Enhanced Data Narratives
Enhanced Data Narratives
To cite this article: Judd D. Bradbury & Rosanna E. Guadagno (2021) Enhanced data narratives,
Journal of Management Analytics, 8:2, 171-194, DOI: 10.1080/23270012.2021.1886883
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/23270012.2021.1886883
Introduction
Data narrative is a category of communication methods that utilize an underlying
quantitative data set as content for a knowledge transfer experience using narrative
structure. As technological advances increasingly facilitate access to “big data”
(Manyika et al., 2011; McAfee, Brynjolfsson, Davenport, Patil, & Barton, 2010),
research teams are developing new methods for the construction of narrative com-
munication using quantitative data sets. Researchers in the fields of data visualization
and artificial intelligence are working to enhance historic traditions of human com-
munication. The need for enhanced communication techniques has evolved in paral-
lel to a rapidly expanding scope of data. Modern analytical methods have the capacity
to process data sets with thousands of variables and millions of records (Elmqvist,
Stasko, & Tsigas, 2008; Eriksson, Byrne, Johansson, Trygg, & Vikström, 2013;
Keim, 2002; Shneiderman, 2008). How do you properly construct communication
about four thousand different facets of a phenomenon that is one million records
deep?
mentally construct a world where information content lives, connecting that world to
personal experience. In many ways, storytelling is the human operating system for
exchanging complex information within a context. This begs the question: which of
the extant enhanced approaches is the most effective technique for creating data nar-
ratives that produce a high degree of knowledge transfer? Below, we review the
benefits of each data-driven approach.
Visual Data Narratives benefit from a significant ability to summarize and com-
putationally encode complex data sets (Larkin & Simon, 1987). Quantitative data sets
with a large scope can be engaged visually with navigation, layering, so-called small
multiples, and multi-dimensional representation (Card, 1999; Munzner, 2009; Shnei-
derman, 1996; Yi, Kang, & Stasko, 2007). The challenge for Visual Data Narratives is
in the scope of audience familiar with frame based presentation of data visualization.
Frame based communication techniques have been used previously in comic books,
storyboards, slide show presentations, new journalistic approaches, and business
intelligence dashboards. While there is a significant amount of experience with
frame based presentation of information, the experience of a general audience with
data visualization presented in frame-based presentations with stepper navigation is
significantly less than the experience of the same audience with communication in
text narrative.
Documentary Data Narratives benefit from a large cross section of people experi-
enced in watching films and from engaging two channels of information processing
versus one (Mayer & Anderson, 1991; Mayer & Moreno, 1998; Mayer & Sims,
1994; Tversky, Morrison, & Betrancourt, 2002). Similar to Visual Data Narratives,
Documentary Data Narratives are constructed from data visualization where audi-
ences have significantly less experience interpreting the information. However,
general audiences do have a great deal of experience watching documentary videos.
When verbal narration (i.e. the “voice-of-god”) is engaged, Documentary Data Nar-
ratives benefit even further from the long history of verbal storytelling containing a
more universal reach of literacy.
Computer Generated Text Data Narratives benefit from looking and feeling
similar to traditional Human Generated Text Data Narratives. Without clearly indi-
cating the source of computer generated data narratives, research subjects may not
be able to determine the difference between a data narrative generated by a human
author versus one generated by a computer. Using the common communication tech-
nique of the written word, Computer Generated Text Data Narratives and Human
Generated Text Data Narratives share a familiar knowledge transfer experience.
Over a number of repetitive reading experiences, if the same construction template
is used, audience members might develop commentary about boredom with the con-
sistent pattern of the stories. This commentary is balanced by the positive feature of
consistency of information communication in a series of stories. Based on literacy in
general, Computer Generated Text Data Narratives have the ability to reach a very
wide audience. Computer Generated Text Data Narratives have challenges when
encoding a large scope of information in a concise way without the use of visual
imagery. Text describes data well, but its nature may be more limited in its ability
to summarize or computationally encode a larger scope of information.
