Schizophrenia Mood Questionnaire Study
Schizophrenia Mood Questionnaire Study
Ho Taik Sung1,2, Palak A. Fichadia1,3, Sreya Kongala1, Mingxin Li1, Susan Sperry1,
Abstract
1 SUNY Upstate Medical University, 750 East Adams Street, Syracuse, NY 13210
Creighton University, 7710 Mercy Road, Suite 601, Omaha, NE 68124 (Present Address)
2
3 Icahn School of Medicine at Mout Sinai/NYC Health+Hospitals, 7901 Broadway Elmhurst, NY 11373 (Present address)
Background: Schizophrenia is widely recognized to include deficits in the expression, experience, and
highlights how emotional processing can vary dramatically between patients from different diagnostic
groups. The five questions inquire about hunger, anxiety, depression, anger, and fear. These questions
assess the level of ego organization, which is closely connected with thought processing. The latter tends
to be impaired in schizophrenia. The “Differentiation of Moods” has no formal scoring. This study
applies a novel scoring system which is modeled from the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale
Objectives: To examine the feasibility and inter-rater reliability of the Mood Differentiation
Questionnaire in schizophrenia spectrum disorders and non-schizophrenia patients. The responses were
rated based on the new scoring guide by two different raters, blind to the participants' diagnostic groups.
Method: This is an observational study of 20 patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders as test
subjects and 20 non-schizophrenia patients as controls. The responses were analyzed according to a new
scoring guide and further classified based on the type of responses as coherent/adequate response (1),
marginal but coherent (2), concrete mode (3), and significant difficulty with abstract thinking with an
Results: The “Differentiation of Moods” questionnaire was well received by patients, easy to administer,
and scored according to the new guide. The inter-rater reliability was calculated using the Bland-Altman
analysis and was found to have substantial agreement, suggesting that the scores obtained had significant
inter-rater reliability.
Conclusion: Strong inter-rater reliability was achieved using the new scoring system derived from the
PANSS N5 scoring guide. Patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder tend to have poorly
disorders.
With 13% of the total global illness burden attributable to all diseases, the burden of illness from mental
health disorders can be considered the greatest of all health issues. (Samartzis and Talias 2019) Serious
and chronic mental illnesses, like schizophrenia, carry social, health, and financial burdens for patients,
their families, and society. (Knapp et al. 2004) Research has demonstrated that early identification and
treatment of psychotic illness can lead to greater improvement in symptoms and function. (Kane et al.
2016) For example, at 5-year follow-up, 281 adults in the early detection and treatment for psychosis
program showed significant improvement in global assessment of functioning for social functioning as
well as improved scores on the negative, depressive, and cognitive factors on the Positive and Negative
Syndrome Scale. The duration of untreated psychosis of greater than a year is associated with less
response to treatment, more persistent positive symptoms, and lower global function. (Black et al. 2001)
This highlights the importance of accurate diagnoses when patients present in acute care settings.
Understanding the full individual is necessary for meaningful psychiatric and psychodiagnostic work. To
understand the complexity of the patient as a whole and have an accurate psychiatric diagnosis, the
similar to various diagnostic tests and scans in medicine. Most psychiatric illnesses are multifactorial, and
no biological tests have been developed that determine the illness from which a patient suffers.
Psychologists are integral to the healthcare team, especially in the inpatient unit. However, based on a
recent study, many inpatient psychologists are available on the unit only once or twice a week due to the
increasing need and chronic deficit in the outpatient setting. (Berry et al. 2022) Because of the limited
number of psychologists providing psychological assessments in inpatient settings, there is an urgent need
to develop additional means of identifying individuals with psychotic illnesses. Novel instruments that
can be administered by psychiatric residents, interns, and medical students are one way this care gap can
be addressed.
Emotional deficits in schizophrenia have been well-documented. Tremeau (2006) conducted a
comprehensive review of the literature and noted that individuals with schizophrenia display deficits in
emotional expression, experience, and recognition (Tremeau et al. 2006). A meta-analytic review has
shown that individuals with schizophrenia have deficits in facial emotion identification and
discrimination tasks. (Kohler et al. 2010) Individuals with schizophrenia are significantly less expressive
significant and stable effect size for deficits in the perception of emotional prosody in schizophrenia
across seventeen studies. (Hoekert et al. 2007) Regarding emotional experience, while research suggests
that individuals with schizophrenia reported their feelings as positive and negative in response to
situations and stimuli evoking those emotions as compared to those without schizophrenia, individuals
with schizophrenia may report experiencing more emotional experiences, including emotions that are
incongruent with stimuli, such as negative and at times positive emotional reactions to neutral stimuli and
negative emotions in response to positive stimuli. Differences in emotional experience may be useful in
Deficiencies in abstraction, driven by cognitive deficits that are known to precede the development of
positive symptoms of schizophrenia, may underlie the aforementioned deficits in emotional expression,
experience, and recognition. Virtanen et al (2017) highlighted the impact of cognitive impairment on the
ability to recognize emotion in facial expressions. Another study demonstrated a significant positive
correlation between the response latency in emotion recognition (measured via the Emotion Recognition
Test of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery) and difficulties in abstract thinking
(measured via PANSS) (Virtanen et al. 2017). This association could possibly be explained by the
observation that patients with schizophrenia, especially those who are cognitively impaired, increasingly
adopt maladaptively concrete thinking. For instance, Gorham demonstrated that psychotic patients
experienced a decline in abstract scores and a rise in concrete scores. To fully grasp and recognize
emotions, the individual should have undergone a cognitive process involving a level of abstraction well
beyond concrete thinking employed by many patients with schizophrenia, in which the person has
intellectualized the emotional content of their reactions to previous experiences (Zierhut et al. 2022).
