
28
available resources distinguishes homosexual sexual scripts as
being quite significant. The homosexual sexual script is a
dynamic, cultural concept linked to the social structure.
Claude Levi-Strauss asserted "all exchange relations involve
costs for individuals, but, in contrast with economic or
psychological explanations of exchange, such costs are
attributed to society-to those customs, rules, laws, and
values" (Turner, 1991: 293). The social structure, as
depicted in the anthropological evidence, determines the cost
and benefits of acquiring the specified script. The emergence
of "being" from the "doing" infers commitment to the
designated script, despite the legacy of heterosexism embodied
in social institutions.
Organization of Dissertation
Given the predominance of heterosexual scripts, and their
relative strength in contemporary society, this dissertation
will apply the sexual script perspective to three selected
dimensions of homosexuality. This dissertation follows a
three article format. The organization of the dissertation
consists of a general literature review of the issue of
homosexual sexual scripts, three articles which apply sexual
scripts to homosexual relational dynamics, and a general
summary chapter relating each article back to the sexual
script perspective. Further, the dissertation also contains
an appendix section which presents the factor analysis results
29
of certain scales used within some of the articles. The
research conducted in articles two and three have been
approved by the Human Subjects Committee at Iowa State
University. The results of the first article is based on
secondary data anylsis.
Each article, in this dissertation, will primarily focus
on one of the three analytical levels of sexual scripts,
merged with other sociological theories (i.e., exchange,
conflict, schema, etc.) to explain the dynamics of homosexual
relationships. In the first article, sexual scripts will be
employed to inspect the disclosure process among gay and
lesbian adolescents to their parents. This article will
center on the role identification with the family affects the
extent of perceived homosexual-supportive resources,
expression of the adolescents homosexual identity, and the
disclosure of their identity. Through social exchange
analysis, the article will balance the rewards and costs
associated with homosexual sexual scripts, compared to the
heterosexual scripts implicitly presumed in family
arrangements.
The second article will contrast homosexual scripts with
heterosexual scripts, by investigating the definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality manifested in the unique
cultural scenario scripts for each group. By extrapolating on
schema theory, the study will delineate the difference between
normative and individual definitional constructs. In this
30
particular case, normative components in the definition of
attractiveness or sensuality will correspond to cultural
scenario scripts; consequently, individual-oriented
definitions will validate schematic suppositions.
The last article will examine physically and sexually
aggressive lesbian and gay relationships. Given the
institutionalized conflict between heterosexual and homosexual
scripts, heterosexual harassment of homosexuals may
circuitously lead to physical or sexual aggression in gay and
lesbians relationships. When homosexual scripts obviously
confront heterosexual scripts (i.e. being "out" of the
closet), heterosexuals will be more likely to enforce the
heterosexual script through, perhaps, violent means. Such
aggressive encounters, endured by homosexuals, may foster
learned helplessness or truculent frustration within
homosexual relationships, leading gays and lesbians
susceptible to potentially violent relationships.
31
PAPER 1. ADOLESCENT DISCLOSURE OF HOMOSEXUALITY
TO THEIR FAMILIES
32
ABSTRACT
The focus of this paper is the disclosure of homosexual
identities among adolescents, when considering their
relationships with their families. Over 170 gay adolescents
responded to a survey, addressing the expression of their
homosexual identity, the extent the adolescents identified
with their immediate family, the extent of resources they
perceived which were supportive of homosexuality, and the
degree of homosexuality disclosure to their family. The
results demonstrated identification with the family detracted
from both the extent of perceived, homosexual-supportive
resources available to gay adolescents, and the salience of
the adolescents' homosexual identity. Further, perceived,
homosexual-supportive resources and the salience of
homosexual identity increased the probability the gay
adolescents would disclose their identity to their family.
Family identification was not directly associated with
disclosure of homosexual identity to the adolescents'
families. The overall model explained more of the common
variance of disclosing to family members for males, than for
lesbians. In short, families of gay adolescents posed
barriers to homosexual identity acquisition. An exchange
analysis would suggest families reflect costs to disclosing
homosexuality; whereas, homosexual-supportive resources and
identity salience provide rewards for disclosure of a
homosexual identity.
33
INTRODUCTION
Contemporary research trends on homosexuality have
shifted from causal inquiries surrounding developmental and
life style issues. In particular, research on homosexual
identity formation has been a growing field of theoretical
analysis in recent years. Such theoretical formulations
implied sexual identities, perceptions of self as
heterosexual, homosexual, or bisexual in relation to sexual,
romantic, and broader cultural arrangements are constructed
through various forces.
The homosexual identity is one of several identities
incorporated into an individual's concept of self. According
to Cass (1984) and depending on the context, the homosexual
identity may function as a self-identity, a perceived
identity, a presented identity, or all three. A self-identity
is characterized as self realization of same-sex desires and
behaviors; however, this may not be known or revealed to
others. When individuals suspect another individual to be
gay, but this notion remains to be confirmed constitutes a
perceived identity." Finally, .a presented identity occurs when
a homosexual announces their life style or identity to others.
These general concepts readily translate into an indicator of
identity disclosure. The primary focus of this research is on
the concept of presented identity.
During the past decade, several researchers have proposed
theoretical models, attempting to explain the formation and
34
continuance of homosexual identities (Cass, 1979, 1984; Lee,
1977; Ponse, 1978; Schafer, 1976; Troiden, 1979; Weinberg,
1979). Although the various models suggested different stages
in the formation of homosexual identities, disclosure of the
homosexual identity appears to be a significant hallmark in
settling homosexual identity development.
The objective of this study was to analyze a specific
model for the disclosure of homosexual identities among gay
and lesbian adolescents to their immediate families. This
research hypothesized that disclosure of identity to the
family was predicated by the salience of the adolescents'
homosexual identity and the extent of perceived resources
serving as social support networks for the adolescents. It
was also posited the interpersonal dynamics of the family,
such as the degree the adolescent identifies with the family,
will influence both the salience of the adolescents'
homosexual identity and the breadth of perceived resources.
The second objective of this study was the consideration
and analysis of sex differences associated with each postulate
of the proposed model. The model contends structural forces,
impeding the expression of homosexual identities, will vary
for each sex, given different gender expectations for male and
female adolescents.
35
Gay and lesbian youths
Very little is empirically known about the life style of
gay and lesbian youths. Theoretical analysis of homosexual
identities suggested the period of adolescence is critical for
the development of a positive gay self-image (Troiden, 1989).
Further, Dank (1972) asserted "coming out" and disclosure may
be occurring at younger ages, and will continue to do so, in
the United States. This is due, in part, to an increased
emphasis and acceptance of sexuality in general, tolerance of
homosexual behavior, and availability of information
concerning homosexuality (Dank, 1972).
Evidence that homosexual identities and homosexual
behaviors (including disclosure) are occurring at younger ages
is reflected in the ramifications of adolescent homosexuality.
The recent impact of AIDS on the gay community may force
disclosure of homosexuality among sexually active gay
adolescents. There is disturbing evidence that the dramatic
rise of teen-age, male prostitution in the United States is
partially due to the intolerance of homosexuality by their
parents (Bales, 1985). Further, the negative consequences of
"coming out" during adolescence may also be mirrored in the
distinct suicide rates plaguing gay and lesbian youth
(Hollinger, 1978; Kournay, 1987).
Other significant problems associated with gay and
lesbian youth relate to identity formation and development.
According to Erikson (1963), identity formation is the most
36
salient developmental task during adolescence. Pressure to
resolve a heterosexual identity from external social forces,
and the internal need to foster homo-erotic drives can be
overwhelming. Maylon (1982) noted such identity confusion can
induce low self-esteem, depression, denial, suppression, and
compartmentalization in homosexual adolescents. To increase
the intensity of the trauma, Maylon (1981) asserted gay and
lesbian adolescents usually do not have full access to gay and
lesbian resources or communities, given the adult-oriented
nature of the communities and resources, which may foster
alienation among adolescents (see also Martin, 1982).
Disclosure and family dynamics
Disclosure of sexual orientation to the family of origin
is, needless to say, a traumatic experience for both the
adolescent and nonhomosexual family members. To date, the
analysis of family reactions to disclosure of homosexual
identities has been dominated by parental responses. Weinberg
(1972) and Jones (1978) concluded parental reactions to
disclosure tended to be quite negative, and consisted of
primarily two characteristics; (1) negative conceptions of
the homosexual identity, and (2) distinct feelings of failure
and guilt associated with their parental role. The child is
often severed from familial ties due to misconceptions about
homosexuality, thus parents may react to the disclosure by
treating the child as a stranger or as a stigmatized.
37
stereotyped entity (Collins and Zimmerman, 1983; Devine, 1984;
Fairchild and Hayward, 1979; Strommen, 1989). Collins and
Zimmerman (1983) and Devine (1984) asserted structural,
regulative, cohesive, and thematic issues particular to the
family guide the parental reaction to the child's disclosure.
One central theme apparent in numerous writings on the subject
is that a positive relationship with parents prior to
disclosure is a good indicator of a healthy resolution
(Borhek, 1983; Fairchild and Hayward, 1979; Silverstein, 1977;
Weinberg, 1972).
Familial factors affecting identity formation or
disclosure of identity have not been confirmed. For example,
birth order has been sufficiently researched, yet has
maintained contradictory findings, regarding the etiology of
homosexual identity formation. Grundlach (1977) concluded
birth order and family atmosphere influenced the disclosure
process among lesbians. However, Perkins (1978) ascertained
from her study of 212 lesbians birth order, and, specifically
being an only child, were not significantly related to a
lesbian identity. Siegelman (1973) asserted birth order and
family size were not significantly distinct between male and
female homosexuals or heterosexuals. In general, the most
accurate empirical generalization regarding the link between
homosexual disclosure of their identity to their family of
origin was the unpredictability associated with familial
response (Borhek, 1983).
38
The role of siblings in the disclosure process has been
recently introduced as a role relationship potentially
affecting homosexual identity formation. However, there has
been little discussion on the particular effects of disclosure
to siblings. Jones (1978) reported siblings tend to respond
to disclosure in a similar fashion as parents. Siblings view
the disclosing sibling as a stranger, or as a stigmatized
entity; however, siblings do not suffer the feelings of guilt
and blame as do parents.
The most significant research, pertaining to the
disclosure process of adolescents to family members, has
integrated not only structural concepts (such as family
structure), but also psychological and interactional
correlates. Savin-Williams' study of 317 gay and lesbian
college students and adolescents demonstrated the significance
of parents and self-evaluation for the "coming-out" process
(1989). Satisfying personal relationships and younger parents
were found to be good predictors for lesbians to disclose
their identity to their parents; however, this was not the
case for males. Further, self-esteem of the youth was
considered as a mild predictor of disclosure. Although the
study was most noteworthy, given the skewed age distribution
of the sample, the findings may not be generalizable to those
adolescents under the age of eighteen, and still residing with
their parents.
39
It is evident further research is needed to examine the
role of internal and structural components influencing the
disclosure process of gay and lesbian youth to their family
origin. It is also necessary to place the role of the family
in context with other structural components which may affect
the salience of an adolescent's homosexual identity.
40
THEORETICAL ANALYSIS
The proposed model in this study focuses on the
correlates which determine the salience of homosexual identity
and identity disclosure, despite structural resistance and
adversity to such identity progression. The model was based
on several theoretical premises, all of which posit homosexual
identity is a negotiated identity, internally and externally
between the self and structural arrangements. It is believed
identity disclosure is partially dependent on environmental
influences and the extent of perceived resources which either
push or pull the individual toward/away from expressing a
salient identity, and disclosure of the identity to family
systems. The model asserts, in part, that disclosure is a
cognitive process; however, the articulation and expression of
the identity (or salience and disclosure of the identity) is
negotiated, involving perceived costs and rewards linked to
structural arrangements. The model is theoretically founded
by both social exchange and sexual script perspectives.
According to social exchange theory, conceptualization of
identities can be expressed by the psychic profit associated
with a cost-benefit ratio. Those behaviors or identities
which incur the greatest rewards or benefits will increase the
salience of a particular behavior or identity complex. Those
identities or behaviors which entail the greatest costs will
decrease the salience of identity constructs or the
probability of retaining certain behaviors.
41
Benefits and costs are decidedly skewed toward
heterosexual relations in the United States. Religion,
political ideologies, family structures tend to reinforce
heterosexual behaviors and identities, and sanction
behaviors/identities linked to homosexuality. Thus,
development and disclosure of homosexual identities procure
perceiving homosexual relations as a benefit as opposed to a
cost. Such a process may be indefinite for some individuals.
However, others are capable of solidifying a homosexual
identity at relatively young ages.
The means of distributing the cost and benefits attached
to sexual orientations are mediated by sexual scripts. Sexual
scripts represent sexual articulations by culture,
interpersonal relationships, and cognitive desires. Scripts
are mental trajectories, a cognitive view of official beliefs
(cultural scenario scripts), relational meanings and
expectations (interpersonal scripts), and sexual fantasies
(intrapsychic scripts). Sexual scripts designate cognitive
and affective limits, indicating appropriate and inappropriate
sexual behaviors (Gagnon and Simon, 1973).
In general, cultural scenario scripts encourage
heterosexuality. The media, religious forces, and society,
overall, dictate a heterosexist norm prevailing through the
embodiment of scripts geared to opposite-sex relations.
Because cultural scenario scripts value heterosexual relations
over homosexual relations, internalizing these official views
42
negatively impacts on homosexual identity formation.
Interpersonal scripts associated with family arrangements also
discourage homosexuality, as documented in the literature.
Given the prevalence of a heterosexual norm among cultural
scenario and interpersonal scripts, homosexual adolescents
must undergo a resocialization process. This resocialization
process involves the formation and expression of a salient
homosexual identity despite relational and structural
constraints.
Figure l illustrates the postulated model of this
research project. The model predicts that a basic structural
and relational arrangement, the family, will influence the
salience of homosexual identities among adolescents, and the
disclosure of their identities to their family. The family
serves as a primary socializing agent which potentially
reinforces the heterosexual norm, and sanctions homosexual
development. The model posits strong identification with the
family of origin will impede the extent of homosexual-
supportive resources perceived by lesbian and gay adolescents
(Arrow A). Given the heterosexual orientation of families, in
general, the structure of these families will limit the
perception of gay adolescents toward viable resources,
supportive of developing a homosexual identity. Further, the
parents of gay and lesbian adolescents in all probability will
not avail resources to their children, assuming their children
to be heterosexual. It is also hypothesized that family
43
identification will negatively impact on the expression of a
homosexual identity (Arrow B). Although past research has
found a positive correlation between family relationships and
acceptance of gay youth, family identification may not be a
significant predictor of disclosure to the family. Though a
positive relationship between the adolescent and their parents
may be conducive to disclosure, given the dissenting sanctions
for homosexuality, adolescents may be reluctant to disclose
their identity to their families for fear of being sanctioned.
It is hypothesized perceived supportive resources will foster
the expression of a homosexual identity among gay adolescents
(Arrow C). Such resources as gay organizations, supportive
friendships and relationships, counselors, etc. should, in
theory, facilitate the process of identity acquisition versus
identity confusion. In short, the resources should pull the
adolescent toward a positive gay self-image, and push the
adolescent away from the fear of parental sanctioning.
Figure 1 about here
There is little past research to speculate on the
relationship between family identification and the decision
regarding disclosure of gay identities. Therefore, it is
hypothesized an exchange occurs between the "pulls" and
"pushes" of the social structure on disclosure of sexual
orientation. The model asserts gay adolescents weigh the
44
perceived costs associated with disclosing a potentially
stigmatized identity to their families (Arrow D), with the
benefits related to perceived supportive resources empowering
the salience and disclosure of gay identities (Arrow E).
Finally, the model maintains homosexual identity expression
will positively influence the extent of disclosure among gay
adolescents to their parents (Arrow F). Because the final
stage of identity formation, in the literature, suggests
commitment to the gay identity, it is hypothesized disclosure
is a sufficient component of commitment, and will entail
disclosure to the adolescents' primary groups (i.e. the
family).
45
METHODS
Sample
The data for this study were obtained from a
questionnaire administered to self-identified gay adolescents,
ranging in age from 14 to 18. The sample originated from a
clinical-based, support group designed to provide resources
for gay and lesbian youth dealing with personal issues
pertinent to their particular life style. The support group
initially involved 35 adolescents. Through snowball sampling,
the adolescents involved in the support group were asked to
administer the survey to their personal network of friends and
acquaintances. A pre-addressed, postage-free envelope was
included with the questionnaire. No identifying marks were
placed on either the questionnaire or the envelope to ensure
confidentiality. A cover letter was also attached to the
questionnaire indicating the purpose of the study and means
undertaken by the research staff to assure anonymity for those
who might feel uncomfortable with their responses or with
others knowing of their participation in the project. The
final sample consisted of 172 self-identified gays and
lesbians, with 85 females and 87 males. The majority of
respondents, 95%, were from a suburban, upper East coast
community. The families of the adolescents were primarily
middle-class, with fairly stable incomes and professional
46
occupations (61%). Further, the family structures of the
families were substantially dual earner, intact families
(78%).
Measurement
In order to operationalize a questionnaire in terms
familiar with gay adolescent needs and comprehension,
consultation with the gay and lesbian support group assisted
in the development of wording for several items on the
questionnaire. It should be noted many of the responses on
the questionnaire rely on the perceptions of the adolescents.
Further, many of the indices consisted of a single indicator;
consequently, single indicator concepts are generally
considered problematic regarding validity and reliability.
Several researchers support perceptual measures as valid
indices of measurement (Acock and Bengston, 1980; Gecas and
Schawlbe, 1986; Kerckoff and Huff, 1974).
Identification with Family. The extent the gay youth
identified with their family was measured by the following
question: "Before you realized (or suspected) you were gay,
how well did you get along with your family?" Respondents
chose one of five responses, with 1="I have never related to
my family", 2="I did not get along with my family to well",
3="I could somewhat relate to my family", 4="I got along with
my family fairly well", and 5="l related to my family very
well."
47
Perceived Resources (Homosexual Social Support Networks).
The extent of perceived resources, functioning as social
support networks, was assessed by asking the respondents to
approximately indicate (to the best of their immediate
knowledge) how many friends, support groups, family members,
organizations, and acquaintances they believed to be
supportive of homosexuals. The adolescents selected one of
the following five response categories: l="0-5", 2="6=10",
3="11-15", 4="16-20", and 5="21 or more."
Disclosure of Identity to Family. Disclosure of gay
identities was ascertained by the degree of knowledge the
adolescents' parents were cognizant of their child's sexual
orientation. The adolescents chose one of five responses to
complete the following statement: "My parents ..." The
response categories ranged from l="have no idea that I may be
gay.", 2="raay suspect me to be gay.", 3="have asked me if I
was gay.", 4="have been told that I am gay, but do not believe
it.", and 5="are fully aware that I am gay."
Expression of Homosexual Identity. The degree the
adolescents' homosexual identity had been enacted upon was
assessed by five questions. The first two questions
ascertained participation and importance of homosexual-
oriented organizations. Respondents were asked to endorse one
of five response categories to the question: "During the past
month, how many gay organizations, groups, or meetings have
you attended?" The responses ranged from 1="0", 2="1", 3="2",
48
4="3" and 5="4 or more." It should be noted frequency of
attendance and defined importance of gay-oriented
organizations are not synonymous. Given extenuating
circumstances within such a stigmatized population,
participation in gay organizations may not always be
commensurate with desired level of attendance. Therefore,
importance of such organizations, as perceived by the
adolescents, may be indicative of participation if homosexual
founded organizations were readily available in the gay
community. Importance of gay-oriented organizations were
assessed by a 5-point Likert scale in response to the
question: "How important are gay organizations, groups, and
meetings to you?" The respondents were asked to select one of
the following categories: l="not important", 2="mildly
important", 3="fairly important", 4="very important", and
5="extremely important."
The third question on the expression of homosexual
identity scale ascertained the importance of homosexual
friends to the gay and lesbian adolescents. Respondents were
asked to select one of five possible responses to the
following question: "How important are gay friends to you?"
The response categories included: l="not important",
2="mildly important", 3="fairly important", 4="very
important", and 5="extremely important."
The last two questions on the expression of homosexual
identity scale assessed gay sexual relations among the
49
adolescents. Frequency and Importance of gay sexual activity
served as the indicators for sexual identity.
The adolescents' responses on the five items were summed
to comprise the scale, objectively measuring the expression of
their homosexual identity formation and development. The
scale produced a possible range of scores from 5 to 25.
Higher scores on the scale were indicative of strong gay
identity expression and formation, lower scores on the scale
suggested suspicion, nonexpression of the identity, or
identity confusion. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for the
expression of homosexual identity scale was .77.
Procedures
Statistical analysis of the data consisted of several
techniques to distinguish significant associations between the
variables used in this analysis. First, a Pearson correlation
matrix of zero-ordered correlations was computed for both male
and female adolescents. Second, path analysis was implemented
to test the hypothesized associations of the proposed model
regarding the influence of the family on gay and lesbian
adolescents. The utilization of path analysis was considered
as a statistical technique to evaluate the strength of the
bonds between key variables, while supplying information
toward causal constructs, given the theoretical disposition of
the model. The variables constituting the formation of the
path model included: identification with family, perceived
50
homosexual-supportive resources, salience of homosexual
identity, and disclosure of identity to the adolescents'
family. To reiterate, the model posits disclosure of identity
to the adolescent's family is contingent upon the extent the
adolescent identifies with their family, as mediated by the
salience of their homosexual identity and the extent of
perceived homosexual-supportive resources available to them.
Third, LISREL VII (Joreskog and Sorbom, 1989) was used to
determine the significance of direct and indirect effects for
each of the preceding variables on disclosure.
51
RESULTS
The zero-ordered correlation coefficients for all the
variables in the study (refer to Table 1), provided strong
evidence that structural factors do impede the extent
homosexual-supportive resources are perceived by adolescents,
expression of homosexual identities among gay adolescents, and
whether or not adolescents disclosed their identities to
immediate family members. For lesbians, identification with
family was significantly correlated with the dependent
variables in the study. For gay males, identification with
family was significantly associated with perceived homosexual-
supportive resources, salience of homosexual identity, and
disclosure of identity to their family.