Empirical research regarding the knowledge transfer performance of these
methods for enhancing data narratives has yet to address which of these methods
is superior. The purpose of this paper is to evaluate the knowledge transfer capability
174 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
for each of these emerging types of enhanced data narratives. Specifically, we examine
the knowledge transfer for each of these data narrative methods, alongside the classic
example of text stories written by humans. We assess the recall performance for par-
ticipants who viewed one of the four data narrative methods, communicating infor-
mation about the common data set.
Specifically, we explore three primary research questions related to a general audi-
ence in this paper:
2015; Howson, 2014; SAP, 2015) Each frame of data visualization takes the shape of an
event in a traditional story, linking cause and effect within time and space (Bordwell,
Thompson, & Smith, 1997). Like narrative storytelling, Visual Data Narratives con-
struct a series of data visualizations positioned as events, linked by cause and effect
when appropriate, sequenced in time and located in space or geography.
One of the simplest methods for constructing a series of visualization events is
defined in the Aristotelian tradition of a three-act play defined as a beginning,
middle, and an ending (Aristotle, 1984). The beginning view creates a frame of refer-
ence for the audience. From the standpoint of spatial and temporal location, this first
frame should passively help the audience locate their proximity to the phenomena
represented in the Visual Data Narrative. The beginning might locate the audience
within an industry, company, geography, virtual concept, and/or point in time. Rheto-
ric, for example using the Aristotelian modes of ethics, logic, or emotion can be
engaged as a theme, bringing human meaning to the presentation. The introduction
of rhetoric helps the audience identify with the narrative. How does this information
relate to me? The middle view then uses a visualization view to highlight an issue or
insight with a significant amount of tension. The more tension created by this middle
view, the more effective the Visual Data Narrative. The ending presents a view of data
that reconciles or summarizes the tension created during the middle view. Risk miti-
gation, alternate courses of action, and new opportunities are fertile approaches for
reconciling the tension created in the middle view. Titles, headings, and annotations in
each of the views help to provide narration of the insights defined in the data
visualizations.
In cases where the content requires a more elaborate treatment of events, a struc-
ture similar to the classic Freytag Pyramid of Setup, Rising Action, Climax, Falling
Action, and Resolution may provide a more interesting composition (Freytag,
1896). The rising action, climax, and falling action provide more intricate treatments
of the presentation of tension in comparison to the simpler middle view of the Aris-
totelian model.
A more recent approach created by Neil Cohn is called Visual Narrative Structure,
emulating the presentation approach in comic books (Cohn, 2013; McCloud, 1993).
Visual narrative structure can be utilized to organize and sequence a series of cause
and effect-linked data visualizations designed around unique styles of visual views.
The visual narrative structure model sequences five styles of visual frames known
as establishers, initials, prolongations, peaks, and releases (Cohn, 2013). The Estab-
lisher frame sets up the action in a scene. Characters are often introduced in the estab-
lisher frame. The Initial frame begins tension as a path of narrative arc. The Initial
frame begins anticipation of the goal/peak of a path. The Prolongation view marks
the middle of the narrative arc and is designed to expand anticipation in the story.
This frame may be a somewhat passive or cyclical engagement of an on-going
process. The Peak frame marks the high point of narrative tension as an event. The
Peak often represents a critical event, or the goal of a path being achieved. The
Release frame then relieves the tension created in the overall narrative arc.
For the present study, we constructed a Visual Data Narrative in the form of an
interactive slide show with multiple frames of data visualization. The interactive slides
are presented in a predefined sequence commonly referred to as a stepper in Tableau
Storypoints. The categorization is derived from a navigation feature representing an
arrow of time that allows the user to advance through data visualization frames, one
176 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
voice-of-god commentary, and narrative storytelling. Our indexical evidence was rep-
resented using the identical six visualization frames utilized in our construction of
Visual Data Narrative with stepper navigation. The navigation of the visualization
frames was set to motion using video instead of user guided stepping. The video
was enhanced with voice-of-god narration engaging the text based insight annota-
tions at the top of each visualization frame. Rhetorical enhancements were intention-
ally minimized to avoid injecting bias into our measurements.
Statements using the themes of ethics and emotion were avoided altogether.
Careful attention was paid to include logical statements about insights that were con-
sistent with the constructions of Visual Data Narrative and Computer Generated
Text Data Narrative. See Figure 7 for the example of Documentary Data Narrative
used in our study.
in English or other natural languages from some underlying and non-linguistic rep-
resentation of information (Reiter & Dale, 1997). The development of this type of
automated construction of news stories is being pioneered by organizations like Nar-
rative Science, Automated Insights, Yseop, and the Los Angeles Times (Automated
Insights, 2015; Narrative Science, 2015; Slate, 2015; Yseop, 2015). As a leader in
this sub-field, Narrative Science began with a business strategy of constructing jour-
nalistic stories for audiences of one that were cost prohibitive for human authors to
write (Borgo et al., 2013). The Gamechanger website application constructs a sports
news story for a little league team using an uploaded box score from the game (Allen,
Templon, McNally, Birnbaum, & Hammond, 2010; Business Insider, 2014). This
novel approach allows feature length stories to be constructed about little league
baseball games without incurring the high labor cost of a professional journalist.
In its second major initiative, Narrative Science created a partnership with Forbes
magazine where the Narrative Science algorithm creates stories about earnings pro-
jections for publicly traded corporations that are published online a few days
before the formal earnings announcements (The Atlantic, 2015). The earnings projec-
tions use a quantitative data set provided by Zacks Investment Research that is
applied to a narrative template of financial sentence phrases using conditional if/
then logical rules to construct a narrative text story about earnings that is then pub-
lished in an online column in Forbes Magazine (Forbes, 2015). The algorithm driven
investing column provides a low cost posting of a news story without the lower return
on investment and tedious effort of a human author.
180 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
In our study, we utilized the Computer Generated Text Data Narrative composed
by the Narrative Science algorithm that ran on July 13, 2015 in the electronic version
of Forbes Magazine (Forbes, 2015). The text of the narrative was provided to our
research participants in a single view as illustrated in Figure 8.
factual content and statements were the same in each of the data narrative construc-
tions, providing focus on the construction techniques as dependent variable con-
ditions for measurement.
In the first version, an example of Visual Data Narrative using Storypoints func-
tionality in Tableau was created. The story building process constructs narratives
using a stepper type function to advance from one data visualization frame to the
next visualization in a sequence. The Visual Data Narrative was crafted with four
data visualization frames of bar charts and a line chart. A blue and orange color
scheme was utilized encoding blue objects as historical data and orange objects as
current data. An introduction frame with a defining text sentence about Intel was
sequenced in front of the four visualization frames, followed by a summarization
frame with two text statements about the optimism and buy rating of financial ana-
lysts. A summarizing statement of insight was defined above each visualization frame
in a light grey box, functioning as a sequential navigation button. A title was provided
for each frame summarizing the content with the assistance of standard axis labelling
and details where appropriate.
In the second version of our story, the Documentary Data Narrative used was
constructed as a full motion video walk through of the same data visualization
frames presented in the example of Visual Data Narrative. The example includes
human voice over narrative describing the dynamic changes to the data represented
in the data visualization.
In the third version, our source for the Computer Generated Text Data Narrative
is an investing column projecting earnings for the Intel Corporation (Forbes, 2015).
The earnings projection story was published a few days before the actual earnings
were released. The article was written by an algorithm at Narrative Science and pub-
lished online in Forbes Magazine (Forbes, 2015). The headline and text of this data
narrative was presented to research subjects in the same manner that it is presented
in the investing column at Forbes Magazine.
In the fourth version, the Human Text Data Narrative uses natural language
written by a human author sourced from Zacks Investment Service from an article
published for the equivalent Intel second quarter earnings announcement (Zacks,
2015). In cases where text sentences equivalent to the Computer Generated Text
Data Narrative were unavailable in the Zacks article, sentences were sourced from
an article published by Seeking Alpha for the Intel second quarter earnings
announcement (Seeking Alpha, 2015). Our objective was the construction of a text
narrative example containing identical content written by a human that was
aligned as much as possible with the Algorithm Text Data Narrative. Sentences for
the Human Text Data Narrative were incorporated from their article sources in
their entirety. Sentences in the Human Text Data Narrative were edited to ensure
the exact same quantitative data elements were presented.
The four types of data narrative communications along with descriptions of their
unique features appear in Table 1. Visual Data Narrative creates the test condition
listed first in the table with sequential frame based visualization construction. Docu-
mentary Data Narrative makes up the next test condition listed second in the table
with full motion video construction. Computer Generated Text Data Narrative
makes up the test condition listed third in the table with Natural Language Generated
text construction. Human Generated Text Data Narrative is the fourth test condition
listed in the table with human authored sentence construction.
186 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
Each of the four examples of data narrative were presented to research subjects in
an equivalent manner with the narrative treatment residing within an equal sized 1024
by 768-pixel frame for viewing content. Instructions for reviewing each type of data
narrative were provided at the top left of the viewing frame as a headline in bold.
Study participants were randomly assigned to view one of the four data narrative con-
structions described in Table 1 and then asked to answer the same set of ten questions.
Participants were allowed to engage the content at their own pace, with all partici-
pants spending a roughly equivalent amount of time on their narrative treatment.
Participants were prevented from re- reviewing the data narrative content while
answering the questions. Participants assigned to review the same data narrative con-
struction were defined as a group of subjects representing that test condition.
Using the four groups of subjects, we applied a quantitative approach to examine
the significance of differences in the average quiz score performance for each of the
research subject groups.
Recall of results
A common data set was utilized to support each of the four types of data narrative
content. The exact same quantitative measurements were defined in each type of
data narrative. Where possible, sentences and headlines used the same size and font
across all of the data narrative types, paying respect to the original intent of the
type of data narrative construction. The factual statements used to construct the
stimulus materials for each data narrative is described below:
All trend questions provided three randomly sequenced answer choices for the
possible trend directions of increased, decreased, or stayed the same. The questions
requiring quantitative measure recall were specified as:
Four answer choices were presented randomly in a vertical sequence for each
quantitative measure question. An example of the randomly sequenced quantitative
measure choices for the question regarding earnings estimates in the year 2015 is
found below:
. 3.16
. .89
. 1.84
. 2.11
The final question regarding the trend of analysts’ sentiment was stated as?
Three randomly sequenced answer choices were provided for analysts’ sentiment
as more optimistic, less optimistic, and about the same. The ten questions were pre-
sented in the same sequence of profit questions, revenue questions, earnings ques-
tions, and finally the sentiment question.
Method
Our study was conducted to assess the knowledge transfer capabilities of emerging
forms of data narrative. Data narrative constructions involving Visual Data Narra-
tive, Documentary Data Narrative, Computer Generated Text Data Narrative, and
188 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
Human Text Data Narrative were presented as data driven communication for par-
ticipants. Each participant was presented with one of the four data narrative con-
structions followed by a ten-item survey of questions designed to assess their recall
of the information presented in the data narrative. Prior to reviewing the data narra-
tives, participants were asked a series of preliminary questions to determine their
interest in financial investments, their knowledge in financial matters, and their
opinion of the products and reputation of the Intel Corporation. The survey con-
cluded with a short set of demographic questions regarding standard indicators of
age, gender, income, industry, level of education, and occupation.
Story; Group 3: Computer Authored Text Story; Group 4: Human Authored Text
Story). Answers to the 10 questions assessing recall/understanding formed the
scoring baseline for comparison of the groups.
Results
A one-way between-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to explore
the impact of data narrative construction on knowledge transfer, as measured by the
survey questions on recall for the factual content of the data narratives. Participants
were divided into four groups according to the type of data narrative they reviewed
(Group 1: Visual Data Narrative; Group 2: Documentary Data Narrative; Group
3: Computer Generated Text Data Narrative; Group 4: Human Text Data Narrative).
There was a significant main effect of data narrative type on recall scores for the four
groups: F(3, 508) = 9.84, p < .001, ŋ2p = .05. The results of the post-hoc analysis is pre-
sented in Table 1.
Post-hocs using the Tukey Honest Significant Difference (HSD), highlighted by
an asterisk in Table 1, indicated that participants’ mean recall scores for the Visual
Data Narrative type of construction performed significantly lower with our test audi-
ence than the other three types of data narrative construction. Mean recall scores in
Groups 2, Group 3 and Group 4 did not differ significantly from each other.