The association between deficiencies in abstraction and emotional understanding could also have a
pathophysiologic basis. Per van del Heuvel MP et al (2014), patients with schizophrenia were found to
have abnormal brain connectivity in the medial temporal limbic and midline diencephalon areas (van del
Heuvel et al. 2014). In addition, Lezak et al (2004) described patients with damage to these areas who
were more likely to have memory storage and consolidation problems. Due to these deficits, these
patients found difficulty assigning meaning to the materials needed to be remembered. These findings, in
conjunction with a known role in the medial temporal limbic area in emotional regulation, may explain
the reason that individuals with schizophrenia often face difficulty in imbuing—and later retrieving—
In gauging the deficits in cognition and abstraction for patients with schizophrenia, the Abstract Thinking
subscale (N5) of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) is most commonly employed in
clinical and research settings. The N5 of PANSS consists of 32 items: 16 similarities and 16 proverbs.
Scoring is based on the patient’s ability to identify similarities between items and interpret the meaning of
proverbs. Proverbs, in particular, can be highly cultural and language-specific, and both the interpretation
by subjects and the evaluation by scorers could be variable depending on their cultural background and
familiarity with the proverbs. The PANSS also does not contain a section that specifically tests for the
patient’s ability to grasp and recognize emotions, which is crucial in successful interpersonal interactions.
Robert Charles Powell, in his book “Differentiation of Moods,” noted that patients with schizophrenia
offered very specific answers in response to questions that asked them to identify their emotions. In
contrast to N5 of PANSS, these five questions conceived by Powell inquire about emotions such as
hunger, anger, fear, sadness, and anxiety, which are universal and shared across cultures. The answers to
these questions may provide information on whether the patient’s mood is well-differentiated, poorly
differentiated, or undifferentiated. In those with poorly differentiated moods, the answer might be literal
or functional in nature, for example, providing a “concrete response” in which the patient relates the
mood to recent precepts or specific examples. For those with undifferentiated moods, the answer might be
entirely incorrect or nonsensical in that they are entirely unable to address the question of how they know
Building upon the work by Powell, we devised a questionnaire and scoring system that classifies the
responses to the questions based on the degrees of “mood differentiation” and abstraction impairment.
Because the test only contains five questions, it can be quickly and easily administered by a broad group
of mental health providers, such as medical students, residents, psychiatrists, and psychologists. This
novel approach has the potential to succinctly evaluate not only deficits in abstraction but also emotional
understanding in psychotic patients—simply by posing five questions about universal feelings of anger,
anxiety, and depression. The accompanying scoring guide has also been designed to maximize ease of use
and minimize errors in interpretation. This is particularly important if the instrument is to be used
Through our study, we hope to highlight the growing importance of these tests and the promising results
This observational study took place in an acute psychiatric 24-bed unit of an academic hospital located in
the city of Central New York, USA. The population of the city is 146,000. The unit admits an average of
75 patients per month from emergency rooms, medical floors, and outlying facilities. The study was
approved by the Institutional Review Board of Upstate Medical University and was carried out per the
latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were native English speakers and belonged
to different ethnicities. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants are summarized in
Table 1. 21 participants with schizophrenia spectrum disorders were selected based on chart review..
Another 20 patients without schizophrenia spectrum disorders diagnoses were selected for the study as
controls.
2.1 Measures
The demographic form was administered after the patient consented, and the chart was reviewed. The
Mood Differentiation Questionnaire was administered. The study’s questionnaire draws from Robert
Powell’s essay “Differentiation of Moods: as a Reflection of Ego Organization and Personality Style.” In
the booklet, Powell discusses a heuristic diagnostic approach in which the clinician poses five questions
about emotions to patients. These five questions have been adopted into a questionnaire format which can
be readily administered in any mental health treatment setting (refer to Appendix A).