Table 1 about here
The results of the path model (see Figure 2) indicated
identification with family was significantly negatively
correlated with the extent of perceived resources or social
support networks available for both gay and lesbian
adolescents (males; beta=-.472, p<.001; females: beta=-.385,
p<.001). The degree of identification the adolescent had with
their family negatively affected the amount of resources
perceived by the adolescent, assisting in the development of a
positive gay self-image. A strong identification with family
impeded or detracted from perceiving the availability of
52
resources, congruent with homosexual-oriented support
networks. Identification with their family explained more of
the total common variance in perceived resources for gay
adolescents than for lesbian adolescents (males: R^=.223;
females: R^=.148).
Figure 2 about here
Identification with the family and the extent of
perceived resources available for both gay and lesbian
adolescents appeared to be strong predictors of the expression
of homosexual identities. The stronger the identification
with the adolescents' families, the less the homosexual
identity was expressed for both males and females (males:
beta=-.631, p<.001; females; beta=-.570, p<.001). In other
words, identification with the family and expression of
homosexual identities for gay and lesbian adolescents were
incongruent. The family inhibits the gay youth from
expressing homosexual relationships and associating in the gay
community. The extent of perceived homosexual-supportive
resources positively contributed to expression of homosexual
identities among gay and lesbian youth (males; beta=.431,
p<.00l; females: beta=.392, p<.001). The more resources,
perceived by both gay and lesbian adolescents which were
considered as supportive to their homosexual identity, the
more their identity as a homosexual was expressed.
53
Identification with the family and perceived homosexual-
supportive resources explained approximately one-half of the
total common variance associated with a salient homosexual
identity for both gay and lesbian adolescents (males:
R^=.482; females; R^=.421).
Identification with family was not significantly
associated with the extent gay adolescents disclosed their
identities to their parents; however, both expression of their
identities and perceived resources did positively affect
disclosure. Expression of homosexual identity was the
strongest predictor of disclosure to parents for both gay and
lesbian adolescents (males: beta=.479, p<.001; females:
beta=.253, p<.05). The extent of perceived, supportive
resources was also positively related to identity disclosure
for both males and females (males: beta=.311, p<.01; females:
beta=.261, p<.05). Given, the significance of family
identification on perceived resources and salient identity
expression, family identification mediated the nature of
disclosure to parents. Therefore, the model suggests
perceived social support and expression of identities may be
more responsible for disclosure than the endurance of family
identification among gay adolescents. The model also implied
perceived resources may be more predictive to disclosure than
expression of gay identities among lesbians. And, salience of
identity appeared to maintain a stronger effect on disclosure
than perceived resources for the males in this study. The
54
proposed model explained twice the total common variance among
gay males than for lesbians (males: R^=.540; females:
1^^.257).
Table 2 about here
A test of the significance for indirect effects provided
intriguing, if not compelling results. Although modest,
identification with family significantly, yet indirectly,
affected the expression of the adolescent's homosexual
identity (males: beta=-.210; females: beta=-.157).
Identification with family significantly and indirectly
decreased the probability that gay adolescents would disclose
his or her identity to their family (males: beta=-.449;
females: beta=-.236). The extent of perceived resources, by
the adolescent, affected the disclosure process indirectly
(males: beta=.213; females: beta=.098). This trend was
significant only for males. Therefore, family identification
did maintain an indirect effect on the disclosure process;
however, this process is mediated by the extent of perceived
resources and the expression of a homosexual identity among
adolescents.
55
DISCUSSION
Identification with parents detracted from the extent of
perceived homosexual-supportive resources among gay
adolescents. Further, identification with the family and
perceived resources significantly predicted the expression, or
salience of gay identities. Finally, social resources and
salience of identity positively affected the disclosure of
identity among adolescents to their parents. Identification
with the family did not directly affect whether or not the gay
adolescent disclosed their identity to their family; however,
family identification is mediated by perceived resources and
the expression of homosexual identities.
The model explained more of the common variance for males
than for females. In general, the tested model maintained
twice the explanatory power (R^) for the gay males when
contrasted with the lesbian adolescents. Perhaps, the
expression of identity scale was more conducive toward
assessing male sexuality and identity than for females. The
slight gender difference could also be a result of community
differences. Theoretically, in the gay male community,
community networks and resources tend to be highly saturated
with sexually schematic themes. In the lesbian community, the
resources involve more intimate friendship-based networks.
Therefore, the measurement of perceived resources and identity
expression may be more valid and reliable indicators for gay
males than for lesbians. The findings of this study could
56
also be the result of structural versus internal negotiations.
Perhaps, for males external, structural forces are more
influential than internal motivations; whereas, for lesbians,
the process may be more defined among internal forces (i.e.,
self-esteem, self-efficacy, etc.).
Disclosure of a gay identity among adolescents entails a
resocialization process, whereby adolescents must secure those
resources which facilitate homosexual scripts over the
crystallized heterosexual scripts indoctrinated by parents.
In short, disclosure is a means of securing enough social
supportive resources to counter the heterosexual norm
maintained through familial interactions. By exploring and
utilizing homosexual-oriented resources, adolescents procure
the means possible to establish a homosexual identity, and by
the same means, initiate a resocialization conducive to
homosexual scripts. After an indefinite period of time,
adolescents become familiar with the relatively new scripts
and, in turn, nourish the continuity of their identity as a
gay individual. As the formation of their new identity comes
to completion, no longer are the adolescents necessarily
dependent on the heterosexual scripts fostered by the family.
When the balance of support provided by the gay community
exceeds the possible rejection or sanctions will the
adolescent disclose their identity to their families.
Homosexual networks and scripts empower adolescents, and
57
adults alike, with confidence and support to disclose their
identity to loved ones.
58
CONCLUSION
The data supported a generalized exchange between
homosexual-supportive resources, salience of identity, and
family identification. Disclosure of sexual identity can be
conceptualized as an exchange system, where the costs of
disclosing a homosexual identity to parents was weighed
against resources supporting the disclosure process. The
impact of heterosexual scripts, via family identification,
reduced the amount of perceived resources available to gay and
lesbian adolescents. Further, identification with the
heterosexual scripts, abundant in the family, detracted from
the expression or salience of acquiring a homosexual identity.
However, family identification did not directly influence
whether gay and lesbian adolescents disclosed their identity
to the parents. Yet, the extent of supportive resources and
the expression of identity were strong predictors of the
disclosure process.
Sexual scripts mediate the exchange of heterosexual and
homosexual rewards and costs. Given the dominating influence
of heterosexual scripts, disclosure becomes a process of
acquiring resources supportive of homosexuality, and
expressing a salient identity based on the perception of such
resources. When the extent of perceived resources and the
internalization of the support, acquired through the
resources, provides more rewards than the associated costs of
risking family identification, then disclosure becomes viable
59
for the adolescent. In short, adolescents are more likely to
disclose their identity to their parents, when the resources
are available to support the potentially negative consequences
associated with disclosure.
60
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Acock, A., and Bengston, V. (1980). "The socialization and
attribution processes: Actual versus perceived
similarity among parents and youth." Journal of Marriage
and the Family^ 42, 501-515.
Bales, J. (1985). "Gay adolescents' pain compounded." APA
Monitor, 16(12), 21.
Borhek, M. (1983). Coming Out to Parents. New York, NY:
Pilgrim Press.
Cass, V. (1979). "Homosexual identity formation: A
theoretical model." Journal of Homosexuality, 4(3),
219-35.
Cass, V. (1984). "Homosexual identity formation: Testing a
theoretical model." Journal of Sex Research, 20(2), 143-
67.
Collins, L., and Zimmerman, N. (1983). "Homosexual and
bisexual issues." In J. Hansen, J. Woody, and R. Woody
(Eds.), Sexual Issues in Family Therapy, (pp. 82-100).
Rockville, MD: Aspen Publications.
Dank, B. (1972). "Why homosexuals marry heterosexual women."
Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality, 6, 14-23.
DeVine, J. (1984). "A systemic inspection of affectional
preference orientation and the family origin." Journal
of Social Work and Human Sexuality, 2, 9-17.
Erikson, E. (1963). childhood and Society. New York:
Norton.
Fairchild, B., and Hayward, N. (1979). Now That You Know:
What Every Parent Should Know About Homosexuality. New
York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, and Jovanovich.
Gagnon, J., and Simon, W. (1973). Sexual Conduct: The
Social Sources of Human Sexuality. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Gecas, v., and Schwalbe, M. (1986). "Parental behavior and
adolescent self-esteem." Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 48, 37-46.
Grundlach, R. (1977). "Sibling size, sibsex, and
homosexuality among females." Transnational Mental
Health Newsletter, 19(1), 3-7.
6l
Jones, C. (1978). Understanding Gay Relatives and Friends.
New York, NY; Seabury.
Joreskog, K. M. and Sorbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A Guide to
the Program and Applications. Chicago, IL; SPSS, Inc.
Kerkoff, A., and Huff, J. (1974). "Parental influence on
educational goals." Sociometry, 37, 307-327.
Kourany, R. (1987). "Suicide among gay adolescents."
Journal of Homosexuality, 13(4), 111-17.
Lee, J. (1977). "Going public: A study in the sociology of
homosexual liberation." Journal of Homosexuality, 3(1),
49-78.
Martin, D. (1982). "Learning to hide: The socialization of
the gay adolescent." In Feinstein, Looney, Schwartzberg,
& Sorosky (Eds.), Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, (pp.
52-65). Chicago, Illinopis: University of Chicago
Press.
Maylon, A. (1981). "The homosexual adolescent; Development
isuues and social bias." Child Welfare, 60, 321-30.
Maylon, A. (1982). "Biphasic aspects of homosexual identity
formation." Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and
Practice, 19, 335-40.
Perkins, M. (1978). "On birth order among lesbians."
Psychological Reports, 43, 814.
Ponse, B. (1978). Identities in the Lesbian World: The
Social Construction of Self. Westport Connecticut:
Greenwood Press.
Savin-Williams, R. (1989). "Coming out to parents and self-
esteem among gay and lesbian youths." Journal of
Homosexuality, 18(1/2), 1-35.
Scahfer, S. (1976). "Sexual and social problems among
lesbians." Journal of Homosexuality, 12( 1),-5.0-69.
Siegelman, M. (1973). "Birth order and family size of
homosexual men and women." Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 41, 164.
Silverstein, C. (1977). A Family Matter: A Parent's Guide
to Homosexuality. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
62
Strommen, E. (1989). "'You're a what?': Family member
reactions to the disclosure of homosexuality." Journal
of Homosexuality, 18(1/2), 37-58.
Troiden, R. (1979). "Becoming homosexual: A model of gay
identity acquisition." Psychiatry, 142(4), 362-73.
Troiden, R. (1989). Gay and Lesbian Idneity: A Sociological
Analysis. Dix Hills: General Hall, Inc.
Weinberg, G. (1972). Society and The Healthy Homosexual.
New York, NY: St. Martin's Press.
Weinberg, T. (1979). "On 'doing' and 'being' gay; Sexual
behavior and homosexual male self-identity." Journal of
Homosexuality, 4, 143-56.
63
Expression of
Homosexual Identity
/
Identification
with Family
Disclosure
of Identity
Perceived Homosexual-
Supportive Resources
Figure 1. Proposed model illustrating the effect of family
identification on disclosure of identity, mediated
by the extent of perceived supportive resources and
identity expression
64
Table 1. Correlation matrix for the variables in the study
among both gay males and lesbians
1. 2. 3. 4.
1. Identification —.472 -.631 -.471
with Family
2. Extent of Supportive -.385 .643 .630
Resources
3. Expression of -.570 .566 X .693
Identity
4. Disclosure of -.340 .431 .445
Identity to Parents
Mean
(males) 3.14 2.33 14.43 2.58
(lesbians) 3.09 2.60 12.92 2.28
Standard Deviation
(males) .11 .13 .47 .15
(lesbians) .13 .14 .44 .14
Note: coefficients for lesbians appear below the diagonal.
•V
Table 2. Decomposition of effects for the proposed model by
gay males and lesbians
TOTAL DIRECT INDIRECT
Hales Females Males Females Males Females
Perceived Resources
Family Identification -.472 -.385 -.472 -.385 .000 .000
Expression of Identity
Family Identification -.631 -.570 -.421 -.413 -.210* -.157*
Perceived Resources .444 .407 .444 .407 .000 .000
Pisploswe gf Identity
Family Identification -.471 -.340 -.022 -.104 -.449* -.236*
Perceived Resources .525 .352 .307 .254 .213* .098
Expression of Identity .479 .242 .479 .242 .000 .000
* indirect effect significant at the .05 level.
66
/r= .551 (.466)
Expression of
Homosexual Identity
- .421»(- .413*)
Identification \ .479» (.242*)
with Family .444» (.407»)
- .022(- .104)
\ Disclosure
.472*(- 385*)
of Identity
.312» (.254^)^,.'
\
«2=.539 (.253)
Perceived Homosexual-
Supportive Resources
/?2= .223 (.148)
Note: Standardized regression coefficients for lesbian
adolescents appear in parentheses.
* p value significant at the .05 level.
Figure 2. The impact of family identification on disclosure
of identity to the family among gay and lesbian
adolescents
67
PAPER 2. THE DEFINITION OF ATTRACTIVENESS AND SENSUALITY:
A SCHEMATIC COMPARISON BETWEEN HOMOSEXUALS AND HETEROSEXUALS
68
ABSTRACT
The objective of this study was to develop the
relationship between sexual scripts and schema toward the
definition of attractiveness and sensuality. By contrasting
the definitions of attractiveness and sensuality among gay
males and heterosexual females, and heterosexual males and
lesbians; gender and sexual orientation differences could be
identified, regarding conceptual definitions of
attractiveness. Content analysis was employed to probe the
differences as they relate to schematic and
cultural/subcultural influences. For the study, two data sets
were utilized. The first data set included over 300 gays and
lesbians. The second data set consisted of 400 Iowa state
University college students. In general, heterosexual females
and homosexual males shared similar definitions of
attractiveness; however, the two groups did not share similar
definitions of sensuality. Among lesbians and heterosexual
males did not share any significant similarities in
definitions of attractiveness or sensuality.
69
INTRODUCTION
Several metaphors have been constructed to designate
general connotations attached to the definition of beauty.
For example, the popular remark "beauty is in the eye of the
beholder," suggests the determinants of beauty are personal,
individual-oriented. "The face of an angel," and "what is
beautiful is good" reflect beneficent personality standards in
the definition of attractiveness. Further, a statement such
as "clothes make a man," incurs subliminal concepts of social
and gender stratification which may play an integral part in
defining beauty. Does culture, the values and norms embedded
within the United States, influence definitions of
attractiveness? Is sensuality linked to the definition of
attractiveness, or is sensuality a markedly different concept
than beauty? And, is the definition of attractiveness and
sensuality composed of different traits based on sexual
orientation differences?
The definition of attractiveness and sensuality employed
in this research is exploratory. The definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality are, by nature, the subject'of
this article. Therefore to define both terms would be
premature. However, the notion of attractiveness in this
research projects connotes general aesthetics appeal. And
sensuality was preconceived as those characteristics which
incur sexual desire.
70
The purpose of this study was to analyze the various
components in the definition of attractiveness and sensuality
among heterosexual females when compared to the responses of
homosexual males, and heterosexual males when compared to
lesbians. The objectives to be accomplished by such research
were focus on (1) normative versus individual-oriented means
of definition construction for sensuality and attractiveness;
and (2) the sexual schematic comparisons among heterosexual
females and homosexual males, and heterosexual males and
lesbians regarding specific traits implicit in defining what
is attractive and what is sexy regarding both males and
females.
71
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
Numerous researchers have conducted studies demonstrating
the importance of being attractive in the United States. The
commonality emerging among these studies concur that
attractive individuals receive preferential treatment. The
effects of facial appearance influences the context of dating
(see Krebs and Aldinolfi, 1975); marriage (Nida and Williams,
1977); politics (Efran and Patterson, 1974); employment
(Waters, 1985); and criminality (Âgnew, 1984). The role of
attractiveness is an important element in social life. The
effects of attractiveness are uncontested by this study;
however, what remains more of a mystery are our perceptions
about the definition of attractiveness.
Objective versus subjective definitions of attractiveness
Patzer (1985) has criticized many research efforts
concerning the effect of attractiveness for inadequate
definitions of attractiveness employed in their research
designs. Many researchers have assumed a common, perhaps
universal, definition of attractiveness (Touhey, 1979;
Sprecher, 1989; Gladue and Delaney, 1990). Such studies have
defined what is attractive by varying means, usually
subjective assessment strategies made by a designated research
team. Consequently, these studies inferred the definition of
attractiveness is similar for all individuals. Such
assumptions are qualified and legitimated by theorizing that
72
normative rules guide the definition of attractiveness. In
other words, attractiveness is not in the eye of the beholder,
it remains in the eye of the culture.
Research on subjectivity and intersubjective agreement
(objectivity), regarding the definition of attractiveness, has
only recently become a concern among attractiveness
investigators. Downs (1990) concluded "interjudge agreement
concerning target subjects' levels of attractiveness is
generally high, which suggests that attractiveness is both
scaleable and culturally-defined" (p. 458). Further, Udry
(1965) found interjudge agreement on female faces tended to be
quite reliable. However, Patzer (1985) suggested the
definition of attractiveness is not a quantitative element.
Patzer contended the definition of attractiveness is a
subjective concept, which eludes codification. Lucker and
colleagues (1981) asserted attractiveness is composed of
objective (empirically measured) criteria and a subjective
element. Therefore, definitions of attractiveness vary
empirically as an objective element; and, according to some
theorists, definitions also rely on a subjective, unmeasurable
quality unique to individuals.
Objective criteria of attractiveness
If attractiveness can be theoretically and empirically
defined at a scaleable level, what constitutes the cultural or
objective definition of attractiveness? In the past, many
73
studies have focused solely on the face in their attempt to
define attractiveness. A study conducted by Alicke, Smith,
and Klotz (1986) argued the body of an individual may be as
equally important as their face in defining attractiveness.
Another study by Franzoi and Herzog (1987) determined women
and men concur buttocks, eyes, legs, and health are
significant features in defining male attractiveness. Women
were also more inclined to consider body scent and physical
stamina as important determinants in judging male
attractiveness.
Other studies have yielded similar findings involving the
male body. Symons (1987) concluded females were more
receptive of moderate-sized men. This finding was supported
by Horvath (1981), asserting moderately broad shoulders were
attractive. Studies conducted in the 1970's revealed similar
premises about the size and muscularity of males, regarding
the definition of attractiveness. Lavrakas (1975) and
Graziano, Brothen, and Bersheid (1978) affirmed a medium-sized
stature were more appealing in defining male attractiveness
among women.
Facial qualities as a determinant for attractiveness has
received greater scientific evaluation. Therefore, the
research on facial characteristics tend to be more advanced
and sophisticated than research conducted on the male body.
Analyses of the male face have proposed maturity, dominance,
social status, and sociability are distinguishing dimensions
74
which increase attractiveness in males (Keating, 1985;
Cunningham, Barbee, and Pike, 1990). In other words, male
facial characteristics corresponding to such personality
qualities tend to connote higher levels of attractiveness.
According to Keating (1985), male faces possessing a square
jaw and thin lips were more likely to be defined as dominating
and attractive. Cunningham et al. (1990) reported large eyes,
prominent cheekbones, a large chin, high-status clothing, and
a big smile were significantly associated with male
attractiveness. Both of these studies suggest not only
physical features are conducive in defining what is attractive
in males, but also assert personality characteristics and
social class may be equally as important in defining
attractiveness.
According to Furnham, Hester, and Weir (1990), males
prefer women possessing large breasts and an hourglass figure
(see also Gitter, Lomranz, Saxe, and Bar-Tal, 1983). Franzoi
and Herzog (1987) concluded body scent, waist, thighs, body
build, buttocks, breasts, eyes, face, and weight were
designated as important features for males defining female
attractiveness. Keating (1985) discovered a strong gender
stereotype in males' ratings of female facial characteristics.
The males in her study delineated immature, and nondominant
facial features as being attractive. Kulik and Harackiewicz
(1979) asserted males found feminine females as being more
attractive than androgynous females; however, males drifted
75
toward androgynous females for companionship rather than
romance. Feinman and Gill (1978) suggested females with
lighter coloration (complexion, hair, eyes) tended to be rated
more attractive among male respondents.
Gender differences
Research studies on attractiveness between men and women
have demonstrated convincing evidence for substantial gender
differences. According to Jackson and associates (1986),
women were more concerned and less satisfied with their
appearance than men. Rand and Hall (1983) concluded men and
women were not congruent in judgements of their own
attractiveness. The researchers argued females were more
susceptible to the cultural norms which based female
attractiveness as more relevant compared to males. The most
thorough analysis of gender differences and attractiveness
stems from the meta-analytic review conducted by Feingold
(1990). Feingold reviewed five research paradigms which
yielded similar findings: men place greater emphasis on
attractiveness than females.
Studies assessing gender differences in what males and
females define or judge to be attractive have illuminated
further theoretical considerations regarding attractiveness.
Keating (1985) discovered mature features were linked to
judging men as attractive, while female faces were considered
more attractive when the face exhibited immature and
76
nondominant features. Feinman and Gill (1978) investigated
attractiveness preferences among college students, resolved a
greater desire for lighter coloration by males, and a stronger
preference for darker coloration by females. Kulik and
Harackiewicz (1979) researched gender identities as a
dimension of attractiveness. They found that females
preferred androgynous male partners, while males favored
feminine women for romantic involvements. The summation of
the research suggested interesting gender trends with how
males and females define attractiveness. Of particular
interest was the influence of personality (gender traits) with
definitions of attractiveness, which had been somewhat
neglected in past research. Further, past studies have
neglected the role of sexual orientation on delineating the
definition of attractiveness along gender lines.
77
A SCHEMATIC ANALYSIS
Although there has been substantial research on
attractiveness, the methodologies employed by some studies may
be considered as problematic for several theoretical reasons.