Bonferroni corrections were applied to the p-values for the relevant comparison
groups. The null hypothesis of a Computer Generated Text Data Narrative being
as effective at knowledge transfer as a Human Generated Text Data Narrative was
affirmed with a value of p = .9992971, with a correction of (1.000000). Visual Data
Narratives underperformed Human Text Data Narratives with a value of p
= .0000246, with a correction of (.000025). Visual Data Narratives also underper-
formed Computer Generated Text Data Narratives with a value of p = .0000181,
with a correction of (.000018). Documentary Data Narratives significantly outper-
formed Visual Data Narratives with a value of p = .0001470, with a correction of
(.00015).
Discussion
In this paper, we began with the purpose of assessing various forms of data narrative
that have emerged over the last several years. Data Visualization, Narrative Visualiza-
tion, Data Stories, and Natural Language Generated Text Stories have become very
popular in the field of management. A great deal of time and energy has been devoted
to these increasingly used methods. Our analysis was designed to apply an empirical
approach to determine if enhanced data narrative communication techniques worked
as well as traditional text narratives, and if they did, which ones worked better, and
why?
Our first research question was an inquiry about the ability of enhanced forms of
data narrative transferring knowledge better than traditional human generated text.
In the case of Computer Generated Text Data Narratives, our results were consistent
with our expectation. We expected participants to understand and recall the infor-
mation presented in the Computer Generated Text Data Narrative at least as well
as the Human Generated Text Data Narrative. After reviewing the Computer Gener-
ated Text Data Narrative, participants scored as well on the ten survey questions as
190 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
the participants that had reviewed a Human Generated Text Data Narrative. Our
results suggest that our example of Computer Generated Text Data Narrative, as
an enhanced form of data narrative, did not perform better or worse than our
example of a data narrative written by a human. In terms of utility, a Computer Gen-
erated Text Data Narrative is not necessarily designed with the objective of improving
knowledge transfer compared to a human authored text data narratives. The objec-
tive of Computer Generated Text Data Narrative is providing a communication
medium that is as effective at knowledge transfer as a human authored text narrative.
The benefit of using Computer Generated Text Data Narratives is located in the cost
savings of not hiring a human to write the data narrative. Given this specific objective,
Computer Generated Text Data Narratives performed as well as Human Generated
Text Data Narratives in our study.
In the case of Visual Data Narratives, we expected Visual Data Narratives to
exhibit a superior capability of knowledge transfer. The results of our study suggest
that the Visual Data Narrative we created underperformed their text-based counter-
parts to a significant degree. Participants that reviewed the example Visual Data Nar-
rative content scored much lower on the ten survey questions requesting recall of
information presented in the data narrative. Participants that reviewed the example
Visual Data Narrative scored significantly lower on average than the participants
that reviewed the example Human Generated Text Data Narrative and Computer
Generated Text Data Narrative. The results of our study suggest that for the
chosen examples we created, the model of Visual Data Narrative did not transfer
knowledge as effectively as the examples of text based data narratives, when reviewed
by a general audience that may lack advanced visual data literacy.
Our second research question asks if data narratives constructed with data visu-
alizations are more effective at knowledge transfer than text narratives. Our study
had mixed results on this question. When our example of frame based Visual Data
Narratives was used without the enhancements of documentary film, the results of
our study suggested that the Visual Data Narrative did not perform better at knowl-
edge transfer than the text data narrative counterparts. On the other hand, our results
suggest that when our example of Visual Data Narrative is constructed using video
presentation, accompanied with verbal voice-of-god narration, the recall perform-
ance of participants improves to the same level as our examples of text data narratives
generated by a human or computer.
Our third research question explored if Documentary Data Narratives are more
effective at knowledge transfer than Visual Data Narratives. Our results suggest
that our examples of Documentary Data Narrative communication techniques out-
performed our examples of Visual Data Narrative communication techniques to a
significant degree. Participants that were asked to review the Documentary Data Nar-
rative example scored significantly higher on the ten survey questions requesting
recall from the information presented, when compared to the group of participants
that reviewed the same content presented in a stepper click based Visual Data Nar-
rative approach.
Our original aspiration in developing this study was to provide some empirical
evidence in favor of enhanced techniques for information presentation. The results
of our study suggest that enhanced techniques for information presentation do
play a productive role in the presentation of complex information for a broad audi-
ence. There is a large body of expert knowledge in the academic community that
Journal of Management Analytics 191
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).
ORCID
Judd D. Bradbury http://orcid.org/0000-0003-3664-2315
References
Allen, N. D., Templon, J. R., McNally, P. S., Birnbaum, L., & Hammond, K. (2010,
November). Statsmonkey: A data-driven sports narrative writer. In 2010 AAAI Fall
Symposium Series.
Amini, F., Henry Riche, N., Lee, B., Hurter, C., & Irani, P. (2015, April). Understanding data
videos: Looking at narrative visualization through the cinematography lens. In Proceedings
of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1459–
1468). ACM.
Aristotle, R. W. (1984). Rhetoric. New York: Random House.
Automated Insights. (2015). Introducing wordsmith. Automated insights website. Retrieved
March 1, 2015. http://automatedinsights.com/wordsmith/
Barkhi, R., & Pirkul, H. (1999). An experimental analysis of face to face versus computer
mediated communication channels. Group Decision and Negotiation, 8(4), 325–347.
Bordwell, D., Thompson, K., & Smith, J. (1997). Film art: An introduction. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Borgo, R., Kehrer, J., Chung, D. H., Maguire, E., Laramee, R. S., Hauser, H., & Chen, M.
(2013, May). Glyph-based visualization: Foundations, design guidelines, techniques and
applications. In Eurographics (STARs) (pp. 39–63).
192 J. D. Bradbury and R. E. Guadagno
Bradbury, J. D., & Guadagno, R. E. (2020). Documentary narrative visualization: Features and
modes of documentary film in narrative visualization. Information Visualization, 19(4), 339–
352.
Business Insider. (2014). If you don’t think robots can replace journalists, check out this article
written by a computer. Business Insider Website. http://www.businessinsider.com/narrative-
science-quill-gamechanger-2014-7
Card, M. (1999). Readings in information visualization: Using vision to think. San Francisco:
Morgan Kaufmann.
Carlson, J. R., & Zmud, R. W. (1999). Channel expansion theory and the experiential nature of
media richness perceptions. Academy of Management Journal, 42(2), 153–170.
CITO Research & Narrative Science. (2015). The automated analyst: transforming data into
stories with advanced natural language generation. Narrative Science Whitepaper.
Retrieved March 1, 2015. http://resources.narrativescience.com/h/i/114675300-white-paper-
the-automated-analyst
Cohn, N. (2013). Visual narrative structure. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 413–452.
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness
and structural design. Management Science, 32(5), 554–571.
Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media selection,
and manager performance: Implications for information systems. MIS Quarterly, 11(3),
355–366.
Dennis, A. R., Fuller, R. M., & Valacich, J. S. (2008). Media, tasks, and communication pro-
cesses: A theory of media synchronicity. MIS Quarterly, 32(3), 575–600.
Denstadli, J. M., Julsrud, T. E., & Hjorthol, R. J. (2012). Videoconferencing as a mode of com-
munication: A comparative study of the use of videoconferencing and face-to-face meetings.
Journal of Business and Technical Communication, 26(1), 65–91.
Elmqvist, N., Stasko, J., & Tsigas, P. (2008). Datameadow: A visual canvas for analysis of large-
scale multivariate data. Information Visualization, 7(1), 18–33.
Eriksson, L., Byrne, T., Johansson, E., Trygg, J., & Vikström, C. (2013). Multi-and megavariate
data analysis basic principles and applications. Umeå: Umetrics Academy.
Fiske, J. (2010). Introduction to communication studies. Oxfordshire: Routledge.
Forbes. (2015a). Narrative science partner. Forbes Magazine. Retrieved March 1, 2015. http://
www.forbes.com/sites/narrativescience/
Forbes. (2015b). Intel profit expected to slip. Forbes Magazine. http://www.forbes.com/sites/
narrativescience/2015/07/13/intel-profit-expected-to- slip/
Freytag, G. (1896). Freytag’s technique of the drama: An exposition of dramatic composition and
art. Chicago, IL: Scholarly Press.
Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds: On the psychological activities of reading.
New Haven: Yale University Press.
Gershon, N., & Page, W. (2001). What storytelling can do for information visualization.
Communications of the ACM, 44(8), 31–37.
Green, M. C. (2004). Storytelling in teaching. APS Observer, 17, 4.
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of nar-
rative persuasion.
Haroz, S., Kosara, R., & Franconeri, S. L. (2015, April). Isotype visualization: Working
memory, performance, and engagement with pictographs. In Proceedings of the 33rd
annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 1191–1200). ACM.
Heer, J., & Bostock, M. (2010, April). Crowdsourcing graphical perception: using mechanical
turk to assess visualization design. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human
factors in computing systems (pp. 203–212). ACM.
Howson, C. (September 4, 2014). Visual data discovery: 4 Storytelling approaches compared.
Information Week.
Hullman, J., & Diakopoulos, N. (2011). Visualization rhetoric: Framing effects in narrative
visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 17(12), 2231–
2240.
Hullman, J., Drucker, S., Riche, N. H., Lee, B., Fisher, D., & Adar, E. (2013). A deeper under-
standing of sequence in narrative visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and
Computer Graphics, 19(12), 2406–2415.
Journal of Management Analytics 193
Shannon, C. E., Weaver, W., & Burks, A. W. (1951). The mathematical theory of communication.
Chicago: UI Press.
Shaw, G., Brown, R., & Bromiley, P. (1998). Strategic stories: How 3M is rewriting business
planning. Harvard Business Review, 76(3), 41–49.
Shneiderman, B. (2003). The eyes have it: A task by data type taxonomy for information visu-
alizations. In The craft of information visualization (pp. 364–371). San Francisco: Morgan
Kaufmann.
Shneiderman, B. (2008, June). Extreme visualization: squeezing a billion records into a million
pixels. In Proceedings of the 2008 ACM SIGMOD international conference on Management
of data (pp. 3–12). ACM.
Slate. (2014). The first news report on the L.A. earthquake was written by a robot. Slate
Magazine. http://www.slate.com/blogs/future_tense/2014/03/17/quakebot_los_angeles_
times_robot_journalist_writes_article_on_la_earthquake.html
Tableau. (2014). 8.2 Preview: storypoints. Tableau. http://www.tableau.com/about/blog/2014/5/
82-preview-tell-story-your-data-story-points- 30761
The Atlantic. (2012). Can the computer at narrative science replace paid writers. The Atlantic
Magazine. http://www.theatlantic.com/entertainment/archive/2012/04/can-the- computers-
at-narrative-science-replace-paid-writers/255631/
The guardian. (2015). datablog. Retrieved March 1, 2015. http://www.theguardian.com/data
Tversky, B., Morrison, J. B., & Betrancourt, M. (2002). Animation: Can it facilitate?
International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57(4), 247–262.
Ward, M. O. (2008). Multivariate data glyphs: Principles and practice. In Handbook of data
visualization (pp. 179–198). Berlin: Springer.
Yee, K. P., Fisher, D., Dhamija, R., & Hearst, M. (2001). Animated exploration of dynamic
graphs with radial layout. In IEEE Symposium on Information Visualization, 2001.
INFOVIS 2001. (pp. 43–50). IEEE.
Yi, J. S., Kang, Y., & Stasko, J. (2007). Toward a deeper understanding of the role of interaction
in information visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 13
(6), 1224–1231.
Yseop. (2015). What is smart BI. Yseop website. Retrieved March 1, 2015. http://yseop.com/
EN/smart-business-intelligence-why
Zacks. (2015). Earnings preview. Zacks Website. Retrieved March 01, 2015, http://www.zacks.
com/earnings.
Ziemkiewicz, C., & Kosara, R. (2008). The shaping of information by visual metaphors. IEEE
Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 14(6), 1269–1276.