2.2 Scoring
A scoring guide has also been developed to serve as a guideline in grading the patients’ responses (refer
to Appendix B). The scoring guide borrows conceptually from Powell’s essay, which categorizes the
potential responses based on the patient’s hypothesized ego organization and personality style. Per
Powell, the responses can range from those demonstrating a clear, personal concept of each mood
(usually from patients with normal/neurotic personalities and well-differentiated moods) to those that
only indirectly address—relying on concrete examples, for example—the question of how the patient
knows when the specific mood is present (from patients with poorly differentiated mood).
These different types of responses were then grouped into four separate levels based on the connoted
degree of impairment in forming concepts and understanding emotions. This categorization borrows from
previous work by Leontieva et al. (2019), which outlined a new scoring guide for the PANSS N5
Similarities and Proverbs scale (Leontieva et al. 2019). Listed in increasing order of impairment in
abstraction, the four levels are as follows: normal/neurotic response; marginal but coherent response;
literal/functional response; and incorrect/psychotic response. If the patient’s response to a question
demonstrated components of different levels of abstraction, the higher score (i.e., the “more
differentiated” response) was given as long as it constituted a significant part of the answer. Each of the
five mood questions was scored separately and then aggregated to determine the final score for the
A study team member conducted the interview in rooms within the unit, free from distractions and noise,
which was timed around 25-30 minutes. No follow-up was necessary. The responses were rated according
to the PANSS N5 new scoring guide by a Psychiatrist and a Psychiatry resident. The cultural backgrounds
of the raters were as follows: LL was Russian-born, fluent English speaker, American-educated, and fully
acculturated; HS was Korean-born, fluent English speaker, American-educated, and fully acculturated.
The raters were blind to the patient identifiers, such as names and diagnoses.
For inter-rater reliability, Bland-Altman analysis was performed to assess the agreement between the two
raters, and a significant agreement between the two raters was assigned as 4.098.
3. Results
The socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants are presented in Table 1 and
Table 2. Briefly, Caucasians constituted the largest proportion (80.4%) of the study participants,
followed by African-Americans (14.6%). The gender distribution was about equal, with 22 male
participants and 19 females. There was also no statistical difference in the gender distribution between
test and control subjects (p value = 0.427). The majority of the patients were housed either in an assisted
living facility or living independently, with a minority of employed individuals (22%). A large fraction of
spectrum disorder (1 patient). Under the mood disorder category, the following diagnoses have been
included - (12 patients), adjustment disorder (1 patient), borderline personality disorder (1 patient),
bipolar type 1 (1 patient), panic disorder (1 patient), unspecified depressive disorder (2 patients),
unspecified trauma and stressor-related disorder (2 patients), and unspecified anxiety disorder (1 patient).
The average ages of the study participants with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and study participants
without schizophrenia spectrum disorders diagnoses were similar at 41.4 and 38.6, respectively (Table
3). Educational level was also similar to the average number of years completed at 12.1 and 13.9,
respectively. The average number of previous hospitalizations was likewise similar at 3.8 and 4.1,
respectively.
For scorer 1, the mean aggregated score of the questionnaire per patient (refer to Methods section) was 12
with a standard deviation of 2.9. For scorer 2, the mean score was higher at 15.3 with a standard deviation
of 2.6.
In terms of correlation analysis of questions from the questionnaire (Table 4), question 1 (“hungry”) was
associated with the best correlation between the raters, with R 2 value of 0.62 (p value<0.001). In contrast,
question 2 (“anxious”) was associated with the worst correlation between the raters with R 2 value of 0.41
(p value=0.008). In terms of Bland-Altman analysis (Table 4), it was found that question 5 (“afraid”)
showed the highest agreement between raters with the mean difference between raters being 0.585 with
95% limit of agreement of -1.273 to 2.444. This was followed by question 1 (“hungry”) with a mean
difference of 0.732 with 95% limit of agreement of -1.023 to 2.486. The worst agreement between raters
was seen with questions 2 (“anxious”) and 3 ("depressed"), with a mean difference of 1.024 for both
questions.
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of study participants.
DEMOGRAPHICS NUMBER %
Ethnicity: 33 80.4
Caucasian
6 14.6
African American
2 4.8
Others
Housing: 34 87.8
Housed
5 12.1
Unhoused
Working 9 22
Schizophrenia 20 48.7
Table 3. Comparison of aggregated score of the questionnaire per patient between scorers
Table 4. Correlation analysis and Bland-Altman analysis for Questions from Mood
Differentiation Questionnaire
S.No Questions from the Mood R2 value p-value Bland-Altman 95% limit of
. Differentiation Questionnaire (Correlation Analysis agreements
Analysis)
1. Q1. Do you ever take a look at 0.62 <0.001 0.732 -1.023 to 2.486
must be hungry?”
hungry?
else?
2. Q2. Do you ever take a look at 0.41 0.008 1.024 -0.959 to 3.008
must be anxious?”
anxious?
something else?
depressed?
something else?
must be angry?”
else?
must be afraid?”
N = 41
[Mean(SD)]
or getting hungry?
getting anxious?
That the feeling you were
getting depressed?
angry?
something else?
afraid?
something else?
4. Discussion
Our study aims to demonstrate the feasibility and inter-rater reliability of Mood Differentiation
Questionnaire, a novel approach of evaluating thought disorder in schizophrenia. With its ease of
use, the Mood Differentiation Questionnaire could be easily adapted by a broad group of mental
health providers to improve accuracy of diagnosing disturbances in thinking for patients with
schizophrenia.
The study demonstrated that patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder tend to have poorly
Differentiation questions, the patients with schizophrenia scored significantly higher – indicating
that they are more likely to answer in a more literal or incorrect manner which is associated with
achieved strong inter-rater reliability between two scorers, who were of different cultural and
educational backgrounds. The inter-rater reliability was calculated using the Bland-Altman
analysis which found a relatively small mean difference between the raters for the five questions,
with the highest agreement being associated with question 5 (“afraid”). In terms of correlation
analysis, the questionnaire and scoring guide also achieved a good correlation with R2 value
Potential future directions may involve further investigating the applicability of the questionnaire
to clinical settings, for example by devising a new test battery including the Mood
Differentiation Questionnaire (using the revised scoring guide), PANSS N5, and other modalities
such as the Pictogram test which assess disturbances in thinking for patients with schizophrenia
(Leontieva et al. 2008&2022 ). Combining three tests that alone showed good discriminant
ability against diagnoses of schizophrenia spectrum disorders will add incremental validity to
such test battery. By combining the three assessments (Mood Differentiation questions, PANSS
N5, and Pictogram test), we anticipate that this new battery of tests could improve upon the
incremental validity (vs. PANSS N5 alone) while being more widely applicable (e.g. across
5. Limitations
A potential limitation of the study is that due to non-randomized subject selection from an
inpatient psychiatric setting, the study may be subject to a number of selection biases. This
includes the Berksonian bias because the study utilizes hospitalized patients with mood disorders
as the control group. This effect was partially mitigated by the recruitment of patients who were
deemed clinically stable. In addition, while the presence of Berksonian bias would more likely
tilt the outcome toward non-significance, our results showed patients with schizophrenia tended
to score higher compared to control patients -- a finding that was statistically significant for each
of the 5 questions.
Furthermore, it was observed that one of the raters (scorer 2) tended to score the responses
consistently higher, with the mean aggregated score of the questionnaire per patient being 12 for
scorer 1 and 15.3 scorer (Table 3). This was a trend that was noticed for each of the 5 questions.
This indicates that some raters may tend to be more stringent in evaluating that an answer is
mitigate some of the potential ambiguity in the interpretation of the scoring guide and improve
the consistency of scoring between different raters. To this end, a revised and more thorough
version of the scoring guide has been devised (Appendix C), which may be utilized for future
studies.
6. Conclusions
The study, which evaluated the feasibility of utilizing the Mood Differentiation Questionnaire in
patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and non-schizophrenia patients, demonstrated that
patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder tend to have poorly differentiated moods
and the accompanying scoring guide achieved strong inter-rater reliability between two scorers,
who were of different cultural and educational backgrounds. With its ease of use, the Mood
Differentiation Questionnaire could be easily adapted by a broad group of mental health
providers to improve the accuracy of diagnosing disturbances in thinking for patients with
schizophrenia.
Funding statement:
Ho Taik Sung - Writing - Original draft, methodology, results, review and editing,
conceptualization
Palak A.Fichadia - Writing - Original draft, methodology, review and editing, data curation,
project administration
Acknowledgments
We thank Robert Charles Powell for inspiring the creation of the Mood Differentiation
Questionnaire based on his book, in addition to providing continued guidance for the
1. Samartzis, L., & Talias, M. A. (2019). Assessing and Improving the Quality in Mental Health
Services. International journal of environmental research and public health, 17(1), 249.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010249
2. Knapp, M., Mangalore, R., & Simon, J. (2004). The global costs of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
3. Kane, J. M., Robinson, D. G., Schooler, N. R., Mueser, K. T., Penn, D. L., Rosenheck, R. A., ... &
Heinssen, R. K. (2016). Comprehensive versus usual community care for first-episode psychosis: 2-year
outcomes from the NIMH RAISE early treatment program. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(4),
362-372.
4. Black, K., Peters, L., Rui, Q., Milliken, H., Whitehorn, D., & Kopala, L. C. (2001). Duration of untreated
psychosis predicts treatment outcome in an early psychosis program. Schizophrenia research, 47(2-3), 215-
222.
5. Berry, K., Raphael, J., