Visible Learning
Visible Learning
One of the most influential research works currently in the field of teaching and learning
is that carried out by John Hattie , from the University of Auckland in New Zealand, who
for fifteen years carried out research based on 800 meta-analyses that have assumed a
total of 50,000 studies and a sample of 80,000 students. As we will see later, the
conclusions reached by Hattie seem to us to be of special interest and applicable to CLIL:
Among the conclusions reached by Hattie (2006, 2011) we will highlight the following:
The most influential aspect in learning is feedback , both that which the teacher
offers to the student and that which the teacher receives from the student. In the
first case, we must distinguish between feedback and praise, the latter has little
value if it is not associated with the work that has been done. Feedback is more
influential than written feedback and must be individualized . On the other hand,
the feedback that the teacher receives is very valuable as we will comment later.
Another of the most influential aspects is the application of the approach called
“Mastery Learning” which presupposes that all students are capable of meeting
the success criteria if the appropriate learning conditions are met. Namely: high
level of cooperation between colleagues; targeted teacher feedback that is both
frequent, diagnostic, and individualized; allow flexibility in the time the student
needs to achieve the objectives.
The expectations that the student has of himself and those that the teacher has
are decisive in improvement and success.
The most important thing is not the content knowledge that the teacher has of his
subject but rather that he be a good evaluator of his own process so that he asks
himself and looks for solutions to the problems he has, using different
approaches: "assess your impact." For this, the feedback they receive about their
performance, both explicit from the students and the effect it has on learning, is of
utmost importance.
Before planning a lesson, the teacher should know what the students know and
can do.
Expert teachers should set challenging goals and not simply ask them to do their
best . They should also invite students to get involved in challenges and achieving
objectives. To achieve this, it is best to communicate and share with students what
objectives are sought and what needs to be done to achieve them.
On the other hand, they create a climate in the classroom where error is welcome.
Expert teachers believe that all students can meet the success criteria. This
expectation implies believing that intelligence is changeable and not fixed, and
that it develops further with exercise . This is related to the concepts of “fixed
mindset” and “growth mindset” (Dweck 2006).
John Hattie's theory is based on the assumption that practically all strategies work to
learn. Of course, there are some that work better than others. To understand its
effectiveness, Hattie has defined a model that identifies the impact on learning of a total
of 150 factors. Based on this, it has created a measurement system that establishes that
any contribution, starting with a score of 0.4, implies a real improvement in the students'
achievements.
Hattie proposes the following as the conceptual bases of visible learning:
1. I am an evaluator.
2. I am an agent of change.
3. I'm talking about learning (not teaching).
4. Exams are feedback.
5. I motivate students with dialogues (not monologues).
6. I like challenges.
7. Motivate through positive relationships.
8. I use the language of learning.
9. I understand learning as hard work.
10. I collaborate and I make people collaborate.
This strategy can be useful to apply when students develop understandings over time. It
may be a concept that students already know a lot about in a certain context, but the
proposed instruction proposes focusing learning on a new situation. Or a concept that
students know, but only in an informal way. Every time you obtain new information, you
can build bridges between new ideas and previous knowledge. The focus is more on
understanding and connecting one's own thoughts than on achieving a specific result.
This strategy can be used as an introduction, where the student writes his initial ideas
individually on paper. For example, if the topic is "rural population", then students will be
able to write 3 thoughts, 2 questions, and 1 analogy. Then students could read an article,
watch a video or participate in an activity related to “rural population.” Provocative
activities and experiences that push students to think in new directions are best. After the
experience, students complete another 3, 2, 1. Students then share their initial and new
thinking, explaining to their classmates how and why their thinking changed.
Make it clear to students that their initial thought is not judged to be correct or not, it is
just a starting point.
The strategy works best when the student responds using all three elements together at
the same time, e.g. "I observe... I think... I wonder..." However, you may experience that
students start using one item at a time, and you have to ask questions to get them to the
next item.
The strategy works well in a group discussion, but in some cases you may want to apply it
individually in writing or have them think for a while before starting to share ideas with
the class. Student responses to the strategy can be written or recorded, so a set of
observations, interpretations and questions can be listed and returned to during the
course.
One of the first systematic studies carried out regarding forgetting were the memory
experiments of Ebbinghaus (1885), defining what is known as the Forgetting Curve.
To do this, this psychologist decided to study memory by investigating his own memory
capacity. He created 2,300 meaningless syllables (consonant-vowel-consonant, for example
“IFD”) and grouped them into lists. The study consisted of learning lists of 13 syllables that
were repeated until no errors were made on two successive attempts. Subsequently, he
evaluated his retention capacity at intervals between twenty minutes and one month.
The results found showed that forgetting occurred already, even after the shortest intervals,
and that it increased as time passed, much at first, and more slowly later in a logarithmic
function.
Thus, the results of the experiments indicate that after the moment of memorizing the list, the
level of memory dropped drastically in the first moments, and more than half of the material
learned during the first day could fade from consciousness. After this, the material continues
to fade, but the amount that is forgotten in a given time decreases until it reaches a point,
approximately after a week, when no further loss occurs.
To estimate the forgetting rate in each period, the “ savings method ” was used, consisting of
measuring the time it took to relearn the list in each interval, so that the more trials needed to
relearn it, the greater the forgetting.
Ebbinghaus showed that 75% of what was learned was forgotten after just 48 hours. This
conclusion has been supported by other authors such as Bloom in 1981.
From the author's perspective, the loss between the material that is initially learned and that
which is maintained is due to both the passage of time and the non-use of the information.
This is what is known as the Decay Theory , whereby the loss of information is mainly due to
the little use given to the information, so the memory trace left in our body weakens and
fades over time.
If the review variable is added to the forgetting curve, a new, totally different graph is
obtained. Thus, according to this new graph, if a review is done the day after studying a
material, 100% of it is remembered again and, what is more important, the forgetting curve is
modified.
The conclusion is obvious, if we want to modify the forgetting curve and achieve more
effective learning we must review but...
By analyzing the forgetting curve we know that, if the information is not reviewed within 24
hours of learning, approximately half of what was learned will have been forgotten. Therefore,
it is advisable to review the next day and, as a general rule, the more times the better, but at a
minimum:
a first review within 24 hours of the first learning, which will allow us to recover 100% of
what was learned
one or two intermediate reviews between days 7 and 15 of the first learning
a final review prior to the memory test (the exam) in which, and if intermediate reviews
have been carried out, we can achieve a recall rate of 90% of what was initially learned