First, few of the studies which link attractiveness to
preferential treatment have neglected actually defining the
concept of attractiveness. Rather, the investigators have
assumed a commonality between their personal assessment of
attractiveness and the subjects. Such studies have fostered a
fairly strong, perhaps unreasonable, assumption in their
studies: everyone has the same definition of attractiveness.
Second, many of the studies conducted on attractiveness have
neglected a personality dimension to the definition of
attractiveness. Though, some of the studies have focused on
physical attributes and performance, they have ignored general
personality constructs (i.e. warmth and sensitivity,
confidence, etc.).
Third, many of the researchers have made another quite
robust assumption. They have assumed definitions of
attractiveness are automatically congruent with what an
individual denotes as romantic or sensual. This assumption
has not been proven valid within any study conducted on
attractiveness and romantic intention. Further, the
definition of sensuality may be conceptually and categorically
different from definitions of attractiveness.
78
Finally, past analyses of gender differences, regarding
attractiveness, have focused their comparisons on
predominantly heterosexual male and female populations. On a
conceptual and theoretical level, it could be argued that
definitions between males and females of opposite sex
individuals are incompatible. In other words, when males
define characteristics important for female attractiveness,
and are then compared to what females define attractive in
males, is like comparing the definitions of apples to oranges.
Males and females may share similar, analogous qualities in
their definition of attractiveness; however, considering the
subjects of their definitions are different, many unique
qualities, specific to males or females, may be expressed.
The Donovan, Hill and Jankowiak (1989) study concluded sex and
sexual orientation of perceivers or observers were irrelevant
to prescaling of female stimuli. They did find sexual
orientation and sex were significant mediating variables when
judging male stimuli.
Schematics
Gender schémas attribute definitions of attractiveness on
which individuals structure, and process their cognitions of
the social world into understandable components. In general,
schémas are much like sexual scripts (Simon and Gagnon, 1984),
which are perhaps more relevant to the understanding of
attractiveness and schematic orientations. Where schémas
79
refer to static normative knowledge, scripts are defined as
dynamic interchanges between the social environment and the
individual based on normative interaction patterns. Sexual
scripts are understood as general guidelines, procedural
diagrams for interpreting sexually relevant expressions and
behaviors. Sexual scripts are typologized by three distinct,
yet interconnected, levels. Cultural scenario scripts dictate
the normatively accepted or shared values attached to
sexuality. Interrelational scripts guide the contexts of
relational sexuality. Intrapsychic scripts define the
motivations, fantasies, and desires for individual sexuality.
If we assume schematics theoretically parallel sexual scripts,
then schémas can be broken into three levels of abstraction;
that is, there are cultural schémas, relational schémas, and
intrapsychic schémas.
From this sexual schematic perspective, definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality vary along schematic levels of
abstraction. Therefore, definitions of attractiveness reflect
cultural standards of attractiveness, relational aspects of
attractiveness, and romantic or sexually desired
characteristics of attractiveness. An individual's definition
of attractiveness constitutes multiple components reflecting
societal, relational, and personal influences. If a common
culture dictates a definition of attractiveness, this may
account for relatively high levels of interrater reliability
in past studies. Definitions of what is attractive for males
80
or females should be shared among all individuals regardless
of sexual orientation.
Definitions of attractiveness at the relational and
intrapsychic schematic levels should not be readily shared or
even similar among individuals. Because each individual
possesses varied past experiences and meanings associated with
sexuality, definitions of attractiveness should be varied.
Furthermore, it is posited definitions of attractiveness at
the relational and intrapsychic level will be more aptly
conceptualized as sensual. In other words, what is sexy is
more likely to be determined by an individual's past sexual
interactions and their respective meanings which they attach
to sexuality. Though it is believed definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality will be dissimilar, a
substantial link between the two concepts should be apparent
because of the reflexivity between the three schematic levels.
Theoretically, the definition of sensuality among raters could
account for the subjective error variations found in many of
the studies conducted on attractiveness.
Gender schematic differences, theoretically, should, be a
relevant concern for researchers. Once again, if we assume
schémas parallel sexual scripts, the gender differences
apparent in sexual scripting should be manifested within
definitions of both attractiveness and sensuality. If
traditional gender roles are present within definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality, then males would be more
81
instrumental in terming their definitions of attractiveness
and sensuality, and females would be more inclined toward
expressive characteristics. If attractiveness is culturally
or normatively defined, and traditional gender roles are an
obvious component in American society, then definitions of
attractiveness should reveal traditional gender differences.
The same should be equally true for definitions of sensuality.
Because traditional gender roles are frequently exhibited
within the context of relational interactions, definitions of
sensuality should also indicate gender biases.
Hypotheses
To reiterate the objectives of this study, the primary
concern of this project is to provide further clarifications
on the definition of attractiveness. The second objective is
to analyze the differences in definitional constructs between
sensuality and attractiveness. The third objective is to
examine gender differences between males and females regarding
the definitions of attractiveness and sensuality, by
controlling for sexual orientation. It should be noted that
definitions of sensuality would be incompatible if given
different targeted stimuli. In other words, males defining
sensual characteristics of females, and females defining
sensual traits in men are incongruent stimuli.
It is expected homosexual males and heterosexual females,
and heterosexual males and lesbians will define attractiveness
82
using the same general characteristics. To reiterate, is it
assumed attractiveness is guided by general norms within the
cultural scenario script which define male and females
attractiveness the same, regardless of sexual orientation.
However, it is predicted sensuality will differ between the
heterosexual and homosexual comparison groups because of
individual motives in the intrapsychic scripts. The
hypotheses stated for this project are delineated below.
(1.) Definitions of attractiveness will significantly
differ when compared to definitions of sensuality
within both comparison groups.
(2.) Definitions of attractiveness will be significantly
similar between heterosexual females and homosexual
males; and heterosexual males and lesbians.
(3.) Definitions of sensuality will be significantly
different between heterosexual females and
homosexual males; and heterosexual males and
lesbians.
83
METHODS
The data for this analysis was theoretically based on
four distinct categories: heterosexual males and females, and
homosexual males and females. To test the propositions
asserted in this study, two samples were collected from these
designated categories. Both surveys were administered during
the spring and summer of 1992.
Homosexual sample
Contact with the homosexual subjects, participating in
this project, was made in three ways: by stratified sampling
through (1) gay/lesbian organizations, (2) gay/lesbian pride
events, and via snowball sampling tactics by (3) acquaintances
in the gay/lesbian community. Numerous organizations were
asked to participate in the project, those who chose to take
part in the study were sent surveys to administer to their
members. Organizers of gay pride events were contacted for
permission to distribute questionnaires for interested
respondents. Several respondents took additional surveys to
give to their gay/lesbian/bisexual friends apart from the gay
pride and the organizational activities. These sampling
procedures were used to gain access to a highly stigmatized
population. Despite the difficulties in reaching a
stigmatized sample, 816 surveys were distributed and 306
surveys were returned. The questionnaire response rate was
calculated at 37.7%. The relatively low response rate
84
resulted due to several factors, including: snowball
sampling, apprehension on the confidentiality of the survey,
and an inability to follow-up on all the surveys. Of the
completed surveys, 169 of the respondents were identified as
gay/bisexual males, and 130 were lesbian/bisexual females.
The average age of the subjects, participating in the
study, was 32. The majority of respondents (79%) identified
themselves as exclusively gay. Only fifteen percent of the
subjects in the project distinguished their orientation
ranging from mostly homosexual to primarily heterosexual with
slight homosexual tendencies. Approximately twenty-two
percent of the respondents classified themselves as students;
forty-four percent as managers, professionals, or executives;
and twenty-three percent as skilled or unskilled workers. The
other eleven percent of subjects were scattered across
miscellaneous occupations, or retired. The majority were
highly educated, possessing or in the process of obtaining a
college degree. Seventy percent of the participants claimed
their annual income was below $30,000, with a mean categorical
response of "between $15,000 and $30,000." The sample
consisted of respondents from over fourteen different states,
from California to New York; however, most of the respondents
were from Iowa. Eighty percent of the sample resided in
metropolitan areas or suburbs. Approximately half of the
sample was committed to a relationship of some sort, while the
85
other half of the respondents were not dating currently, or
were dating casually.
Heterosexual sample
Slightly over two-hundred heterosexual females and one-
hundred and forty-nine males responded to a survey
administered in a large sociology service course at a major
midwestern university. The mean age of the respondents was
approximately 20-21. The majority of the participants were
from a town or small city (41%). Over seventy-one percent of
the respondents were classified as either sophomores or
juniors. The students were from a wide range of majors. The
majority of the respondents lived off-campus (43% for females,
and 46% among males), while thirty percent resided in
dormitories. Approximately twenty-six percent of females and
twenty-three percent of males participated in
sorority/fraternity housing. The families of the participants
in this study tended to possess medium-level incomes. Over
half of the sample reported their family's income was over
forty-thousand dollars, and only three percent responded their
family's income was below fifteen-thousand dollars.
Measurement
Two general, open-ended questions ascertained general and
specific attributes guiding definitions of attractiveness and
sensuality among both heterosexual and homosexual respondents.
86
The first question asked both samples to identify three to
five characteristics which makes an individual attractive.
The second question asked the respondent to identify three to
five characteristics which makes an individual sexy. Open-
ended questions were believed to be superior for this study as
opposed to predefined scale items for several reasons.
Predefined scale items would have limited the respondents to
specify the importance of several designated items in which
past researchers had proposed were qualities of
attractiveness. This method would have restricted the
definition of attractiveness to attributes imposed by
researchers. Secondly, the categories offered by researchers
in the past were usually measured by the degree of importance
for each particular item. The problem with such a format
transposes nominal categories into interval level data, which
may lose the subjective essence of the category through the
digitization of responses. "Importance" scales also produce
asymmetrical distributions, as evidenced by the substantial
means presented in studies using such a design (see Franzoi
and Herzog, 1987).
The subjects' responses to the two questions were coded
in a parallel fashion. A random selection of fifty surveys
(twenty-five from each sample) were previewed to denote
possible categories in the subjects' responses to definitions
of attractiveness and sensuality. A general coding scheme was
established using the parameters initially defined by the
87
previewed surveys. Each new response by the participants, not
undertaken by the initial coding scheme, was added to the
scheme. By the end of the content analysis, the coding scheme
included over forty categories used by the respondents to
define attractiveness and sensuality.
The coding categories represent generalities, as opposed
to specific attributes. For example, the category mouth
included such aspects as lips and teeth. Further, the
category mouth does not specify specific attitudes reflected
on the mouth (i.e. thick lips, small mouth, etc.). One
category, in particular, should be cautiously interpreted:
muscularity. The concept muscularity employed muscle tone and
weight as general dimensions. Weight was not considered as a
distinct dimension because of the extended variability
associated with weight. The same coding scheme was applied to
the personality categories identified by the respondents.
In order to capture very general definitions, not
specific to body, face or personality characteristics, four
"catch-all" categories were formed. General body, face and
personality categories were developed to secure general
preferences. The categories were only used for respondents
who simply indicated a one-word statement, such as "their
personality", or "their body." The last general category
established was overall looks or appearance. This category
included statements like "good-looking", or "the way they
look." Such statements do not reflect specific attributes
88
from a definitional stance; however, the statements, though
obtuse, do connote general attitudes about attractiveness and
sensuality.
After the content analysis was conducted on the data, the
categories were collapsed into three general concepts to be
tested between and within groups on their definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality. The coding scheme was broken
into three major components: face, body, and
personality/style. The categories were fairly simplistic to
divide, except for a few intangible qualities which were not
as obvious. Clothes and hygiene were conceptualized as
personality dimensions, as was the monetary dimension.
Because a smile represents a particular mood or personality
condition, it was operationalized as a personality dimension
as opposed to a facial characteristic. The category age was
dropped from the comparative analysis, due to the obtuse
nature of the concept, and its low frequency among the two
samples. Responses in the general looks/overall appearance
category were divided equally among the base categories of
body, face, and personality.
The responses were tabulated by summing the frequency of
responses, not the number of respondents specifying a
particular category. Given the respondents had an opportunity
to list several characteristics, it seemed reductionistic to
limit the unit of analysis to the respondent. Tests for
significant differences between sample groups was ruled out
89
due to the nominal context of the data, and that the unit of
analysis was responses versus respondents. The frequency of
responses were crosstabulated within the two male-oriented
sample groups (heterosexual females and homosexual males) and
the female-oriented sample (heterosexual males and lesbians)
regarding the differentiation of the perspective groups
definition for sensuality and attractiveness. The responses
were then compared between the heterosexual and homosexual
samples regarding the definitions of sensuality and
attractiveness.
Cautions and limitations
The data for this analysis should be interpreted with
some degree of caution. First, the samples are not readily
comparable for a precise analysis between groups. The gay
males used in the analysis are substantially older than the
females, which may suggest an age or cohort differentiation as
opposed to a gender difference. Secondly, it is assumed the
gender identity of the two samples are normally distributed.
This remains a substantial assumption providing gender tests
were not administered to the participants. Third, the
analysis is simplistic. The categories reflect only
dimensions of attractiveness or sensuality, not the extent
(variability in the dimension) or the relative importance of
the dimensions.
90
RESULTS
Definitions of attractiveness
Tables 1, 3, 5, and 7 reproduce the frequency of
responses specified by homosexual males and females, and
heterosexual females and males regarding the definition of
attractiveness. Based on the percentage of responses for the
variety of traits, several traits emerged as more viable than
others.
Homosexual males. If based on the criteria of
exceeding twenty percent of the total responses as being
highly relevant traits, than homosexual males designated eyes,
hair, height, muscularity, buttocks (approximately), chest,
and general facial features as being attractive. When
compared to heterosexual females, gay males exhibited a
stronger preference for hands, genitalia, chest, hygiene, the
general categories: face, body, and looks. Gay males also
responded more often to the subject of body hair contrasted
with the responses of heterosexual females. The category of
body hair for gay males exceeded female responses by nine
percent. For many of the gay males specifying body hair,
implied the more body hair the better, whereas for
heterosexual females they indicated the less body hair the
better. With regard to hygiene, only seven percent of the
responses made by gay males reported the way a man smelled was
significant to them; consequently, only two percent of
heterosexual female responses specified this particular
91
category. The most startling preference indicated by gay
males, not issued by heterosexual females, was that of
genitalia. Seventeen percent of the gay males responses
favored large genitalia, as compared to zero responses by
heterosexual females.
Heterosexual females. Heterosexual females specified
eyes, hair, height, muscularity, buttocks, general facial
characteristics, and smiling as being prominent categories.
It appears heterosexual females place a greater emphasis on
eyes, mouth, and hair when compared to homosexual males.
Twice as many females indicated hair as being relevant in
their definitions of attractiveness than the gay males in this
study. Slightly more heterosexual females (by six percent of
responses) over males indicated eyes and mouth were of
interest in defining attractiveness for males. Heterosexual
female responses were also more favorable toward height,
smiling, and muscularity when compared with the responses of
gay males. The difference in responses describing muscularity
was over twenty percent. Both height and smiling
differentiated by approximately eleven percent between the
.... ^
males and females in this study.
Heterosexual males. Again, using the twenty percent
and over criteria as a means to specify particular traits as
analytically meaningful, then heterosexual males selected
eyes, hair, muscularity, legs, breasts, buttocks, and face (in
general) as being substantially significant. Twice as many
92
males stipulated height, legs, and breasts when compared to
lesbians. Further, approximately nineteen percent of
heterosexual males designated overall body features when
contrasted with less than one percent of the lesbian sample.
Lesbians. Lesbians indicated eyes, hair, muscularity,
general looks, and smile as relevant features in defining
female attractiveness. When comparing lesbians to
heterosexual males in definitional content for female
attractiveness, thirteen percent more lesbians specified the
category of eyes over heterosexual males. Eight percent more
lesbians cited smile as being considered attractive over
heterosexual males. Twice as many lesbians indicated
mannerisms/movement, and affectionate/romantic characteristics
as being more attractive when compared to heterosexual males.
And fourteen percent more lesbians were more likely to
designate eyes as being attractive when contrasted with
heterosexual males.
Definitions of sensuality
Analyzing the percentage of responses between homosexual
males and heterosexual females revealed some intriguing
contrasts. Tables 2 and 4 reflect the number of responses,
and percentage of responses for each category among homosexual
males and heterosexual females. First, it appeared the
percentage of responses for the definition of sensuality were
more varied among the designated categories than the responses
93
regarding the definition of attractiveness. Once again,
heterosexual females indicated, by their responses, a stronger
preference for muscularity (by eleven percent) than gay males.
The women in the study also exhibited higher responses for the
categories of smiling^ affectionate/romantic behaviors, and
warmth/sensitivity. The differences between sample groups
were not exceptionally large, but perhaps substantively
significant. On the other hand, homosexual males were more
likely to respond to the categories of hair, genitalia,
intelligence, and body hair when compared to females.
Tables 6 and 8 reflect the frequency of responses in the
definition of sensuality among heterosexual males and
lesbians. Again, more heterosexual males (fourteen percent)
indicated hair was sensuous than lesbians; however, more
lesbians responded eyes (11 percent) were sensuous over
heterosexual males. Twice as many males concluded muscularity
was a sensuous characteristic compared to lesbians.
Heterosexual males were also more likely to designate
buttocks, breasts, and clothes as sensuous features for
females when contrasted to lesbians. Thirteen percent more
lesbians than heterosexual males targeted confidence as
sensuous; whereas, twenty-seven percent more heterosexual
males over lesbians specified affectionate/romantic features
as sensuous for females.
94
Definitional differences between attractiveness and sensuality
A major proposition posited in this study asserted what
is sensual and what is attractive are two unique, and distinct
concepts. This hypothesis was verified in the crosstabulation
analysis of the respondents' definitions of attractiveness
when compared to sensuality. Tables 9, 10, 11, and 12 present
the number of responses for each major classification (face,
body, and personality) concerning the definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality within each targeted sample.
Among homosexual males (Table 9), body characteristics were
more important than facial features for definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality. However, personality traits
were least relevant regarding definitions of attractiveness;
conversely a man's personality was preferred over facial and
body characteristics for gay males' definition of sensuality.
The Chi square product yielded by the test of significance was
overwhelming (chi square with 2 degrees of freedom = 224.163,
p < .000). A parallel trend was evident for heterosexual
females (see Table 10). A man's body tended to be more
important than his face when judging levels of both
attractiveness and sensuality in men. And personality
characteristics were very irrelevant for definitions of
attractiveness, but were exceptionally important in judging a
male's sensuality among heterosexual females. The chi square
product was considerably higher for heterosexual females [chi
square (2)=334.470; p < .001], indicating a significant
95
difference between definitions of attractiveness and
sensuality.
Table 11 reflects the crosstabulation analysis between
the definition of attractiveness with the definition of
sensuality among lesbians. A considerable difference was
apparent in the contrasting definitions. With regard to
attractiveness, a woman's face and body were equally
important, more so than personality. However, personality
was, by far, the most explicit category cited by the lesbian
sample in defining sensuality (chi square with two degrees of
freedom = 130.947, p < .001). Table 12 reports the contrast
in definitional components between attractiveness and
sensuality among heterosexual males. Similarly, heterosexual
males reported the face and body were significant features
which defined female attractiveness. However, personality
characteristics appeared to be the most influencing factor in
the definition of sensuality, with body being second (chi
square with two degrees of freedom = 195.103, p < .001).
Group comparisons
The numerous categories of attractiveness were collapsed
into three major categories: face, body, and personality.
The categories were then contrasted between the four sample
groups using a crosstabulation of responses. A chi square
product was then calculated to estimate the significance of
differences associated in heterosexual females' and homosexual
96
males' definitions of male attractiveness; and, between
heterosexual males and lesbians in definitions of female
attractiveness. Table 13 presents the percent of responses
made by gay males and heterosexual females. The chi square
indicated no significant differences between gay males and
straight females concerning definitions of attractiveness (chi
square with two degrees of freedom = 4.08, p < .200). It
appeared a man's body dominated the percentage of responses
for both target groups; a man's face was second in percentage
of responses. And a man's personality was the least indicated
among the participants in this study. Table 15 illustrates
the comparison of responses between lesbians and heterosexual
males. The chi square product indicated a significant
difference in the means lesbians and heterosexual males
specify qualities of female attractiveness among the
categories of face, body, and personality (chi square with two
degrees of freedom = 24.104, p < .001). It appeared a woman's
body is of more interest to heterosexual males than lesbians;
consequently, twice as many lesbians indicated personality
characteristics were important if defining female
attractiveness.
As with attractiveness, a chi square test was conducted
on the definition of sensuality between the four groups.
Again, the categories were condensed into three basic
classifications. Table 14 illustrates the differences in
definitions of sensuality between heterosexual females and
97
homosexual males. The highest percentage of responses for
both males and females fell into the classification of
personality. It appeared personality was a convincing trait
in the definition of sensuality. A man's body was the second
most identified category among the respondents, and facial
characteristics maintained the lowest percentage of responses.
The response differential between heterosexual females and
homosexual males regarding the perspective groups' definition
of sensuality was significant (chi square = 18.71, p < .001).
The results indicated a significant difference in percentage
of responses for personality traits, and facial
characteristics among the two samples. A man's body yielded
similar response rates for straight females and gay males.
More gay males than straight females reported facial
characteristics, in general, were more significant in their
definitions of sensuality. And more females than males
demonstrated a stronger preference for personality traits.
Table 16 presents the crosstabulation of responses
between heterosexual males and lesbians regarding the
definition of sensuality. The overwhelming majority of
respondents denoted personality characteristics over face and
body features in the definition of female sensuality. More
lesbians indicated preference for personality characteristics
than heterosexual males. And more heterosexual males
delineated body features over lesbians. Slightly more
lesbians specified facial qualities when compared to
98
heterosexual males. The results of the chi square test was
significant (chi square with two degrees of freedom = 18.97, p
< .001).
99
DISCUSSION
The results of this study suggested that the construct
definitions of sensuality and attractiveness were
significantly different. The definition of sensuality was
markedly different from the definition of attractiveness for
both females and males. This finding critically challenged
past studies conducted on the effect of attractiveness on the
selection of romantic partners. Given the two unique
constructs, future researchers should be concerned with
topological definitions utilized in their projects. Further,
future investigators should employ personality dimensions in
their analysis of both attractiveness and particularly
sensuality. Since the definition of sensuality was
significantly composed of personality characteristics,
including a personality dimension to definitions of. sensuality
should heighten the results of future research efforts.
The schematic arrangements of cognitive structures
employed in defining attractiveness and sensuality varied
along gender lines. Although some characteristics
demonstrated similar importance, other categories yielded
differential importance between males and females. Eyes,
hair, height, muscularity, and general facial characteristics
were commonly judged as important components of attractiveness
for both males and females. Both lesbians and heterosexual
females were more likely to signify smiling as important in
their definitions of attractiveness. On an abstract level.
100
smiling infers an expressive element dependent on mood, and
feelings. Gay males denoted genitalia and chest as being
relevant characteristics in assessing attractiveness.
Genitalia and chest as characteristics important for sexual
relations may be defined as instrumental. The same was true
for heterosexual males, defining both buttocks and breasts as
attractive. Therefore, both heterosexual and gay males were
more likely to indicate instrumental qualities as attractive,
and females were more likely to focus on expressive qualities.
Gender differences were more apparent regarding
definitions of sensuality. By analyzing the crosstabulation
of sensuality between heterosexual females and homosexual
males, it appears females were significantly more interested
in personality categories; whereas homosexual males favored
physical attributes. Once again, the two groups differed
along gender schematics: males endorsing instrumental
features of strength and masculinity, and females subscribing
to expressive traits like personality dimensions. In short,
gender schémas are relevant to the definitions of sensuality
and attractiveness and should be studied further.
The definition of attractiveness was found to be
significantly similar among heterosexual females and
homosexual males, but not so between lesbians and heterosexual
males. This result supports the hypothesis that
attractiveness is linked to a cultural schema which dictates a
normative structure to the definition of attractiveness for
101
both gay males and heterosexual females. It could be inferred
from the data that lesbians, perhaps, maintain a unique
cultural schemata pertaining to the definition of female
attractiveness when compared to heterosexual males. The data
indicated lesbians admired personality qualities as attractive
over obvious physical attributes. Whereas, heterosexual males
paid more attention to the physical attributes of body and
face. The cultural scenario script of lesbians focused on
personality traits such as warmth and sensitivity, emphasizing
intimacy attributes over the physical. The cultural scenario
script for heterosexual males indicated straight males
centered on more implicitly sexual qualities, such as buttocks
and breasts. The contrast between the cultural scenario
scripts among heterosexual males and lesbians reflected the
stark subcultural differentiation between intimacy versus
sexuality, regarding the definition of attractiveness.
Definitions of sensuality were significantly different
between the four comparison groups. This finding infers
sensuality is conceptually linked to the cognitive structures
congruent with relational and intrapsychic schémas. Since
relational and intrapsychic schémas are individually variant,
this finding suggests definitions of sensuality are
individual-oriented. What is sensual to one individual is not
necessarily sensual for another. Considering the variance of
responses among all four comparison groups, it appears the
definition of sensuality is in the eye of the beholder.
102
Though there is some overlap among the subjects' responses
between attractiveness and sensuality, sensuality appears to
be more of a personality quality as opposed to a complete
physical attribute.
In conclusion, it is hoped that future investigators
first clarify and substantiate their definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality in their research. Second,
further analyses is in order to substantiate tests of
significance on gender, and sexual orientation differences.
Third, an analyses on the importance of certain features over
others should be evaluated. The study has indicated gender,
and sexual orientation differences exist regarding the
definitions of attractiveness and sensuality. Perhaps, it
could be inferred from this study that attractiveness is in
the eye of the culture or cultures, and sensuality is in the
eye of the beholder.
103
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Agnew, R. 1984. "Appearance and delinquency." criminology:
An Interdisciplinary Journal 22:421-440.
Alicke, M. D., R. H. Smith, and M. L. Klotz. 1986.
"Judgments of physical attractiveness: the role of faces
and bodies." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
12:381-389.
Cunnigham, M. R., A. P. Barbae, and C. L. Pike. 1990. "What
do women want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple
motives in the perception of male facial physical
attractiveness." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 59:61-72.
Donovan, J. M., E. Hill, and W. R. Jankowiak. 1989. "Gender,
sexual orientation, and truth-of-consensus in studies of
physical attractiveness." The Journal of Sex Research
26:264-271.
Downs, A. C. 1990. "Objective and subjective physical
attractiveness judgments among young adults." Perceptual
and Motor Skills 70:458.
Efran, M., and E. Patterson. 1974. "Voters vote beautiful;
the effects of physical attractiveness on a national
debate." Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science 6:352-
356.
Feingold, A. 1990. "Gender differences in effects of
physical attractiveness on romantic attraction: a
comparison across five research paradigms." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 59:981-993.
Feinman, S., and G. W. Gill. 1978. "Sex differences in
physical attractiveness preferences." The Journal of
Social Psychology 105:43-52.
Franzoi, S. L., and M. E. Herzog. 1987. "Judging physical
attractiveness: what body aspects do we use?"
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 13:19-33.
Gladue, B. A., and H. J. Delaney. 1990. "Gender differences
in perception of attractiveness of men and women in
bars." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
16:378-391.
Glaser, B. G., and A. L. Strauss. 1967. The Discovery of
Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research.
New York, NY: Aldine De Gruyter.
104
Graziano, W. G., T. Brothen, and E. Berscheld. 1978. "Height
and attraction: do men and women see eye-to-eye?"
Journal of Personality 46:128-145.
Harry, J. 1990. "A probability sampling of gay males."
Journal of Homosexuality 19:89-104.
Hovrath, T. 1981. "Physical attractiveness; the influence of
selected torso parameters." Archives of Sexual Behavior
10:21-24.
Jackson, L. A., L. A. Sullivan, and J. S. Hymes. 1986.
"Gender, gender role, and physical appearance." The
Journal of Psychology 121:51-56.
Keating, C. F. 1985. "Gender and the physiognomy of
dominance and attractiveness." Social Psychology
Quarterly 48:61-70.
Krebs, D., and A. Adinolfi. 1975. "Physical attractiveness,
social relations, and personality style." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 31:245-253.
Kulik, J. A., and J. Harackiewicz. 1979. "Opposite-sex
interpersonal attraction as a function of the sex-roles
of the perceiver and the perceived." Sex Roles 5:443-
452.
Lavrakas, p. L. 1975. "Female preferences for male
physique." Journal of Research in Personality 9:324-334.
Lucker, G. W., W. E. Beane, and K. Guire. 1981. "The
ideographic approach to physical attractiveness
research." The Journal of Psychology 107:57-67.
Nida, S., and J. Williams. 1977. "Sex stereo-typed traits,
physical attractiveness, and interpersonal attraction."
Psychological Reports 41:1311.
Patzer, G. 1985. The Physical Attractiveness Phenomena. New
York, NY: Plenum Press.
Rand, C. S., and J. A. Hall. 1983. "Sex differences in the
accuracy of self-perceived attractiveness." Social
Psychology Quarterly 46:359-363.
Simon, W., and J. H. Gagnon. 1984. "Sexual scripts."
Society 22:53-60.
105
Sprecher, S. 1989. "The importance to males and females of
physical attractiveness, earning potential, and
expressiveness in initial attraction." Sex Roles 21:591-
607.
Symons, D. 1987. "An evolutionary approach: can Darwin's
view of life shed light on human sexuality?" Pp. 91-126
in Theories of Human Sexuality, edited by J. H. Geer and
W. T. O'Donohue. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Touhey, J. C. 1979. "Sex-role stereotyping and individual
differences in liking for the physically attractive."
Social Psychology Quarterly 42:285-289.
Udry, J. 1965. "Structural correlates of feminine beauty
preferences in Britain and the U.S.: a comparison.
Sociology and Social Research 49:330-342.
Waters, J. 1985. "Cosmetics and the job market." Pp. 51-87
in The Psychology of Cosmetic Treatments^ edited J.
Graham and A. Kligman. New York, NY: Praeger Press.
106
Table 1. Number of responses, and percent of homosexual males
responding to definitions of attractiveness for
males (n=158)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 48 30.38%
2. Hair 48 30.38%
3. Nose 2 1.27%
4. Chin 4 2.53%
5. Jaw 1 .63%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 0 0%
7. Mouth 5 3.16%
8. Skin Color (race) 5 3.16%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 6 3.80%
10. Facial Hair 10 6.33%
11. Height 41 25.95%
12. Muscularity 85 53.80%
13. Shoulders 2 1.27%
14. Arms 10 6.33%
15. Legs 24 15.19%
16. Hands 11 6.96%
17. Feet 2 1.27%
18. Hips 1 .63%
19. Genitals 27 17.09%
20. Abdomen 5 3.16%
21. Buttocks 31 19.62%
22. Chest/Breasts 37 23.42%
23. Clothes 4 2.53%
24. Hygiene 12 7.60%
25. Sense of Humor 2 • 1.27%
26. Energetic 1 .63%
27. Sensitivity 2 1.27%
28. Confident 1 .63%
29. Af f ect ionate/RomantIc 1 .63%
30. Seductive 0 0%
31. Intelligence 4 2.53%
32. Monetary 1 .63%
, 33. Mannerisms/Movement 2 1.27%
34. Voice/Communication 2 1.27%
35. Nice Personality 0 0%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 20 12.66%
37. Face (general) 86 54.43%
38. Body (general) 23 14.56%
39. Personality (general) 5 3.16%
40. Smile 18 11.39%
41. Body Hair 16 10.13%
42. Style 3 1.90%
43. Age 3 1.90%
Note: respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
107
Table 2. Number of responses, and percent of homosexual males
responding to definitions of sensuality for males
(n=154)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 42 27.27%
2. Hair 25 16.23%
3. Nose 1 .65%
4. Chin 1 .65%
5. Jaw 1 .65%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 1 .65%
7. Mouth 5 3.25%
8. Skin Color (race) 4 2.60%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 3 1.30%
10. Facial Hair 6 3.90%
11. Height 8 5.19%
12. Muscularity 38 24.68%
13. Shoulders 0 0%
14. Arms 3 1.30%
15. Legs 8 5. 19%
16. Hands 4 2.60%
17. Feet 1 .65%
18. Hips 2 .65%
19. Genitals 23 14.94%
20. Abdomen 3 1.30%
21. Buttocks 14 9.09%
22. Chest/Breasts 8 5.19%
23. Clothes 22 14.29%
24. Hygiene 4 2.60%
25. Sense of Humor 23 14.94%
26. Energetic 14 9.09%
27. Sensitivity 14 9.09%
28. Confident 17 11.04%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 11 7.14%
30. Seductive 12 7.79%
31. Intelligence 33 21.43%
32. Monetary 1 .65%
33. Mannerisms/Movement 27 17.53%
34. Voice/Communication 22 14.27%
35. Nice Personality 11 7.14%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 24 15.58%
37. Face (general) 23 14.94%
38. Body (general) 21 13.64%
39. Personality (general) 39 25.32%
40. Smile 15 9.74%
41. Body Hair 14 9.09%
42. Style 8 5.19%
43. Age 2 1.30%
Note: respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
108
Table 3. Number of responses, and percent of heterosexual
females responding to definitions of attractiveness
for males (n=188)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 120 63.82%
2. Hair 68 36.17%
3. Nose 2 1.06%
4. Chin 3 1.60%
5. Jaw 2 1.06%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 2 1.06%
7. Mouth 17 9.04%
8. Skin Color (race) 0 0%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 15 7.98%
10. Facial Hair 2 1.06%
11. Height 71 37.77%
12. Muscularity 139 73.94%
13. Shoulders 12 6.38%
14. Arms 10 5.32%
15. Legs 17 , 9.04%
16. Hands 12 6.38%
17. Feet 0 0%
18. Hips 1 .53%
19. Genitals 0 0%
20. Abdomen 5 2.66%
21. Buttocks 43 22.87%
22. Chest/Breasts 22 11.70%
23. Clothes 5 2.66%
24. Hygiene 5 2.66%
25. Sense of Humor 3 1.60%
26. Energetic 0 0%
27. Sensitivity 1 .53%
28. Confident 3 1.60%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 0 0%
30. Seductive 0 0%
31. Intelligence 1 .53%
32. Monetary 0 0%
33. Manner isms/Movement 7 3.72%
34. Voice/Communication 0 0%
35. Nice Personality 2 1.06%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 15 7.98%
37. Face (general) 42 22.34%
38. Body (general) 13 6.91%
39. Personality (general) 5 2.66%
40. Smile 52 27.66%
41. Body Hair 3 1.60%
42. Style 4 2.13%
43. Age 0 0%
Note: respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
109
Table 4. Number of responses, and percent of heterosexual
females responding to definitions of sensuality for
males (n=176)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1.Eyes 56 31.82%
2.Hair 14 7.95%
3.Nose 1 .57%
4. Chin 0 0%
5. Jaw 2 1.14%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 1 .57%
7. Mouth 3 1.70%
8. Skin Color (race) 0 0%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 8 4.55%
10. Facial Hair 2 1.14%
11. Height 15 8.52%
12. Muscularity 63 35.80%
13. Shoulders 2 1.14%
14. Arms 2 1.14%
15. Legs 9 5.11%
16. Hands 2 1.14%
17. Feet 0 0%
18. Hips 0 0%
19. Genitals 0 0%
20. Abdomen 4 2.27%
21. Buttocks 18 10.23%
22. Chest/Breasts 5 2.84%
23. Clothes 26 14.77%
24. Hygiene 9 5.11%
25. Sense of Humor 34 19.32%
26. Energetic 24 13.64%
27. Sensitivity 40 22.73%
28. Confident 26 14.77%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 27 15.34%
30. Seductive 13 7.39%
31. Intelligence 14 7.95%
32. Monetary 1 .57%
33. Mannerisms/Movement 41 23.30%
34. Voice/Communication 22 12.50%
35. Nice Personality ' ' ...1^ 10.23%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 28 15.91%
37. Face (general) 14 7.95%
38. Body (general) 16 9.09%
39. Personality (general) 36 20.45%
40. Smile 43 24.43%
41. Body Hair 4 2.27%
42. Style 8 4.55%
43. Age 0 0%
Note: respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
110
Table 5. Number of responses, and percent of heterosexual
males responding to definitions of physical
attractiveness for females (n=149)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 59 39.59%
2. Hair 71 47.65%
3. Nose 4 2.68%
4. Chin 0 0%
5. Jaw 0 0%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 1 .67%
7. Mouth • 6 4.03%
8. Skin Color (race) 1 .67%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 10 6.71%
10. Facial Hair 0 0%
11. Height 23 15.44%
12. Muscularity 39 26.17%
13. Shoulders 1 .67%
14. Arms 0 0%
15. Legs 42 28.19%
16. Hands 3 2.01%
17. Feet 1 .67%
18. Hips 1 .67%
19. Genitals 0 0%
20. Abdomen 4 2.68%
21. Buttocks 48 32.21%
22. Chest/Breasts 49 32.89%
23. Clothes 8 5.37%
24. Hygiene 8 5.37%
25. Sense of Humor 1 .67%
26. Energetic 2 1.34%
27. Sensitivity 2 1.34%
28. Confident 1 .67%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 2 1.34%
30. Seductive 1 .67%
31. Intelligence 5 3.36%
32. Monetary 0 0%
33. Mannerisms/Movement 3 2.01%
34. Voice/Communication 0 0%
35. Nice Personality 3 2.01%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 11 • 7.38%
37. Face (general) 58 38.93%
38. Body (general) 29 19.46%
39. Personality (general) 5 3.36%
40. Smile 18 12.08%
41. Body Hair 0 0%
42. Style 7 4.70%
43. Age 0 0%
Note; respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
Ill
Table 6. Number of responses, and percent of heterosexual
males responding to definitions of sensuality for
females (n=149)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 34 22.82%
2. Hair 41 27.52%
3. Nose 1 .67%
4. Chin 0 0%
5. Jaw 0 0%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 1 .67%
7. Mouth 4 2.68%
8. Skin Color (race) 0 0%
9. Skin complexion (tan) 8 5.37%
10. Facial Hair 0 0%
11. Height 3 2.01%
12. Muscularity 35 23.49%
13. Shoulders 0 0%
14. Arms 1 .67%
15. Legs 27 18.12%
16. Hands 2 1.34%
17. Feet 1 .67%
18. Hips 2 1.34%
19. Genitals 0 0%
20. Abdomen 7 4.70%
21. Buttocks 19 12.75%
22. Chest/Breasts 25 16.78%
23. Clothes 43 28.86%
24. Hygiene 6 4.03%
25. Sense of Humor 20 13.42%
26. Energetic 10 6.71%
27. Sensitivity 14 9.40%
28. Confident 11 7.38%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 51 34.23%
30. Seductive 20 13.42%
31. Intelligence 17 11.41%
32. Monetary 0 0%
33. Mannerisms/Movement 25 16.78%
34. Voice/Communication 12 8.05%
35. Nice Personality 12 8.05%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 22 14.77%
37. Face (general) 11 7.38%
38. Body (general) 18 12.08%
39. Personality (general) 27 18.12%
40. Smile 18 12.08%
41. Body Hair 0 0%
42. Style 17 11.41%
43. Age 0 0%
Note: respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
112
Table 7. Number of responses, and percent of homosexual
females responding to definitions of physical
attractiveness for females (n=130)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 69 53.08%
2. Hair 48 36.92%
3. Nose 2 1.54%
4. Chin 1 .77%
5. Jaw 1 .77%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 2 1.54%
7. Mouth 12 9.23%
8. Skin Color (race) 5 3.85%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 7 5.38%
10. Facial Hair 2 1.54%
11. Height 10 7.69%
12. Muscularity 46 35.38%
13. Shoulders 4 2.68%
14. Arms 6 4.62%
15. Legs 16 12.31%
16. Hands 10 7.69%
17. Feet 1 .77%
18. Hips 2 1.54%
19. Genitals 1 .77%
20. Abdomen 3 2.31%
21. Buttocks 8 6.15%
22. Chest/Breasts 22 16.92%
23. Clothes 7 5.38%
24. Hygiene 11 8.46%
25. Sense of Humor 1 .77%
26. Energetic 3 2.31%
27. Sensitivity 2 1.54%
28. Confident 10 7.69%
29. Aff ectionate/Romantic 0 0%
30. Seductive 0 0%
31. Intelligence 6 4.62%
32. Monetary 1 .77%
33. Manner isms/Movement 8 6.15%
34. Voice/Communication 2 1.54%
35. Nice Personality 2 1.54%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 7 5.38%
37. Face (general) 30 23.08%
38. Body (general) 1 .77%
39. Personality (general) 7 5.38%
40. Smile 27 20.77%
41. Body Hair 3 2.31%
42. Style 6 4.62%
43. Age 1 .77%
Note; respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
113
Table 8. Number of responses, and percent of homosexual
females responding to definitions of sensuality for
females (n=130)
Items No. of Responses % of Respondents
Specifying Trait Specifying Trait
1. Eyes 45 34.62%
2. Hair 17 13.08%
3. Nose 1 .77%
4. Chin 2 1.54%
5. Jaw 2 1.54%
6. Cheeks (cheekbone) 1 .77%
7. Mouth 7 5.38%
8. Skin Color (race) 2 1.54%
9. Skin Complexion (tan) 4 3.08%
10. Facial Hair 1 .77%
11. Height 2 1.54%
12. Muscularity 12 9.23%
13. Shoulders 1 .77%
14. Arms 0 0%
15. Legs 5 3.85%
16. Hands 3 2.31%
17. Feet 2 1.54%
18. Hips 3 2.31%
19. Genitals 1 .77%
20. Abdomen 1 .77%
21. Buttocks 7 5.38%
22. Chest/Breasts 9 6.92%
23. Clothes 11 8.46%
24. Hygiene 5 3.85%
25. Sense of Humor 24 18.46%
26. Energetic 12 9.23%
27. Sensitivity 12 9.23%
28. Confident 30 23.08%
29. Affectionate/Romantic 9 6.92%
30. Seductive 13 10.00%
31. Intelligence 23 17.69%
32. Monetary 0 0%
33. Mannerisms/Movement 23 17.69%
34. Voice/Communication 19 14.62%
35. Nice Personality 14 10.77%
36. Looks/Overall Appearance 13 10.00%
37. Face (general) 5 3.85%
38. Body (general) 14 10.77%
39. Personality (general) 32 24.62%
40. Smile 15 11.54%
41. Body Hair 3 2. 31%
42. Style 16 12.31%
43. Age 1 .77%
Note; respondents were given an opportunity to specify more
than one item.
114
Table 9. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality among homosexual males
Face Body Personality Total
Attractiveness 214 339 60 613
Sensuality 137 149 272 558
Total 351 488 332 1171
Chi Square (2) = 224.163
p < .001
Table 10. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality among heterosexual
females
Face Body Personality Total
Attractiveness 273 363 88 724
Sensuality 100 167 383 650
Total 373 530 471 1171
Chi Square (2) = 334.470
p < .001
115
Table 11. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality among homosexual
females
Face Body Personality Total
Attractiveness 180 164 93 437
Sensuality 88 76 256 420
Total 268 240 349 857
Chi Square (2) = 130.947
P < .001
Table 12. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality among heterosexual
males
Face Body Personality Total
Attractiveness 204 265 58 522
Sensuality 98 158 263 519
Total 302 423 321 1041
Chi Square (2) = 195.103
p < .001
116
Table 13. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness between heterosexual females and
homosexual males
Percentiles Total No.
Face Body Personality of Responses
Heterosexual 37.71% 50.14% 12.15% 724
Females
Homosexual 34.91% 55.30% 9.79% 613
Males
Total 36.42% 52.51% 11.07% 1337
Chi Square (2) = 4.08
p < .200
Table 14. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
sensuality between heterosexual females and
homosexual males
Percentiles Total No.
Face Body Personality of Responses
Heterosexual 15.383 25.69% 58.92% 650
Females
Homosexual 24.553 26.70% 48.75% 558
Males
Total 19.62% 26.16% 54.22% 1208
Chi Square (2) = 18.71
p < .001
117
Table 15. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
attractiveness between heterosexual males and
homosexual females
Percentiles Total No.
Face Body Personality of Responses
Heterosexual 38.71% 50.28% 11.01% 527
Males
Homosexual 41.19% 37.53% 21.28% 437
Females
Total 39.84% 44.50% 15.66% 964
Chi Square (2) = 24.104
p < .001
Table 16. Comparison of general qualities in definitions of
sensuality between heterosexual males and
homosexual females
perçentiles Total No.
Face Body Personality of Responses
Heterosexual 18.88% 30.44% 50.684 519
Males
Homosexual 21.00% 18.004 61.00' 420
Females
Total 19.81% 24.92% 55.27% 939
Chi Square (2) = 18.97
p < .001
118
PAPER 3. THE VIOLENT EFFECTS OF DISCRIMINATION
ON GAY AND LESBIAN RELATIONSHIPS
119
ABSTRACT
The objectives of this study were, in general, to analyze
the relationship between physical discrimination and
aggression in gay and lesbian relationships. The proposed
models asserted physical and sexual discrimination was caused
by the extent of being "out of the closet". Further, it was
hypothesized discrimination would increase the extent of
physical victimization, the perpetration of -abuse, and sexual
victimization in gay and lesbian relationships. Finally,
abuse incurred in the relationship would lead to unstable
relationships. The models were moderated by self-esteem. The
sample consisted of 126 lesbians, and 167 gay males. The
results of the analyses indicated being "out" fostered
discrimination among lesbians with high and low self-esteem;
however, being "out" lead to discrimination for gay males with
low self-esteem only. Discrimination was significantly
associated with physical victimization, the use of violence,
and being a victim of sexual aggression among gay males
regardless of self-esteem. Discrimination was related to
physical victimization, the use of violence, and sexual
aggression only among lesbians with low self-esteem. The
" * •..
various forms of abuse were highly associated with relational
stability. The more abuse encountered in the relationship,
the less likely the relationship was maintained for both gay
males and lesbians, regardless of self-esteem.
120
INTRODUCTION
Very little empirical research has been conductcd on
violent gay and lesbian relationships. The sparse research,
which has been available on homosexual victimology, is
dramatic in its presentation. However, duo to the qualitative
nature of the data, past research sacrifices generalization
and probability analysis to the phenomenon. Of the studies
which have been conducted on gay/lesbian abuse, the results
have primarily been descriptive. For example, Brand and Kidd
(1986) concluded lesbians reported slightly less abuse (both
physical and sexual) when compared to heterosexual females.
On the other hand, Kelly and Warshafsky (1987) found the vast
majority of a self-selected sample of gays and lesbians were
verbally abusive; and, over forty percent of the sample had
used physically aggressive tactics during the course of their
relationships. However, the researchers did not examine the
extent of perpetration or victimization regarding the
aggressive relationships. Kelly and Warshafsky (1987) did
conclude gay males were more likely to encounter violence in
their relationships than lesbians, but not significantly.
According to Duncan (1990), gays and lesbians were more likely
to experience forced sexual participation than heterosexual
college students, although, their sample consisted of less
than seventy gay/lesbian subjects. Finally, substance abuse
has been documented, among lesbian partners, as a correlate of
domestic violence. Schilit, Lie, and Montagne (1990)
121
recounted two-thirds of their sample asserted alcohol or drugs
were implicated during an abusive relationship.
122
MODELING VIOLENCE IN GAY/LESBIAN RELATIONSHIPS
Gay and lesbian relationships have posed unique problems
to family violence researchers. Generally, homosexual
relationships are structured differently than heterosexual
relationships, from the inherent structure of the relationship
to the prevailing stigma of homosexuality in the dominant
culture. Therefore, various considerations are necessary when
modeling the potential indicators and causes of violence in
gay and lesbian relationships. First, a model diagramming gay
and lesbian violence needs to account for the general cultural
context gay and lesbian relationships are consistently exposed
to. Second, a model of gay and lesbian violence has to
incorporate how the stigma associated with homosexuality
affects the nature of violence in their relationships, whether
physical, sexual, victimization, or perpetration. Third, the
model should investigate the affect of the violent episodes on
the stability of the relationships.
This study focuses on applying a sexual script
perspective to various violent episodes in gay and lesbian
relationships. The analysis in this study incorporates the
three interdynamic levels of sexual scripts. The three levels
of sexual scripts include cultural scenario scripts,
interrelational scripts, and intrapsychic scripts. In
general, sexual scripts are general cognitive guidelines
ordering sexual definitions, attitudes and behaviors.
Cultural scenario scripts are scripts directed by broader
123
social forces, detailing appropriate and inappropriate sexual
conduct. Interrelational scripts are scripts which reflect
the interdynamic qualities of past and present relationships
regarding an individual's sexual identity. Intrapsychic
scripts describe sexual desires, fantasies, and motivations.
An examination of how all three levels of scripting influence
not only the effect of violence, but also the correlates of
abuse in gay and lesbian relationships. For example, by
investigating the relationship between cultural perspectives
toward homosexuality (cultural scenario scripts), with the
psychological stamina of gays and lesbians (intrapsychic
scripts), the effect of violence in gay/lesbian relationships
(interrelational) can be further evaluated. Therefore, it is
necessary to examine all three aspects of sexual scripts to
obtain a fully developed understanding of how violence is
initiated, mediated, and sustained within gay and lesbian
relationships.
Cultural scenario scripts
It is posited being "out of the closet", gays and
lesbians would incur more discrimination than those gays and
lesbians who do not readily disclose their identity.
Although, disclosure of homosexuality may be a precursor to
discrimination, there are different intervals of disclosure.
It could be argued those gays and lesbians who are flamboyant
with their sexual identity are more likely to incur
124
discrimination. While those gays and lesbians who disclose
their sexual identity to only a few individuals are not as
likely to face the extent of discrimination as those who
conspicuously disclose their orientation. Further,
conspicuous homosexuals would incur more discrimination than
those who do not disclose their sexual identity. From a
cultural scenario perspective, it could be argued heterosexual
sexual scripts entail moderate social approval of violence
directed at gays and lesbians. According to Wolfgang and
Ferracuti (1967) subcultural approval toward violence is a
matter of differential identification and association with the
predominant values of a given culture. Therefore, the extreme
forms of physical and sexual harassment endured by gays and
lesbians could amplify frustration or extend current
definitions of violence within gay/lesbian relationships.
Hvpothesis 1. Being "out of the closet" will increase
the amount of discrimination incurred among gays and lesbians.
According to the National Gay and Lesbian Task Force over
ninety percent of gays and lesbians are victimized, harassed,
and discriminated against on the basis of sexual orientation
(Mohr, 1988). Given cultural scenario scripts are decisively
geared toward heterosexual-oriented life styles and behaviors,
discrimination toward gays/lesbians reflects the extent of
homophobia apparent in society. Discrimination based on
sexual orientation serves as a stress producing mechanism
within gay/lesbian relationships, amplifying frustration and
125
aggression. Physical or sexual violence or harassment of
homosexuals because of their sexual orientation may be
conducive as an arousal stimulus toward perpetrating violence,
or serve to displace aggression in their own intimate
relationships.
Hypothesis 2. High levels of discrimination will
increase the perpetration of violence in gay and lesbian
relationships.
Discrimination may also serve as a means to broaden an
individual's definition of victimization. By employing a
symbolic interactionist perspective to explore discrimination
and relational violence, for the focus of such a theory would
examine "the different meanings of violence people hold ...
and the consequences of some meanings in situational settings"
(Bersani and Chen, 1988; p. 65). The violence, imposed on
gays and lesbians via discrimination, may actually broaden
their symbolic criteria of victimization; and, perhaps they
will redefine similar episodes, occurring in their
relationship, as being violent.
Another potential correlate of abuse in gay/lesbian
relationships is power. Bologna, Waterman, and Dawson (1987)
suggested a perceived lack of power was associated with both
victimization and perpetration in lesbian relationships. Both
physical and sexual discrimination undermines the perception
of social power for both gays and lesbians. Given
heterosexuals maintain the power in society, and enforce their
126
power via discrimination, prejudices based on sexual
orientation eventually disrupt the relational quality of gay
and lesbian relationships. Further, the detrimental effects
of discrimination on gay and lesbian relationships cuts across
all power or socio-economic levels. For example, Kelly and
Warfshafsky (1987) reported no significant relationship
between status differentials and incidence of abuse in
gay/lesbian relationships.
Hypothesis 3. High levels of discrimination will
increase physical victimization in gay and lesbian
relationships.
Hypothesis 4. High levels of discrimination will
increase sexual victimization in gay and lesbian
relationships.
The relationship between discrimination and victimization
may be a product of several sources. One explanation could be
a sensitization effect, whereby gay males and lesbians who had
been victimized by heterosexuals may be more likely to define
themselves as victims in their own intimate relationships. A
second explanation could stem from an unmeasured intervening
variable, such as deviant coping mechanisms. Further,
discriminated homosexuals may suffer from low self-esteem and
seek out potentially violent relationships because they have
been conditioned to accept deviance as a lifestyle. A third
explanation could be assertiveness.
127
Interrelational scripts
It is posited the more violent the relationship, the more
likely the relationship will be unstable. It is assumed
violence in a relationship will detract from the stability of
the relationship, as a whole. Stemming from this postulation,
is a further inquiry. If violence has no significant effect
on the stability of gay/lesbian relationships, then it might
imply the gay and lesbian subculture leans toward a high
approval of violence within their relationships, supporting
the subcultural violence thesis.
Because gays and lesbians tend to maintain "closed"
relationships, the balancing of autonomy and attachment is a
more prevalent concern for homosexual relationships (Peplau,
Cochran, Rook, and Padesky, 1978; Kreston and Bepko, 1980).
As a "closed" system, relational, discrimination faced by
lesbians and gays may foster higher degrees of attachment,
which may prove to be conflictual when problems of autonomy
surface (McCandlish, 1982; Lindenbaum, 1985). Therefore, gay
and lesbian partners tend to be more dependent on their
partners within their relationships. Renzetti (1988)
demonstrated dependency appears to be a strong correlate with
abuse in lesbian relationships. As a lesbian or, perhaps, a
gay male partner attempts to achieve autonomy, and if their
partner is highly dependent and aggressive, the relationship
may become abusive. The correlate between dependency and
128
abuse may further be extenuated when discrimination is
encountered by gays and lesbians.
According to Strube (1988), the decision to leave an
abusive relationship is, in part, based on perceived
entrapment, learned helplessness, and a cost/benefit analyses.
Given there are few mediation agencies and support services
for victims of gay and lesbian abusive relationships, many gay
males and lesbians, who are sexually or physically abused,
must feel entrapped by their relationship and by society's
general disapproval of their sexual life style. Further, gays
and lesbians do not have the social resources available to
them to promote a perceptual benefit for leaving an abusive
relationship. The lack of available resources also fosters a
feeling of learned helplessness. Therefore, it would not be
surprising to find gays and lesbians are less likely to leave
abusive relationships. However, according to Celles (1976)
heterosexual women who had incurred severe or frequent abuse
were more likely to seek intervention, with separation and
divorce as the most prominent forms of intervention.
Hypothesis 5. Perpetration of violence, physical
victimization, and sexual victimization will increase the
instability of gay and lesbian relationships.
Intrapsychic scripts
It is hypothesized the self-esteem of the subjects will
moderate the extent discrimination will influence the level of
129
abuse (whether physical, sexual, or battery) and the stability
of the relationship after the violent episode. Self-esteem
can be conceptualized as a variable at the intrapsychic level,
conducive to explain sexual and physical abuse, and
perpetration of violence in intimate relationships. Low self-
esteem, in the violence literature, has been substantiated as
a characteristic of batterers or aggressors (Gayford, 1975;
Goldstein and Rosenbaum, 1985). Low self-esteem has also been
correlated with being a victim of abuse (Walker, 1979;
Carlson, 1977). It is apparent self-esteem, as a personality
construct, is associated with violence, whether through
perpetration or victimization. Therefore, it is posited self-
esteem, as a moderating variable, will influence the
relationship between discrimination and perpetrating, or being
a victim of aggression. Lesbians and gay males with low self-
esteem will be more likely to adopt a victim perspective
whether through discriminatory acts or abusive relationships.
Further, lesbians and gay males with low self-esteem will be
more likely to abuse their partners, because they will not
possess the means to adequately deal with the stress
associated with discrimination.
According to Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978), one
means for assessing learned helplessness is self-esteem. In
their research (Abramson, Garber, and Seligman, 1980) proposed
self-blame for uncontrollable events are more likely to
manifest low self-esteem. Victims of discrimination, a
130
relatively uncontrollable event from the perception of gays
and lesbians, are more likely be influenced by learned
helplessness. Therefore, lesbians and gay males who are
physically discriminated against are more likely to consider
themselves as victims in abusive relationships.
Hypothesis 6. Gays and lesbians with high self-esteem,
when compared to those with low self-esteem, will be less
influenced by the association between discrimination and
perpetration of violence, physical victimization, and sexual
victimization.
Hypothesized models
Figure 1 illustrates the three proposed models of this
study. The three models differ regarding the paths from
discrimination to the three forms of violence examined in this
study. Model A illustrates the model examining the effect of
discrimination of physical victimization. Model B diagrams
the model analyzing the effects of discrimination on
perpetration of violence. Model C indicates the effect of
discrimination on sexual aggression. To reiterate, it is
hypothesized being "out" will increase the probability for
gays and lesbians to be victims of physical/sexual
discrimination by heterosexuals (Arrows A). Gays and
lesbians, who are victims of violent forms of discrimination,
will be more apt to define aggressive episodes in their
relationships as violent (Arrow B) or sexually abusive (Arrow
131
C). Further, discrimination may increase stress within gay
and lesbian relationships, which could amplify aggressive
tendencies; consequently, discrimination may increase the
probability gays and lesbians resort to using violence within
their relationships (Arrow D). And, those gays and lesbian
who incur violence within their relationships will be more
likely to maintain relatively unstable relationships (Arrows
E). Finally, it is posited gays and lesbians with high self-
esteem will not be as likely to be victimized or perpetrate
violence in, their relationships as gays and lesbians with low
self-esteem (Arrows F).
Figure 1 about here
132
METHODS
Measurement
The key variables in this study include: being "out"
(disclosure), physical and sexual discrimination, physical
violence, sexual aggression, the effect of the violent episode
or sexual aggressive act on the relationship, and self-esteem.
Phvsical and sexual discrimination. Physical and
sexual discrimination were assessed by the summing of two
general dichotomous items. Both items attempted to ascertain
the extent of victimization by heterosexuals. The
introductory statement for the two items read; "The following
list are examples of things that could have happened to you.
For each item, circle either yes or no. Circle yes only if
this directly happened to you AND if it occurred because
others believed you are a gay/lesbian." Two items were
selected from a list of possible discriminatory outcomes. The
initial list included: physical, sexual, occupational,
housing, property-related, and familial. Both physical and
sexual discrimination were targeted because of the direct
violent nature of both forms of discrimination. Though the
other forms of discrimination faced by gays and lesbian, may
constitute relational and psychological stress, they are not
necessarily direct, physical confrontations.
Physical discrimination was measured as a dichotomous
category (either "yes" or "no") to the following question:
"Someone threatened to or did physically hurt me (verbal
133
threats, hitting, slap, punch, kick, beat-up, strike with an
object, or use a weapon) because I am gay/lesbian." Sexual
discrimination was assessed in a similar fashion. Respondents
selected either the category of "yes" or "no" to the following
statement: "Someone threatened to or did sexually assault me
or attempt to assault me because I am gay/lesbian (i.e. verbal
threats or made an unsuccessful or successful attempts to
touch breasts or genitals, or force oral, anal, or vaginal
intercourse)." The scores for the respondents were added to
compile a physical/sexual discrimination scale, ranging in
possible responses from 0 to 2. The two items were correlated
at .372 for lesbians, and .268 for gay males. Both
correlations were significant at the .01 level.
Out. Being "out" served as an indicator of gay
identity disclosure and lifestyle activity. Respondents
selected one of five possible response categories to the
following question; "On a scale of 1-5, how would you rate
yourself as 'out of the closet'." The categories included
l="very out", 2="somewhat out", 3="out", 4="somewhat in", and
5="in the closet."
Self-esteem. Self-esteem was assessed by five, general
questions adapted from Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale
(Rosenberg, 1965). The five items consisted of "on the whole
I am satisfied with myself"; "at times I think I am no good at
all"; "I certainly feel useless at times"; "I feel that I am a
person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others"; and
134
"I take a positive attitude towards myself." Respondents were
requested to select one of five possible response categories
to indicate the extent with which they strongly agreed,
agreed, were unsure, disagreed, or strongly disagreed with the
accorded statements. The items were then summed to create a
scale, with possible values of 5 to 25. A factor analysis
demonstrated all the items loaded fairly well on a single
factor. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for males was .746,
and for females it was .836.
Physical tactic conflict scale. The physical conflict
scale was devised to isolate using violence and being a victim
of violence. The scale was a modified version of the Conflict
Tactic Scale (Straus, Celles, and Steinmetz, 1980). Each
conflict scale ascertained the extent of physical violence
incurred during a gay and lesbian relationship. With regards
to being a victim of violence, participants responded to the
question; "Please indicate the type of violence used toward
you in an intimate homosexual/lesbian relationships (circle
all that apply)." Further, respondents selected the various
forms of violence which applied to the statement: "Please
indicate the types of violence YOU used against your partner
in an intimate homosexual/lesbian relationship. The types of
violent episodes considered for each tactic scale included;
threats, pushing, slapping, punching, striking with an object,
and using a weapon. Each conflict tactic was assessed by a
dichotomous category with l="experienced", and 2="not
135
experienced." None of the respondents indicated they had used
a weapon against their partner during the course of their
relationship history. Therefore, using a weapon was omitted
from the use of violence scale. The various modes of
violence, for each scale, were summed to create an expression
of physical confrontation in gay and lesbian relationships.
The scale results ranged in possible values from 0 to 6 for
the physical victimization scale, and 0 to 5 for the use of
violence scale. The alpha coefficient for the victimization
scale was .838 for gay males, and .742 for lesbians. The
alpha coefficient on the use of violence scale was .787 for
gay males, and .745 for lesbians. A factor analysis conducted
on both scales for both gays and lesbians indicated all items
loaded on a single factor, respective of perpetration and
victimization.
Sexual aggression. The sexual aggression scale
initially consisted of 12 items. The items consisted of a
vast range of possible methods used to gain sexual behaviors
from the subject. The indicators included such methods as:
intoxication, threats to terminate the relationship, threats
to disclose negative information, guilt, physical detainment,
use of false promises, use of lies, persistent physical
attempts, being held down, use of a weapon, threats of
physical force, and use of physical force (Waldner-Haugrud and
Magruder, 1991). Respondents were asked to indicate the most
intimate behavior that occurred with a person they were
136
involved with in a lesbian/homosexual relationship despite
their wish not to participate in the sexual behavior. The
response categories for the items were ranked and coded as a
hierarchy of intense sexual involvement, where O="not
applicable", l="kissing, breast/buttocks fondling, genital
fondling", and 2="oral/anal/sexual intercourse." The method
of using a weapon was dismissed from the scale because none of
the respondents indicated this type of event had occurred. In
a factor analysis on the scale, all items loaded moderately on
a single factor, except for physical detainment among
lesbians. Due to the item's substantive strength, and to be
consistent across sexes, physical detainment remained in the
scale despite its poor factor loading. It was believed
physical detainment, although uncorrelated with the other
items in the scale among lesbians, contributed as a extensive
measure of sexual aggression. The eleven items were summed to
composite a scale, indicative of the extent of sexual
behaviors incurred by various means based on undesired
participation. The range of the scale consisted of possible
values, extending from 0 to 20. The alpha reliability
coefficients for the sexual aggression scale were .762 for
lesbians, and .746 for gay males.
Relational stability. The effect of the violent
episode on the relationship, whether inflicted upon
(physically or sexually) or caused by the respondent, was
assessed by three similar questions. Each relational
137
stability question directly followed the victim of violence
scale, the use of violence scale, and the sexual aggression
scale. The effect of the violent or sexually aggressive
episode was assessed by the following question: "How did the
incident affect your relationship?" Respondents selected 1 of
five possible categories for each aggressive conflict scale.
The categories included; O="not applicable", l="no change",
2="relationship became worse, but still involved",
3="relationship improved" and 4="relationship ended." The
scale was recoded to be indicative of a relational stability
continuum. Therefore, the higher the score on the relational
stability index, the more the subject was involved in the
relationship after the violent episode; consequently, the
lower the score the more likely the subject was to abandon the
relationship.
Procedures
In accord with the exploratory nature of this study,
frequencies on all the key variables were tabulated to profile
the extent of violence occurring in gay and lesbian
relationships. Second, the self-esteem scale was dichotomized
-' » .
at the median to represent a four cell typology: high self-
esteem gay males, high self-esteem lesbians, low self-esteem
gay males, and low self-esteem lesbians. Third, t-tests were
conducted within and between typological groups to test for
significant differences for all the variables in the study.
138
save self-esteem. Fourth, four sets of correlation matrices,
including all the variables in the study were computed among
gay males and lesbians by high and low self-esteem. Finally,
stacked structural equation models were employed to test for
significant differences along the various paths in the
hypothesized models, controlling for both sex and self-esteem.
139
RESULTS
The frequency distributions on the variables in the study
yielded interesting findings on the extent of victimization
faced by both gays and lesbians. In general, 46 percent of
lesbians and 29 percent of males recounted they were
physically victimized during the course of their
relationships. The maximum values for both males and females
was 6, on a possible six point scale. The analysis on the
scale was continued on each individual item to further
document the extent of victimization. For example, 32
lesbians and 32 gay males reported being threatened by their
partner during the course of a past relationship. Further, 45
lesbians and 30 males asserted being pushed by their partner.
One quarter of the lesbians were slapped, and similarly,
fifteen percent of lesbians were punched; consequently,
approximately seventeen percent of gay males were slapped, and
fifteen percent reported being punched by their partners.
Only 9 lesbians and 11 males accounted being struck by an
object. Finally, only 3 lesbians and 4 males recounted being
assaulted with a weapon by their partner in a relationship.
With regard to the sample using violence during the
course of their past relationships, a parallel frequency
distribution was noted. As a whole, 37 percent of lesbians
and only 22 percent of gay males reported using some sort of
violence in their relationships. Both males and females
yielded a maximum value of five on the 6-point scale. Fifteen
140
percent of lesbians, and eleven percent of gay males stated
they had threatened to use violence against their partner.
Twenty-seven percent of lesbians had pushed their partners;
consequently, slightly less lesbians (seventeen percent)
recounted slapping their partners. Eleven percent of the
males in the sample admitted to pushing their partners, and
twelve percent of the males had slapped their partners.
Twelve percent of lesbians, and nine percent of males
confessed to punching their partner at least once in their
relationships. Only four lesbians acknowledged striking their
partner with an object, while none stated they had used a
weapon against their partner. Finally, 5 males reported
striking their partner with an object, and none of the males
recounted using a weapon during his intimate relationship.
The sexual aggression items revealed a similar trend, as
evident in the conflict tactic items. In general, 46 percent
of lesbians and 56 percent of gay males stated they had been
sexually victimized during the course of their relationships.
For lesbians, the maximum scale value was 12, and for males it
was slightly higher at 16. An examination for each of the
various items detailed the extent of sexual victimization
among males and lesbians. Eighteen percent of lesbians and
nineteen percent of gay males reported being purposely
intoxicated by a partner to engage in unwanted sexual
participation. Four percent of lesbians and gay males stated
their partners had threatened to terminate their relationship
141
unless they were to participate in unwanted kissing, petting,
oral, anal, or vaginal intercourse. Only one lesbian and two
gay males admitted to engaging in unwanted sexual activity
because their partner had threatened to disclose negative
information about them. Twenty-two percent of lesbians and
twenty-four percent of males reported being made to feel
guilty in order to participate in forced sexual relationships.
Approximately four percent of lesbians and eleven percent of
males stated they were physically detained in order to take
part in unwanted sexual activities. A relatively high percent
of lesbians (twenty-seven percent) and males (thirty-five
percent) recounted being victimized by persistent physical
attempts in order to be "turned on." Sixteen percent of
lesbians and twenty-five percent of males asserted being lied
to in order for a partner to obtain unwanted sexual activity.
Similarly, nine percent of lesbians and nine percent of males
acknowledged the use of false promises employed by past
partners to procure undesired sexual relations. Although not
included in the scale, five percent of lesbians and nine
percent of males asserted they were physically held down in
order to obtain unwanted sexual activity. None of the sample,
either lesbians or males, admitted to having a weapon used
against them in order to gain unwarranted sexual
participation. Only one lesbian and five males engaged in
unwanted sexual relations because their partners had
threatened physical force. Finally, three lesbians and seven
142
gay males stated they had engaged in undesired sexual activity
because their partners had employed physical coercion.
Regarding the stability of the relationships after the
incidence of physical victimization, the majority of males (17
percent) who had endured physical victimization stated the
relationship had ended. Five percent of males reported the
relationship had worsened, but were still involved in the
relationship. Six percent recounted there was no change in
the relationship, two percent stated the relationship
improved. Twenty percent of lesbians reported their
relationship ended after the physical victimization. Seven
percent of lesbians stated there was no change in the
relationship. Fifteen percent asserted the relationship
worsened, but remained involved. And only four percent of
lesbians reported the relationship improved after the violent
incident.
Nine percent of males asserted there was no change in the
relationship after they had employed violent tactics. Five
percent stated the relationship worsened, however they
remained involved. Only one percent of males reported the
relationship had improved. And eight percent of the gay male
sample recounted the relationship had ended after the violent
episode. In contrast, thirteen percent of lesbians reported
their relationship worsened, yet remained involved, after they
had employed some act of violence. Twelve percent of lesbians
stated there was no change in their relationship after they
143
had used a violent tactic. Four percent of lesbians asserted
the relationship had improved, and nine percent of lesbians
stated the relationship ended.
For sexually aggressive relationships, nine percent of
lesbians reported the relationship remained the same after the
sexually aggressive incident. Only two percent stated the
relationship had improved. The majority of lesbians (27
percent of the lesbian sample) asserted the relationship had
worsened. Only one percent of the gay males asserted the
sexually aggressive relationship had improved. Fifteen
percent stated there was no change in the relationship after
the aggressive episode. And, again, the majority of the gay
males (twenty-seven percent) recounted the relationship had
worsened.
The distribution on the other variables in the study
indicated eighty percent of lesbians considered themselves to
be "out", "somewhat out", or "very out" of the closet;
consequently, eighty-seven percent of the males reported being
"out", "somewhat out", or "very out" with regard to their
sexual identity. One third of lesbians and forty-seven
percent of males recounted being hurt or physically threatened
by discrimination. The average number of assaults reported by
those lesbians and males who were harassed were 7 and 13,
respectively. The incidence of physical discrimination ranged
from 1 to fifty for lesbians, and up to 98 for males.
Fourteen percent of lesbians and nineteen percent of males
144
stated being sexually assaulted or threatened by
discriminatory harassment. Lesbians reported an average of
ten sexual assaults; however, gay males contended an average
of only 4 sexual assaults.
The self-esteem scale yielded fairly normative results
for both males and lesbians. The range of values for lesbians
extended from six to twenty-five, with a mean value of 18.8.
For males, the values on the self-esteem scale ranged from 10
to 25, with a mean of 19.3. Therefore, to create two groups
representing high and low self-esteem, the scale was
dichotomized at the mean/median. Scores of 18 and lower were
considered to be indicative of low self-esteem, and scores
consisting of nineteen and higher were believed to represent
individuals with high self-esteem.
Table 1 about here
Table 1 presents the means and the standard deviations
for the assorted variables in the study, by sex and self-
esteem. A test of significant differences, controlling for
sex and self-esteem, yielded no significant differences among
lesbians possessing high or low self-esteem. However, a few
significant differences were found between gay males with high
or low self-esteem. Low self-esteem males reported
experiencing significantly more instances of physical
victimization, sexual victimization, and using violence in
145
their relationships than males with high self-esteem.
Further, low self-esteem males were more likely to recount
they stayed within the sexually aggressive relationship than
males with high self-esteem. And low self-esteem males stated
they maintained the relationships in which they used physical
violence. Among low self-esteem groups, males were
significantly more likely to report they remained in
relationships which they were physically victimized, and they
had employed violent tactics. Among high self-esteem groups,
males again stated they were significantly more likely to be
physically victimized and use violence than high self-esteem
lesbians.
Tables 2 and 3 about here
Tables 2 and 3 present the correlation matrices of the
variables used in the study for lesbians and gay males,
controlling for high and low self-esteem. Among lesbians with
low self-esteem when contrasted to lesbians with high self-
esteem (refer to Table 2), being "out" maintained a'higher
degree of correlation between discrimination, physical
victimization, and the degree of stability for relationships
where lesbians were physically victimized and employed violent
tactics. This finding was also characteristic of gay males
(refer to Table 3). Therefore being "out" or public displays
of identity were more readily associated with violence with
146
homosexuals possessing low self-esteem. However, gay males
with low self-esteem maintained a higher degree of association
between being "out", sexual aggression, and the stability of
the relationship after a sexually aggressive incident than
lesbians with low self-esteem.
Incurring physical discrimination was moderately
associated with all the variables in the analysis for lesbians
with low self-esteem when compared to lesbians with high self-
esteem. Again, this trend was repeated among the gay males.
The correlation between discrimination and sexual aggression
relational stability was higher among gay males with low self-
esteem than lesbians with low self-esteem. Further, the
correlations between discrimination and the other variables in
the study were higher among gay males with high self-esteem
when contrasted with lesbians with high self-esteem.
Physical victimization was highly correlated with using
violence and sexual aggression for both high and low self-
esteem lesbians, and gay males. This finding supports a
reciprocal effect in violent tactics, although it was not an
strongly correlated with sexual aggression among lesbians.
Therefore, relational violence among gay males is more likely
to be manifested in sexual relationships when paralleled to " •
lesbians. Further, physical victimization was highly
correlated with the relational stability variables. In
particular, the correlation between physical victimization and
relational stability for sexually aggressive relationships was
147
twice as high among lesbians with high self-esteem than
lesbians with low self-esteem. The association between using
violence and sexual aggression was twice as high for lesbians
with high self-esteem when compared to lesbians with low self-
esteem. Using violence maintained a comparatively higher
correlation with sexual aggression for gay males with low
self-esteem than lesbians with low self-esteem.
In short, the correlation matrices suggest violence, all
forms, incurred in a relationship was negatively, and highly
associated with relational stability. Therefore, gays and
lesbians are not likely to maintain the relationship after
being physically victimized, using violence, or enduring
sexual aggression. Further, the relatively high correlations
among the conflict tactic scales indicate violence leads to
violence. This effect was especially warranted between
physical victimization and the use of violence, for both gay
males and lesbians regardless of self-esteem. However, gay
males with low self-esteem were more likely to transcend their
victimization from physically aggressive relationships to
sexually aggressive relationships, more so than lesbians with
low self-esteem.
Figures 2 and 3 about here
Figures 2, and 3 illustrate the findings of the proposed
models for lesbians and gay males, controlling for high and
148
low self-esteem. For lesbians (refer to Figure 2), being out
was positively and significantly related to discrimination for
lesbians with both high and low self-esteem. Naturally, this
finding was evident in all three models displaying the effects
of various violent tactics.
However, among gay males (refer to Figure 3), being "out"
was positively and significantly related to enduring
discrimination for low self-esteem males, but not
significantly for males with high self-esteem. Respondents,
describing themselves as being "out of the closet", were more
likely to endure both physical and sexual discrimination from
heterosexuals, except among gay males with high self-esteem.
This trend was evident across all three models.
For lesbians, discrimination was positively related to
physical victimization (Model A), using violence (Model B),
and sexual aggression (Model C); however, this finding was
only significant among lesbians with low self-esteem. Fixing
this path to be equal between lesbians with high and low self-
esteem, significantly changed the chi square of the stacked
model. This significant moderating effect was only true for
the effect of discrimination on using violence (Model B), and
being a victim of physical violence (Model A) [Model A; change
in chi square (1 degree of freedom) = 3.59, p < .1; Model B:
change in chi square (1 degree of freedom) = 4.55, p < .05].
This significant change in the chi square value indicated a
significant contrast between lesbians with low self-esteem and
149
lesbians with high self-esteem. A significant change in chi
square was not noted regarding the moderating effect of self-
esteem on the relationship between discrimination and sexual
aggression (Model C).
For gay males, discrimination was positively related to
physical victimization (Model A), using violence (Model B),
and sexual aggression (Model C). The trend was significant
regardless of controlling for high and low self-esteem. There
was no significant change in chi square values when the paths
were set to be equal between high and low self-esteem among
the gay males in the study. Therefore, a significant
moderating effect of self-esteem on discrimination effecting
physical victimization (Model A), using violence (Model B), or
sexual aggression (Model C) for gay males was not confirmed.
All three of the conflict tactic scales (physical
victimization, using violence, and sexual aggression) were
negatively related to the extent of relational stability after
the violent episode. This finding was highly significant
among both gay males and lesbians, after controlling for self-
esteem. This finding indicated gay males and lesbians with
either ^igh or low self-esteem did not maintain their
relationship after the violent incidence. Further, the more
violent the incident, or the more extensive the violence, the
less likely the relationship continued.
150
DISCUSSION
This study demonstrated the violent effects of physical
and sexual discrimination on the intimate relationships of
gays and lesbians. The more identified gays and lesbians were
as homosexuals, or being "out", the more likely they were to
suffer from violent discrimination and abuse by heterosexuals.
Further, the effects of the discrimination were manifested in
relatively violent forms within their relationships.
Discrimination was positively related to the extent of
physical victimization gays and lesbians reported.
Discrimination also lead to an elevated level of perpetrating
violent acts in the relationship by gays and lesbians. And
finally, discrimination was positively related to the extent
of sexual aggression incurred by gays and lesbians in their
relationships. These aggressive encounters undermined the
stability of the relationships. The respondents indicated the
more violent the relationship, the more likely the
relationship was terminated. The moderating effects of self-
esteem were only significant in defining the relationship of
discrimination with both physical victimization and the use of
violence among lesbians. In short, discrimination was not
significantly related to either physical victimization and the
use of violence among lesbians with high self-esteem; however,
the relationships were significantly associated among lesbians
with low self-esteem.
151
The proposed models offered support to the sexual script
perspective, by implicating the violent effects of
discrimination on gay and lesbian relationships. Given the
cultural scenario scripts in American society are skewed
toward heterosexuality, tumultuous discrimination serves as a
means for reinforcing this dominant sexual perspective. The
increase in hate crimes, directed toward gays and lesbians, in
recent years have confirmed this proposition. It should also
be noted, physical discrimination is more probable among
individuals who identify with the gay culture. By being "out"
homosexuals place themselves at risk for being victimized by
the violent nature of heterosexual scripts. The effects of
physical discrimination on gay and lesbians relationships
tends to further violent outcomes. Unfortunately, the rise
and legitimation of prejudice towards homosexuals increases
the extent of violence in gay and lesbian relationships.
Although this study documented a relatively high amount
of violence, either physical or sexual, this study did not
find support of the subcultural approval of violence
hypothesis. The strong association between the various
dimensions of violence and relational stability indicated that
gays and lesbians who incurred some form of violence were
likely to terminate the relationship. This finding inferred
gays and lesbians do not, in general, maintain or accept
violent relationships. Theoretically, it was considered gays
and lesbians might be prone to a general acceptance of
152
violence in their relationships due to the stigmatized nature
of their life style. This was not, however, substantiated.
Another theoretical consideration stemmed from the
autonomy/dependency correlate found in past literature. It
appeared gays and lesbians do not confront problems in leaving
violent relationships based on problems with overdependency,
lack of perceived resources, learned helplessness, and
entrapment. However, further research in the area of leaving
an abusive gay and lesbian relationship is needed. The
results of this study are only tentative findings based on
cross-sectional data. More extensive quantitative and
qualitative data are necessary to rule out the influence of
mediating correlates which impair or foster leaving an abusive
gay and lesbian relationship.
Very few significant gender differences were established
in this study. The only significant association, after
controlling for gender, was among the intercorrelations of the
various violent tactic scales. Gay males were more likely to
report a strong interrelationship between physical
victimization, sexual victimization, and using violence. In
contrast, physical victimization and the use of violence were
highly correlated among lesbians; however, sexual aggression
was not as strongly correlated with the former conflict
scales. Future analyses on sexual aggression among lesbians
should focus careful attention on physical detainment, which
was excluded from this study. This finding may be the result
153
of an emphasis placed on sexuality among gay males. Gay males
may utilize a sexual script which accentuates the role of
sexuality, linking it with other aspects of a relationship, at
least more so than lesbians. In short, violence appears to be
highly reciprocal in gay and lesbian relationships, given the
strong correlations between using violence and being a victim
of violence.
In conclusion, the violence in gay and lesbian
relationships mirror heterosexual relationships. Abuse is
reciprocal, and individuals who are abused maintained unstable
relationships. However, gays and lesbians are confronted with
a more divisive conflict, discrimination. The more
homosexuals exhibit their identity as a gay or lesbian, the
more likely they are to incur the scripted wrath of the
dominant, heterosexual culture. And with this discrimination,
gays and lesbians are more likely to endure elevated forms of
violence in their own relationships.
154
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Abramson, L. Y., Garber, J., and Seligman, M. E. P. (1980).
Learned helplessness in humans: An Attributional
analysis. In J. Garber and M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.),
Human Helplessness: Theory and Applications (pp. 3-34).
New York: Academic Press.
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., and Teasdale, J. (1978).
Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and
reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-
74.
Bersani, Carl A., and Chen, Huey-Tsyh. (1988). Sociological
perspectives in family violence. In V. B. Van Hasselt,
R. L. Morrison, A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook
of Family Violence (pp. 58-87). New York: Plenum Press.
Bologna, M. J., Waterman, C. K., and Dawson, L. J. (1987).
Violence in gay male and lesbian relationships:
Implications for practitioners and policy makers. Paper
presented at the Third National Conference for Family
Violence Researchers, Durham, NH, July.
Brand, P. A., and Kidd, A. H. (1986). Frequency of physical
aggression in ,heterosexual and female homosexual dyads.
Psychological Reports, 59, 1307-1313.
Carlson, B. E. (1977). Battered women and their assailants..
Social Work, 22, 455-461.
Duncan, David F. (1990). Prevalence of sexual assault
victimization among heterosexual and gay/lesbian
university students. Psychological Reports, 66, 65-66.
Gayford, J. (1975). Wife battering: A preliminary survey of
100 cases. British Medical Journal, 1, 195-197.
Gelles, R. J. (1976). Abused wives: Why do they stay?
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 659-668.
Goldenstein, D., and Rosenbaum, A. (1985). An evaluation of
the self-esteem of maritally violent men. Family
Relations, 4, 425-428.
Kelly, E. E., and Warshafsky, L. (1987). Partner abuse in
gay male and lesbian couples. Paper presented at the
Third National Conference for Family Violence
Researchers, Durham, NH, July.
155
Krestan, J., and Bepko, C. S. (1980). The problem of fusion
in the lesbian relationship. Family Process, 19, 277-
289.
Liddenbaum, J. P. (1985). The shattering of an illusion:
The problem of competition in lesbian relationships.
Feminist Studies, 11, 85-103.
McCandlish, B. M. (1982). Therapeutic issues with lesbian
couples. Journal of Homosexuality, 7, 71-78.
Mohr, Richard D. (1988). Gay Justice: A Study of Ethics,
Society, and Law. New York: Columbia University Press.
Peplau, L. A., Cochran, S., Rook, K., and Padesky, C. (1978).
Loving women: Attachment and autonomy in lesbian
relationships. Journal of Social Issues, 34, 84-100.
Renzetti, C. (1988). Violence in lesbian relationships: A
preliminary analysis of causal factors. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 3, 381-399.
Renzetti, C. (1989). Building a second closet; Third party
responses to victims of lesbian partner abuse. Family
Relations, 38, 157-163.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Schilit, Rebecca, Gwat-Yong Lie, and Marilyn Montagne.
(1990). Substance use as a correlate of violence in
intimate lesbian relationships. Journal of
Homosexuality, 19, 51-65.
Straus, Murray, Celles, Richard, and Steinmetz, Suzanne.
(1980). Behind Closed Doors: Violence in the American
Family. New York: Doubleday.
Strube, Michael J. (1988). The decision to leave an abusive
relationship: Empirical evidence and theoretical.issues.
Psychological Bulletin, 104, 236-250.
Waldner-Haugrud, Lisa, and Magruder, Brian. (1991). Male and
female sexual victimization in dating relationships:
justification for the use of gender-neutral scales.
Presented at the Annual Midwest Sociological Society
Meeting, Des Moines, Iowa.
Walker, L. E. (1979). The Battered Woman. New York: Harper
and Row.
156
Wolfgang, M. E. and Ferracuti, F. (1967). The Subculture of
Violence.. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Wolfgang, M. E., and Ferracuti, F. (1982). The Subculture of
Violence (2nd ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
157
Model A:
Being "out" Physical
victimization
A
B E
Physical/Sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
Self-Esteem
Model B:
Being "out" Use of
violence
Physical/Sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
Self-Esteem
Model C:
Being "out" Physical
victimization
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
Self-Esteem
Figure 1. Proposed models illustrating the relationship
between disclosure, discrimination, physical
victimization, using violence, sexual
victimization, and relational stability among gay
males and lesbians
158
Table 1. Comparison of means among all the variables in the
study by sex and self-esteem
Variables Hales Females
Low High Low High
Self-Esteem Self-Esteem Self-Esteem Self-Esteem
Being "Out*
mean 3.912 4.133 3.725 4.000
standard deviation 1.129 1.068 1.176 1.043
PieqrininatiM
mean .735 .651 .532 .483
standard deviation .745 .688 .718 .682
Physical Victimization
mean 1.088 .470^ 1.242 .948^
standard deviation 1.646 1.162 1.724 1.161
Relational Stability
mean 1.080 .817 .985^ 1.417
standard deviation 1.549 1.553 1.398 1.759
Dse of Violence
mean .853 .229® .807 .690^
standard deviation 1.417 .816 1.329 • 1.111
Relational Stability
mean 1.029 .312® .794b .900
standard deviation 1.464 .932 1.241 1.349
Sexual Victimization
mean 3.147 1.759® 2.097 1.633
standard deviation 3.311 2.662 3.217 2.490
Relational Stability , ^
mean 1.409 .918® .969 1.172
standard deviation 1.379 1.276 1.321 1.403
® significant difference at the .05 level between high and low
self-esteem groups among males and females.
^ significant difference at the .05 level between males and
females, controlling for self-esteem.
159
Table 2. Correlation matrix for all the variables in the
study among lesbians, controlling for high and low
self-esteem [N; (low self-esteem=54, high self-
esteem=72)]
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
1. Being "Out" X .370* .252 -.278 .154 -.216 .150 .049
2. Discrimination .247 X .464* -.368* .350* -.248 .417* -.176
3. Physical victimization .000 .099 X -.751* .708* -.638* .365* -.262
4. Relational Stability .086 .060 -.794* X -.620* .850* -.359* .357*
5. Using Violence -.061 .016 .694* -.649* X -.702* .100 -.189*
6. Relational Stability .093 .037 -.654* .724* -.742* X -.224 .374*
7. Sexual Aggression -.162 .126 .327* -.340* .295 -.230 X -.605*
8. Relational Stability .067 -.306 • -.432* .323* -.308 .341* -.734* X
Note: correlations for lesbians with low self-esteem appear
above the diagonal.
* significant at the .05 level.
160
Table 3. Correlation matrix for all the variables in the
study among gay males, controlling for high and low
self-esteem [N: (low self-esteem=75, high self-
esteem=92)]
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 3.
1. Being "Out" X .246 .134 -.115 .163 -.221 .200 -.205
2. Discrimination .114 X .484* -.341* .324* -.225 .490* -.410*
3. Physical Victimization .067 .330* X -.710* .772* -.527* .518* -.315
4. Relational stability .046 -.237 -.675* X -.629* .776* -.424* .334*
5. Using Violence -.021 .318 .747* -.472* X -.684* .541* -.314
6. Relational Stability .085 -.180 -.581* .550* -.714* X -.383 .286
7. Sexual Aggression .080 .333 .502* -.382* .284 -.315 X -.656*
8. Relational Stability -.095 -.128 -.237 .318 -.097 .239 -.704* X
Note: correlations for gay males with low self-esteem appear
above the diagonal.
* significant at the .05 level.
161
.109 (.239)
Model A:
Being "out" Physical
fl^.oi3(,o6i) victimization
I
.n4(.J46«) I .()75*( .7in"l «• •1561 SIM'
I .3.(0 •(.4S9')
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination instability
.*''(6)= 1.70 p=.945
GFI=.991
«^-.lOl(.lOJ)
Model B:
Being "out" use of
/
Violence «^ = .5ioi.)fi«>
.II4(.246»)X, / ., -.714•( .684») /
Y ,,--'''^318«(.324«) ./
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination instability
.*2(6) = 3.977 p =. 681
GFI=.991
R^-.in(.240 )
Model C:
/>
Being "out" ff^-:.oi3(.o6i ) Sexual
I Victimization .4,6,430,
•\ ^ .333'(.490»)
• \
Physical/sexual
Discrimination Relational
X- IT,) = 5.4.^/, =. 4')» Instability
GFI=.984
Note: standardized regression coefficients for gay males with
low self-esteem appear in parentheses.
* significant at the .05 level.
Figure 2. The impact of discriminatory stress on physical
victimization, use of violence, and sexual
victimization in gay male relationships,
controlling for high and low self-esteem
162
Model A:
Being "out" , Physical
R :-.06t(.i37) Victimization
7114'( ,711") 0)1) I tW I
.24T(..)70*)
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
(6) = 5.99 p = . 424
GFI=.970
/î^ =.004(.123)
Model B: 1/
Being "out" «^=.06i(.i37) yge of
\ j Violence
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
X- (6) - 2.36 p - . 883
GFI=.994
«^=.016(.174)
Model C:
Being "out" sexual
Victimization
/'
/ ~ , ,734 •( .605'I
y// -<I26(.4I7«) \
:
Physical/sexual Relational
Discrimination Instability
.v^ (()) = 12.270 p = . ()5()
GFI=.934
Note: standardized regression coefficients for lesbians with
low self-esteem appear in parentheses.
* significant at the .05 level
Figure 3. The impact of discriminatory stress on physical
victimization, use of violence, and sexual
victimization in lesbian relationships, controlling
for high and low self-esteem
163
GENERAL SUMMARY
Summary of Results
Each of the three articles in this dissertation has
focused on the sexual script perspective as applied to
homosexuality. The common theme in the three articles was the
examination of how cultural scenario scripts impact on the
interrelational and intrapsychic scripts for both gay males
and lesbians. Paper 1 analyzed the impact of identification
with heterosexual scripts, via the family, on the disclosure
of homosexual identities among gay and lesbian adolescents.
Paper 2 examined the diversity between cultural and
intrapsychic scripts among homosexuals and heterosexuals,
regarding the definition of attractiveness. Paper 3
investigated the negative influence of stigmatization
(discrimination) toward homosexuality, apparent in the
dominant cultural scenario script, on gay and lesbian
relationships. Each of the papers examined the role cultural
scenario scripts assert on homosexuality, whether it was
manifested in disclosure, the definition of attractiveness, or
in abusive relationships.
Disclosure of identity to the family
The model, proposed in Paper 1, asserted disclosure of
homosexual identities among gay and lesbian adolescents was
indirectly influenced by heterosexual scripts, manifested
through family identification. However, this process was
164
mediated by the extent of perceived resources which were
supportive of homosexuality, and the expression of homosexual
identities among the adolescents in the study. The model was
based on the social exchange and the sexual script
perspectives. It was posited family identification was one of
the primary socializing agents for heterosexual sexual
scripts. Such an influence could be conceived as a cost when
disclosing a gay identity. This cost was weighed against the
degree of perceived supportive resources and the exploration
of the adolescent's own homosexual identity. In short, the
family was seen as a "push" toward a heterosexual life style,
whereas expression of homosexual identities and the extent of
perceived resources represented a "pull" toward a homosexual
life style. The ratio between the heterosexual scripted
influence and the homosexual scripted influence would affect
whether or not the adolescents disclosed their identity to
their parents.
The results of the study indicated identification with
the family was negatively related to the extent of perceived
resources, which were considered by the adolescent respondents
as supportive of homosexuality. Family identification was
negatively related to the expression of homosexual identities
among both gay and lesbian adolescents. However, family
identification was not significantly related to disclosing the
identity to the parents of the respondents. The extent of
perceived homosexual-supportive resources was positively
165
associated with the expression or salience of homosexual
identities. The extent of perceived resources was also
positively associated with disclosure of gay and lesbian
identities to parents. Finally, the expression of homosexual
identities, among the gay and lesbian adolescents, was
positively related to disclosure. Few significant gender
differences were found in the general model. The one
contrast, between gay males and lesbians, was the extent of
common variance explained by the significant relationships in
the model. Identification with family, the extent of.
perceived resources, and expression of homosexual identity
explained twice the common variance in disclosure for gay male
adolescents, when compared to lesbians.
The merging of the sexual script perspective with social
exchange theory explained the process by which homosexual
adolescents disclosed their identities to their parents. In
short, identification with the sexual scripts fostered by
parents and families was not significantly linked to whether
the adolescents disclosed their identities to their parents.
However, identification with heterosexual scripts, as
socialized via the family, did significantly detract from
perceiving homosexual-supportive resources and expressing the
adolescents' identities through organization participation,
sexual contact, and friendship networks. In turn, the more
the adolescents secured and perceived various resources,
supportive of homosexuality, the higher the benefit of
166
disclosing their identity. Similarly, the more the
adolescents investigated homosexual scripts, by perceiving
extensive resources supporting homosexuality, the lower they
perceived the costs of disclosing their identities to their
parents. Social support resources facilitated the disclosure
process by socializing gay and lesbian adolescents with
appropriate homosexual sexual scripts.
By educating and resocializing gay and lesbian
adolescents with sexual scripts, favorable toward
homosexuality, provided the adolescents with the ability to
envision potential benefits of adopting their homosexual
identities. Whereas, identification with the family, dictated
the inherent costs of breaking the normative guidelines of
heterosexuality. Therefore, expression of homosexual
identities, for adolescents, may be conceptualized as the
theoretical weight associated with the costs of dissolving the
bonds of heterosexual scripts, advocated by the family, versus
the perceived benefits facilitated by homosexual-oriented
organizations and resources. The two competing forces are
best expressed by the regression coefficients between
identification with the family and the extent of perceived
supportive resources on expression of homosexual identities.
The "push/pull" mechanisms, affecting the expression of
homosexual identities among gay and lesbian adolescents,
reflects the innate struggle between heterosexual and
homosexual sexual scripts. Again, identification with the
167
family "pushes" the adolescent away from expressing their
homosexual identities, via the sanctions toward homosexuality
as indoctrinated by heterosexual sexual scripts. While
homosexual-oriented resources "pull" the adolescent toward
expressing their homosexual identities by resocializing the
adolescent with homosexual sexual scripts.
Definitions of attractiveness and sensuality
The second article in this project focused on the
differences in definitions of attractiveness among homosexuals
and heterosexuals. The general assertion posited by the
study, contended the definition of attractiveness was based on
shared cultural scenario scripts. It was asserted males and
females maintain different cultural scenario scripts which
guide the definition of attractiveness for the appropriate
sex. For this study, it was assumed cultural scenario scripts
comprise the unwritten rules and guidelines in society which
dictate the normative standards associated with
attractiveness. The study posited heterosexual females and
gay males would share similar cultural scenario scripts
regarding the definition of attractiveness for males.
Similarly, lesbians and heterosexual males would share
corresponding cultural scenario scripts guiding the definition
of attractiveness for women.
Further, the study asserted several conceptual and
methodological considerations for studying the definition of
168
attractiveness. First, researchers on attractiveness should
consider the scripted difference between sensuality and
attractiveness. The study posited the definition of
attractiveness is standardized, based on the dominant cultural
scenario script. Whereas, the composite definition of
sensuality is individual-based, guided by an individual's
intrapsychic scripts (fantasies, desires, motivations,
arousal). Second, the study contended the definition of
attractiveness should not be limited to only physical
qualities. It was posited the definition of both
attractiveness and sensuality would be partially constructed
with personality characteristics, as well as physical
attributes.
In general, the study found no significant differences
between the definitions of attractiveness among gay males and
heterosexual females. This finding indicates gay males and
heterosexual females share a similar definition of
attractiveness, and therefore similar cultural scenario
scripts regarding the definition of attractiveness for men.
However, significant differences were discovered when
contrasting the definitions of sensuality among homosexual
males and heterosexual females. This finding suggests that
though gay males and heterosexual females share similar
cultural scenario scripts, they significantly differ regarding
the definition of sensuality, or the composition of their
respective intrapsychic scripts.
169
In the comparison of the definition of attractiveness
among heterosexual males and lesbians, a significant gender
difference was established. Also, significant gender
differences were found in definitions of sensuality among
heterosexual males and lesbians. Both findings indicate
lesbians and heterosexual males share different cultural
scenario scripts and intrapsychic scripts for both the
definitions of attractiveness and sensuality. Lesbians tended
to focus more on personality constructs regarding
attractiveness. Heterosexual males, on the other hand,
concentrated on more physical attributes, such as the face
and, in particular, the body.
It could be inferred from these results that heterosexual
males and lesbians possess different cultural scenario scripts
at the subcultural level. It appears the lesbian subculture
prizes personality qualities, such as warmth and affection,
over physical attributes, such as the face and body.
Consequently, the cultural scenario script guiding the
definition of attractiveness among heterosexual males targets
physical characteristics over personality attributes. Perhaps
this difference in scripts stems from a gender contrast
between lesbians and heterosexual males. Based on traditional
notions of gender scripts, women should view warm and
affectionate women as being attractive (Dew, 1985). Given
cultural scenario scripts define women who are sensitive and
warm as attractive, it is not surprising that lesbians would
170
place preference on those women possessing such attributes.
However, this distinction in scripting appears to be gender
biased. Heterosexual males maintain a cultural script which
emphasizes physical characteristics above personality
attributes. Again, this is not necessarily surprising. A
further examination of the results indicate sensuality and
attractiveness scripts for heterosexual males tend to overlap.
There is much similarity in the definitions of attractiveness
and sensuality among heterosexual males. This finding
suggests heterosexual males do not separate the constructs of
attractiveness and sensuality. To this grouping, the two
constructs are synonymous.
The general conclusion of the study suggests heterosexual
males, heterosexual females, and homosexual males place a
strong emphasis on physical attributes in defining
attractiveness. However, lesbians appear to prioritize
personality characteristics over physical elements when
ordering attractive qualities. Further, heterosexual females,
homosexual males, and lesbians lean toward personality
qualities in defining sensual features. However, heterosexual
males tend toward physical attributes in defining sensuality.
The broader message of the scripted perspective, when given
these general trends, suggest that what is beautiful is not
necessarily in the eye of the beholder. In defining what
beautiful is, one must first differentiate between sensuality
and attractiveness. What is attractive appears to be in the
171
eye of the culture, whereas what is sensual appears to be in
the eye of the individual.
The violent effects of discrimination
The three models, proposed in Paper 3, asserted gays and
lesbians, who considered themselves as being "out", were more
likely to incur both physical and sexual discrimination by
heterosexuals. The extent of discrimination incurred by gays
and lesbians would affect the degree of violence within their
relationships. The three models examined in the study,
focused on three forms of relational violence; physical
victimization, perpetration or use of violence, and sexual
aggression. It was predicted the more violent the
relationship, the more likely the relationship would be
unstable. Finally, it was postulated the effects of
discrimination on abuse would be moderated by the self-esteem
of the gays and lesbians in the study.
The findings of the study indicated gays and lesbians,
who described themselves as being "out", were more likely to
incur discrimination. However, this finding was not supported
for gay males with high self-esteem. Discrimination was
positively related to all three forms of abuse for gay males,
regardless of self-esteem. Discrimination was significantly
and positively associated with physical victimization, the use
of violence, and sexual aggression only for lesbians with low
self-esteem. In comparing stacked structural equation models
172
between lesbians with high and low self-esteem, the data
yielded a significant change in the chi square products when
the path from discrimination to physical victimization and the
use of violence was set to be equal for the two groups. For
the model concerning physical victimization, the significance
level was less than .05; and, the significance level, for the
model concerning the use of violence, was less than .10. A
comparison between the stacked structural equation models for
lesbians with high and low self-esteem, regarding the path
from discrimination to sexual aggression, did not yield any
significant changes in the chi square product. Finally, for
both gay males and lesbians, regardless of self-esteem,
violence in the relationship made the relationship unstable.
Many of the gays and lesbians, who incurred violence in their
relationship (whether it was physical victimization,
perpetration of violence, or sexual aggression) eventually
terminated the relationship after the violent episode.
The theoretical basis for the three models in the study
was structured around a sexual script perspective. Given
cultural scenario scripts assert negative sanctions associated
with homosexual behaviors and life styles, the social
acceptance of sanctioning homosexuality is manifested and
perpetuated by cultural scenario scripts. Cultural scenario
scripts mandate homosexuality is an inappropriate sexual
behavior. Discrimination toward homosexuals appears to be the
end result of this general negativity toward homosexuality in
173
heterosexual scripts. The abuse of homosexuals has reached an
extended level in American society, based on such cultural
scenario scripts. It has become socially accepted to
physically and sexually harass gays and lesbians, based solely
on their sexual orientation. In fact, it may be asserted such
discrimination is actually encouraged by heterosexual sexual
scripts, in defining the appropriate sexual behavioral
standards for members in society. The data Indicated the more
socially "out" homosexuals perceived themselves to be, the
more discriminatory episodes they encountered by
heterosexuals.
The effect of discrimination increases the probability
gays and lesbians would incur some form of violence within
their relationships, whether physical victimization,
perpetrating violent behaviors, or being a victim of sexual
aggression. The effect of the cultural scenarios scripts,
which foster discrimination toward homosexuals, appears to
facilitate violence in gay and lesbians relationships.
Therefore, the discrimination directed at gays and lesbians
becomes filtered into the interrelational scripts among gays
and lesbians. This process of violent "filtration" among gays
and lesbians may be a result of several factors, including:
stress amplification, frustration-aggression, redefining
victimization, learned helplessness, etc.. Both stress
amplification and frustration aggression hypotheses assert
gays and lesbians who incur discrimination tend channel or
.174
displace the stress and frustration, associated with the
discriminatory acts, to the perpetration of violence in their
relationships. Or, the stress and frustration, associated
with the discrimination, fosters alienation and helplessness
among gay and lesbian victims. The stress of discrimination,
compiled with the lack of supportive resources designed for
gays and lesbians, could become manifested in further forms of
aggression under the context of relational violence. Cultural
scenario scripts, which facilitate discrimination toward
homosexuals, simultaneously defines gays and lesbians as
legitimate victims of cultural abuse. The effect of such
cultural scenario scripts ultimately impacts on the
interrelational scripts of gays and lesbians in defining
themselves as legitimate victims. Gays and lesbians who
utilize violence within their relationships, definitively
support violence toward gays and lesbians, and become
legitimated by the broader sexual scripts which foster such
violence based on sexual orientation. In turn, those gays and
lesbians who are victims of physical or sexual abuse, define
themselves as deserving victims if they have incorporated the
values embedded in cultural scenario scripts into their
interrelational scripts. Further, gays and lesbians who are
abused, may have learned they are helpless victims under the
broader sexual schema as appropriated by cultural scenario
scripts.
175
It was, perhaps, encouraging to note the data indicated
that many of the abusive relationships were unstable
relationships. The frequency results suggested many of the
participants in abusive relationships terminated the
relationship after the violent incident. The cultural
scenario scripts, which define abused gays and lesbians as
legitimate victims, is a transient aspect among homosexual
interrelational scripts. Despite the lack of social services,
providing assistance to abused gays and lesbians, victimized
gays and lesbians still maintained enough social resources to
abandon abusive relationships. It is posited the
interrelational scripts among gays and lesbians, in general
cases, does not condone long-term, abusive relationships. It
could also be the same cultural scenario scripts which remit
discrimination toward gays and lesbians, also perceive gay and
lesbian relationships as illegitimate associations. And such
illegitimacy, attached to gay and lesbian relationships, may
predispose gay and lesbians to maintaining relatively unstable
relationships. Therefore, leaving an abusive gay and lesbian
relationship may not pose as many social sanctions, when
compared to abusive heterosexual relationships. The "closed"
nature attached to homosexual relationships may actually
reduce the social disapproval mechanisms, which are generally
associated with divorce and separation incurred in
heterosexual relationships.
176
The only gender differences reported in this study tended
to be correlated with self-esteem. It was found among gay
males with high self-esteem, that being "out" was not
significantly related to incurring physical and sexual
discrimination. However, this finding was significant for
lesbians, regardless of self-esteem, and among gay males with
low self-esteem. The intrapsychic scripts, guiding both
lesbians and gay males, reflects the internal mechanisms
linked to a homosexual identity. Such internal mechanisms,
underlying intrapsychic scripts, includes concepts like self-
esteem. It is posited that gay males with low self-esteem do
not possess the identity salience to sufficiently cope with
being "out" and the occurrence of discrimination. Perhaps gay
males with low self-esteem are more likely to attribute
discrimination to the extent they flaunt or disclose their
homosexual identity. Whereas, gay males with high self-esteem
maintain confidence about their identity and intrapsychic
scripts. Therefore, gay males with high self-esteem do not
readily associate any discriminatory acts they may incur with
their homosexual life style.
The second gender difference found in this study focused
on the impact of discrimination on sexual aggression, physical
victimization, and the use of violence. Among gay males, and
lesbians with low self-esteem, the effect of discrimination on
relational violence was significant. However, among lesbians
with high self-esteem, the relationships between the three
177
forms of violence and discrimination were found to be
nonsignificant. Lesbians with high self-esteem appear to be
able to diffuse the negative effects of discrimination on
relational violence. Given lesbians have interrelational
scripts which tend to be built on friendship networks and
intimacy, this may account for the apparent gender difference
concluded in this study. If lesbians with high self-esteem
built their relationships on the basis of intimacy and
friendship, perhaps their interrelational scripts are more
forgiving of discriminatory violence, and capable of
nonviolently coping with discrimination.
Integrating a sexual script perspective
The three studies presented in this dissertation have
investigated the effect of cultural and structural
arrangements as they have affected the perspective of
homosexual sexual scripts. The general attempt by each of the
projects has been to analyze the various means by which
structural or cultural arrangements have influenced the
development of homosexual sexual scripts. The general
postulate governed by the homosexual sexual script perspective
asserts homosexual relationships are sanctioned by the broader
cultural values and norms.
The sexual script perspective defines homosexuality in
terms of public labeling and the attendant roles and scripts
associated with this label. The criticism attached to the
178
sexual script perspective contends the approach does not allow
for the development of sexual identities or the influence of
physiological conditions. Where the sexual script perspective
views homosexuality as loosely defined behavior patterns based
on interactions with others, it tends to neglect the power of
cultural arrangements which influence identity construction
and development. The first article in this dissertation
attempted to merge the two approaches in the homosexuality
literature: identity development and sexual scripts. The
model proposed in the first article asserted homosexual
identities are facilitated or detracted from developing into
an integrated identity (disclosing the homosexual identity)
based on the stigma associated with the label of homosexuality
(via family identification). The staunch theoretical position
asserted by Sagarin (1975), contended homosexuality is simply
comprised of a set of roles pertaining to a series of scripted
situational contexts, was not necessarily supported by the
article. Rather, the identity/script perspective, advocated
by Troiden (1979, 1984/1985), was sustained by the findings in
the study. In general, the results of the project indicated
structural forces, such as the family, affect the expression
and disclosure of homosexual identities among adolescents.
The heterosexual scripts, promoted by the family, detracted
from the salience of a homosexual identity.
The second criticism attached to the sexual script
perspective is directed at the ambiguity of scripted
179
behaviors. Sexual scripts represent general guidelines,
interactions, values, and norms; however, the detail of the
scripting process tends to be quite innocuous. The aim of the
second article was to detail the characteristics of one
scripted dimension, attractiveness. Analyzing the open-ended
responses of the participants, revealed a normative script
which denoted general characteristics composing the definition
of attractiveness. Specific personality, facial, and bodily
characteristics were emphasized by the subjects. These
representations comprised a mosaic of cultural expressions
which delineate the cultural dimensions and norms defining
attractiveness.
The separation of attractiveness and sensuality, as two
distinct definitive constructs, further detailed the
specificity associated with sexual scripts. The findings in
the study indicated the composite qualities associated with
attractiveness were fairly similar among homosexual males and
heterosexual females. However, the two groups significantly
differed in the characteristics which define sensuality among
men. The definition of sensuality is perhaps centered around
intrapsychic scripts, suggesting what is sexy is guided by
individual fantasies and desires. Among both sample groups,
homosexuals and heterosexuals, responses varied significantly
between compatible groups. Homosexual males and heterosexual
females differed significantly in their responses with regard
to defining male sensuality. And heterosexual males and
180
lesbian differed significantly when the characteristics of
body, face and personality were contrasted in the definition
of female sensuality. Therefore, the sexual scripts guiding
the definition of sensuality is either subculturally specific,
or individual-oriented.
It is the viewpoint of this dissertation that the sexual
scripts, defining sensuality are individual-oriented, rather
than subcultural specific. After reviewing the qualitative
responses of each sample group, it appeared individuals
maintained unique expressions of sensuality, regardless of
subcultural affiliation. For example, some homosexual males
asserted bald men were sexy, and some heterosexual males
stated women with small breasts were sensual. Although such
responses were not normative, they represented a faction of
responses which were unique. It is posited individuals
construct intrapsychic scripts which define sensuality on an
individual level. This construction could be due to
socialization patterns or symbolic attachment to various media
representations. Whatever the origin of the intrapsychic
script, the subjects' responses reflected individual qualities
which delineated certain responses from a "normative" picture
of attractiveness. In short, by examining the individual
responses to the open-ended questions, it appeared the
definition of attractiveness bared striking similarities
across responses; consequently, an inspection of responses to
sensuality yielded dissimilar characteristics.
181
A third criticism of the sexual script perspective is the
narrow field in which the perspective is utilized. The
perspective tends to be offered to explain patterns in sexual
interactions, but is neglected when offering commentary about
other relational aspects over and above sexuality. The third
paper in this dissertation attempted to generalize the sexual
script perspective to other relational dynamics other than
sexuality. The third article focused on the effects of
discrimination on relational violence. Physical
discrimination, based on sexual orientation, was found to
increase the probability of being a victim of violence in gay
and lesbian relationships, and increase the likelihood of
abusing. Cultural scenario scripts define homosexuality as a
deviant status in the United States, which subjugates
homosexuals to discrimination. The stress associated with
physical harassment eventually becomes a part of the dynamics
of gay and lesbian relationships, and can be correlated with
being a victim or using violence. Further, those who
maintained some aspect of physical violence in their
relationships were more likely to have unstable relationships.
The sexual script perspective can assist other theories
in explaining not only sensuality, but interrelational
dynamics and exchanges. The sexual script perspective can
offer a certain understanding about the way culture, social
structure, and individuals interact with one another.
Scripts, as a theory can be used to explain the general
182
guidelines and norms encompassing relational interactions, or
by extrapolating the perspective to other relational
phenomenon, above sexuality. Further, sexual scripts can be
utilized to approach specific issues in intimate
relationships, for example attractiveness. By understanding
the interdynamic levels of sexual scripting, the various
levels of analysis can be centered to explain the relationship
between the individual and culture, or social structure.
Limitations and Future Research
It is hoped each of the three studies have contributed to
the understanding of homosexual sexual scripts. By noting how
sexual scripts affect the development of homosexual
identities, norms, and relationships, each article has
furthered the research knowledge on homosexuality. However,
much remains to be investigated, especially concerning
homosexual relationships. Prior to discussing potential
avenues for future research, it is necessary to address the
criticisms and limitations of each of the three studies in
this dissertation.
Limitations
Family disclosure. In general, there are three major
limitations associated with the article on the disclosure of
identities to the family among gay and lesbian adolescents.
First, the representativeness of the sample should be
183
evaluated. Given the data for the study emerged from a
snowball sampling technique, the sample does not necessarily
convey the extent of representativeness as associated with a
random sample. However, the limitations posed by the snowball
sample must be realistically addressed. Gay and lesbian
adolescents comprise a unique and highly stigmatized
population. To obtain a random sample of gay and lesbian
adolescents verges on the improbable.
The second limitation with the article stemmed from the
measurement of several key concepts. Single indicators were
used to measure the concepts of family identification, the
extent of perceived homosexual-supportive resources, and
disclosure of their identities to their family. The use of
single indicators posed a threat to both the reliability and
validity of the tested model. The utilization of a single
indicator for family identification may have "tapped" into
variant dimensions of the general construct, such as
satisfaction with family members, and family value
association. Further, the use of a single indicator for the
variable of family identification may be confounded with
potentially spurious or intervening variables, such as
parenting style, religious values, peer associations, and
sexual values. The same problem exists with the extent of
perceived resources measure. Again, the construct may be
drawing upon the dimensions or spurious correlates of
community size, the actual versus perceived resources, and the
184
utilization of such supportive résources. The disclosure
variables may have targeted single dimensions of disclosure,
or the intervening correlates of disclosure. For example, the
disclosure variable is not directly ascertaining whether or
not the adolescents disclosed their sexual identity to their
parents, rather the variable attempted to estimate whether or
not the parents knew of the adolescent's sexual identity.
The third limitation of the disclosure study focuses on
the causal ordering assumed by the hypothesized model. The
model asserted family identification detracted from the extent
of perceived resources, and the expression of the adolescents'
homosexual identity. It was also posited the extent of
perceived resources and the expression of a homosexual
identity would facilitate the disclosure process among gay and
lesbian adolescents. Given the study was conducted with
cross-sectional data, there is no way to validate the extent
identification with the family was a product of the disclosure
process or directed the disclosure process. Although the
literature does advocate that parental attitudes and values
precipitate the acquisition of homosexual identities.
Definition of attractiveness. The major limitation
posed by the attractiveness study consists of comparable
samples. In short, the two major samples of heterosexuals and
homosexuals were not directly analogous. The homosexual
sample was older, maintained a higher social economic status,
and were more diverse in regional composition. This
185
limitation may cause problems in asserting that unique sexual
scripts exist for homosexual and heterosexual cultures, in
particular for scripts dealing with sensuality. For example,
the difference between lesbians and heterosexual males in the
definition of attractiveness could be the result of older
lesbians.
A second limitation posed by the project was in the
assessment of attractiveness and sensuality. General
characteristics were utilized, versus specific qualities.
Although respondents indicated they found, for example,
muscularity was attractive, the respondents did not indicate
how important muscularity was when compared to other
characteristics. The elusive nature of the measures did not
ascertain the extent to which respondents favored various
characteristics over others.
The third limitation of the study is centered on the
interpretation of the results. The tests of significance in
the study should be interpreted very carefully. In order to
conduct the test of significance the categories were collapsed
into three major sections: face, body, and personality. The
tests offer limited information regarding the general priority
of general attributes over others, but failed to provide
specific information regarding particular characteristics.
The violent effects of discrimination. Although one of
the first studies to quantitatively examine gay and lesbian
violence, the study suffers from several limitations. First,
186
the measures in the study could be improved upon. The
discrimination variable was measured by using two dichotomous
items, with no indication of time or place of occurrence. The
study cannot delineate the causal influence of discrimination
on violence due to the cross-sectional data. For example, it
could be possible those who used violence in their
relationships, or were victims of violence (physical or
sexual) established themselves as potential targets of
discrimination after the violent episode.
Further, homosexual identity was assessed by the degree
the subjects were "out" regarding their life style. The
measure did not address the extent the respondents were
satisfied or happy with their sexual identity. Although the
association between being committed to a homosexual identity
and discrimination may appear to be trivial. The relationship
assumes those who were committed to a homosexual identity were
more likely to be discriminated against, and thus incur some
form of violence in their relationships. It may be more
appropriate to assess not just the extent the respondents were
"out of the closet", but also the degree they were comfortable
with their sexual identity.
The second criticism of the .study is with the self-esteem
variable. By dichotomizing the variable at the median reduced
the variance associated with self-esteem. In other words,
those with high self-esteem and moderately high self-esteem
were grouped together. Using self-esteem as a dichotomous
187
grouping was selected for the ease of modeling the mediating
effects of the variable. It is possible to expand on the
results of the study by treating self-esteem as a continuous
interaction variable, and utilizing the entire variance
associated with self-esteem.
Future research
Future research in the area of homosexual sexual scripts
is encouraged. Little is empirically known about homosexual
life styles and relationships. Any further attempt to clarify
the scripts in homosexual relationships would contribute to
the sociological literature on both sexual scripts and
homosexuality. Not only is general research encouraged on
homosexual sexual scripts, but each article has particular
areas of concern which should be addressed by further
investigations.
Family disclosure. Much research on homosexual
identity and disclosure have been conducted, yet little of the
research has been done on adolescents. It is hoped more
research on homosexual identity acquisition will be devoted
toward adolescent samples, instead of adult retrospective
studies. Future studies, utilizing gay and lesbian
adolescents, should focus on the processes and resources used
in the development of a homosexual identity. As the study
demonstrated, perceived resources and support networks
promoted a sense of security toward the disclosure process.
188
Further, future research should incorporate stronger more
diverse measures of support and resource utilization to build
a stronger and more reliable index of social support. In
addition, it is hoped researchers will elaborate on the family
relationship/identification variable. Homosexuality
investigators should consider such variables as parent/child
communication patterns, and supportiveness.
Ideally, a longitudinal or panel design would be the most
extensive means to document the actual processes adolescents
go through in the development and disclosure of a homosexual
identity. A qualitative study could facilitate further
understanding of homosexual identity formation. In addition
to identifying the various resources used in the development
process, a qualitative study could address the particular
concerns and identity struggles among gay and lesbian
adolescents.
Second, it is hoped future research will focus on
contextual variables which would moderate the proposed
relationships in the model. Some variables which may be
considered would include: socio-economic status, peer group
affiliation, gender identity, the role of siblings, etc. The
model would also benefit by contrasting urban from rural
adolescents. Third, it is hoped the model could be
generalized from adolescents to adults who pursue a homosexual
identity or disclosure in adulthood. By targeting other
189
factors which could affect the disclosure process, should shed
insight on the subject of homosexual identity formation.
Definition of attractiveness. Although quite a bit of
research has been conducted on physical attractiveness, it is
hoped future research will investigate the dimensions of
attractiveness beyond physical characteristics. One of the
contributions of the study, to the conceptual definition of
attractiveness, was the focus on personality features. Future
research on attractiveness should further explore the
personality dimension in relation to the physical dimension.
It is also hoped attractiveness researchers could
differentiate between concepts like sensuality and
attractiveness. The study found significant differences in
the definitions of attractiveness and sensuality.
Second, it is hoped research on the differences between
homosexuals and heterosexuals will be further addressed. For
example, it would be interesting to decompose various
subcultural influences in developing respective definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality. Or to test the interaction
effect of gender identity in delineating the definitions of
attractiveness and sensuality. Finally, subcultural
influences, such socio-economic status or friendship
affiliation, would be an intriguing correlate to definitions
of attractiveness or sensuality.
The violent effects of discrimination. As one of the
first articles to quantify physical and sexual violence in gay
190
and lesbian relationships, it is hoped much research will stem
from this article. First, a qualitative analysis is
recommended on the subject. It is important to learn why and
how harassment leads to using violence in gay and lesbian
relationships, and being a victim of physical and sexual
aggression. A qualitative analysis may improve on the
theoretical development of the article in providing a
contextual framework in which the association between
discrimination and violence is exacerbated.
Second, another study is recommended to validate the
findings of this study. It is advisable that a follow-up
study consist of a longitudinal or panel design in which the
causal relationships between the various forms of violence and
discrimination could be further documented. Further, better
measures of the critical variables in the study should be
considered. For example, a follow-up study should expand on
the discrimination measure by distinguishing when and how the
act of harassment had occurred. It is also recommended other
forms of discrimination should be investigated, such as
employment and housing crises. Further research on the
effects of discrimination is encouraged. It would be
interesting to note the effects of discrimination on health or
relationship quality, for example.
Homosexuality researchers need to broaden their general
concept of homosexual identity. In the past, many researchers
have primarily focused on the psychological aspects of a
191
homosexual identity. In general, it is hoped research trends
will move toward a structural perspective which can
incorporate structural influences with psychological factors.
It is also hoped that homosexual identity researchers will
build on the presented studies by elaborating on the
conceptual ideas and models.
Conclusion
The purpose of this dissertation was to apply the sexual
script perspective to various dimensions of homosexuality.
The second objective of the dissertation was to show the
conflict between heterosexual and homosexual sexual scripts.
In doing so, the dissertation attempted to map homosexual
sexual scripts, and heterosexual scripts dealing with
homosexual issues. Three studies were conducted to examine
various aspects of homosexual sexual scripts and the influence
of heterosexual scripts on homosexual themes. The first paper
addressed homosexual identities, and the problems faced by
adolescents in disclosing their identities to their parents.
The project found adolescents were caught in an exchange
struggle between the "pushes and pulls" of cultural and
subcultural influences. In other words, adolescents must
weigh the costs and benefits of forfeiting heterosexual sexual
scripts for the possible benefits of acquiring homosexual
sexual scripts.
192
The second paper targeted the dimensions of interpersonal
dynamics among heterosexual and homosexuals. The definition
of attractiveness and sensuality were contrasted among
heterosexuals and homosexuals. The study concluded
homosexuals and heterosexuals overlap in their definition of
attractiveness, with the exception of the lesbian sample;
however, homosexuals and heterosexuals were dissimilar in
their definition of sensuality. Once engaging a homosexual
script, gays and lesbians (in particular) were more likely to
view definitions of sensuality apart from heterosexual
influences.
The third study focused on violence in gay and lesbian
relationships. The more gays and lesbians were committed to a
homosexual script (being "out", so to speak), the more likely
gays and lesbians were physically harassed and discriminated
against. Further, the more physical discrimination gays and
lesbians incurred, the more likely they were to be victims of
sexual aggression or violence in their relationships. And
discriminated gays and lesbians were more likely to use
violence within their relationships. The results in the study
suggested gays and lesbians, though articulating a unique
sexual script, were not beyond the sanctions and influences of
heterosexual scripts. In this case, the influence of
heterosexual scripts (via discrimination) appears to have
decisively negative consequences for gay and lesbian
relationships.
193
In conclusion, homosexual sexual scripts cannot be
examined as an entity by themselves. Homosexual sexual
scripts operate under the influence of the dominant
heterosexual culture, and therefore, are subjugated to
heterosexual influences. In the first and third article, both
studies illustrated the influence heterosexual scripts have on
the development of homosexual identities, and the continuance
of homosexual relational scripts. In short, it appears
heterosexual scripts can detract from the development of
homosexual scripts and identities, and serve as potential
barriers or obstacles after an individual embraces a
homosexual identity and the scripts which accommodate such an
identity.
Homosexual sexual scripts, in some ways, are similar to
heterosexual scripts. The second article demonstrated
similarities in the definition of male attractiveness among
heterosexual females and homosexual males. However, in other
ways, homosexual sexual scripts are different from
heterosexual sexual scripts. For example, in the same
article, heterosexual males and lesbian possessed different
attitudes about what characteristics constitute female
attractiveness.
The sexual script perspective tends to be viewed as an
incomplete theory. The three studies in this dissertation
have attempted to fill in some of the gaps in the theory, to
present a broader application of the perspective. By merging
194
the sexual script perspective with other existing theories and
hypotheses, facilitated a broader and perhaps more complete
understanding of intimate relationships.
195
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Adam, Barry D.
1978. The Survival of Domination. New York: Elsevier.
1985. "Age, structure, and sexuality: reflections on
the anthropological evidence on homosexual
relations." Journal of Homosexuality^ 11(3/4); 19-
33.
Anderson, R. C. and J. Pichert
1978. "Recall of previously unrecallable information
following a shift in perspective." Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17: 1-12.
Athanasiou, R., P. Shaver, and C. Travis
1970. "Sex." Psychology Today, July; 50.
Bartlett, F. C.
1932. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social
Psychology. Cambridge; Cambridge University Press.
Blackwood, Evelyn
1985. "Breaking the mirror; the construction of
lesbianism and the anthropological discourse on
homosexuality." Journal of Homosexuality, 11(3/4):
1-17.
Burgess, E.W. and P. Wallin
1953. Engagement and Marriage. Chicago; Lippincott.
Cass, Viviene c.
1979. "Homosexual identity formation: a theoretical
model." Journal of Homosexuality, 4(3); 219-35.
1983. "Homosexual identity: a concept in need of
definition." Journal of Homosexuality, 9(2/3): 105-
26.
Coleman, Eli
1982. "Developmental stages of the coming-out process:"
In Homosexuality: Social, Psychological, and
Biological Issues, edited by William Paul, James D.
Weinrich, John C. Gonsiorek, and-Mary E. Hotvedt,
149-58. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Corbett, Sherry, Richard Troiden, and Richard Dodder
1974. "Tolerance as a correlate of experience with
stigma: the case of the homosexual." Journal of
Homosexuality, 3(1): 3-13.
196
Cuber, J.F. and P.B. Harroff
1965. The Significant Americans. New York: Appleton-
Century.
Davenport, W.
1965. "Sexual patterns and their regulation in a society
of the Southwest Pacific." In Sex and Behavior.
New York: Wiley.
Dew, Mary A.
1985. "The effects of attitudes on inferences of
homosexuality and perceived physical attractiveness
in women." Sex Roles, 12(1/2); 143-155.
Gagnon, John
1977. Human Sexualities. Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman.
Gagnon, John H. and William Simon
1973. Sexual Conduct: The Social Forces of Human
Sexuality. Chicago; Aldine.
Goode, Erich
1973. The Drug Phenomenon: Social Aspects of Drug
Taking. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
Hayes, T. and F. Oziel
1976. "Capturing the invisible population." Journal of
Homosexuality, 10(1): 125-37.
Hunt, Morton
1974. Sexual Behavior in the 1970s. Chicago; Playboy
Press.
Kinsey, Alfred, Wardell Pomeroy, and Clyde Martin'
1948. Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. Philadelphia:
Saunders.
Laws, Judith L. and Pepper Schwartz
1977. Sexual Scripts: The Social Construction of Female
Sexuality. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden Press.
Lee, John
1977. "Going public: a study in the sociology of
homosexual liberation." Journal of Homosexuality,
3(1): 49-78.
Levine, Martin P.
1987. "Gay Macho: Ethnography of the Homosexual Clone."
Doctoral dissertation. New York University.
197
Masters, W. and V. Johnson
1979. Homosexuality in Perspective. Boston; Little
Brown.
Money, J.
1980. Love and Love Sickness: The Science of Sex,
Gender Difference, and Pair-Bonding. Baltimore:
John Hopkins University Press.
Plummer, Kenneth
1975. Sexual Stigma: An Interactionist Account.
London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Ponse, Barbara
1978. Identities in the Lesbian World: The Social
Construction of Self. Westport, CT; Greenwood
Press.
Rainwater, Lee
1970. Behind Ghetto Walls: Black Families in a Federal
Slum. Chicago: Aldine.
Sagarin, Edward
1973. "The good guys, the bad guys, and the gay guys."
Contemporary Sociology, 2(1): 3-13.
1975. "The tyranny of 'isness." In Deviants and
Deviance: An Introduction to the Study of Disvalued
People and Behavior, 144-54. New York: Praeger-
Holt.
1976. "Thieves, homosexuals, and other deviants: the
high personal cost of wearing a label." Psychology
Today, 9(10): 25-31.
1979. "Deviance without deviants: the temporal quality
of patterned behavior." Deviant Behavior, 1(1): 1-
13.
Schank, R. C.
1982. Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and
Learning in Computers and People. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Schwartz, Barry and Daniel Reisberg
1991. Learning and Memory. New York: W. W. Norton and
Company.
Shafer, Siegrid
1976. "Sexual and social problems among lesbians."
Journal of Sex Research, 12(1): 50-69.
198
Shutz, Alfred
1962. Collected Papers. Vol. I: The Problem of Social
Reality. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
1967. The Phenomenology of the Social World. Evanston,
IL: Northwestern University Press.
Simon, William , and Gagnon, John H.
1984. "Sexual scripts." Society, 22(1): 53-60.
Thio, Alex
1983. Deviant Behavior. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Troiden, Richard
1977. "Becoming homosexual: research on acquiring a gay
identity." Journal of Homosexuality, 1(1): 31-46.
1979. "Becoming homosexual: a model of gay identity
acquisition." Psychiatry, 42(4): 362-73.
1984/1985. "Self, self-concept, identity, and homosexual
identity: constructs in need of definition and
differentiation." Journal of Homosexuality,
10(3/4): 97-109.
Turner, Jonathan
1991. The Structure of Sociological Theory. Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth, Inc.
Weinberg, Thomas
1977. "Becoming homosexual: self-disclosure, self-
identity, and self-maintenance." Doctoral
dissertation. University of Connecticut.
1978. "On 'doing' and 'being' gay; sexual behavior and
homosexual male self-identity." Journal of
Homosexuality, 4(2): 143-56.
199
APPENDIX
200
Table 1. Factor analysis results for the expression of
homosexual identity scale for gay males and lesbians
as reported in Paper 1
Variables in the Scale Males Females
Frequency of attendance .740 .83 3
in gay organizations
Importance of gay .801 .817
organizations
Frequency of gay .678 .662
sexual contact
Importance of gay .742 .771
sexual contact
Importance of gay .756 .443
friendships
Standardized alpha .753 .798
Cronbach's alpha .745 .794
Table 2. Factor analysis results for the physical
victimization scale as reported in Paper 3
Variables in the Scale Males Females
Threatened .839 .649
Pushed .837 .795
Slapped .779 .681
Punched .826 .781
Struck with an Object .646 .616
Use of a Weapon .485 .436
Standardized alpha .833 .745
Cronbach's alpha .838 .742
201
Table 3. Factor analysis results for the use of violence
scale as reported in Paper 3
Variables in the Scale Males Females
Threatened .780 .708
Pushed .843 .780
Slapped .778 .717
Punched .836 .751
Struck with an Object .594 .587
Use of a Weapon X X
Note: "X" indicates zero cases occurred for the particular
variables.
Standardized alpha .825 .753
Cronbach's alpha .825 .745
202
Table 4. Factor analysis results for the sexual aggression
scale as reported in Paper 3
Variables in the Scale Males Females
Intoxication .512 .446
Threat to Terminate .412 .665
Relationship
Threatened to Disclose .570 1 Case
Negative Information
Guilt .660 .694
Physical Detainment .552 .191
Partner Tried to Turn On .607 .702
Use of Lies .626 .764
Use of False Promises .431 .779
Physically Held Down .572 .709
Use of a Weapon X X
Threatened to Use .647 1 Case
Physical Force
Use of Physical Force .457 .655
Note: "X" indicates zero cases occurred for the particular
variables.
Standardized alpha .763 .747
Cronbach's alpha .746 .762
203
Table 5. Factor analysis results for the self-esteem scale as
reported in Paper 3
Variables in the Scale Males Females
On the whole I am .694 .811
satisfied with myself.
At times I think I am .710 .749
no good at all.
I certainly feel .617 .690
useless at times.
I feel that I am a person .793 .823
of worth, at least on an
equal plane with others.
I take a positive .800 .871
attitude towards myself.
Standardized alpha .773 .849
Cronbach's alpha .746 .836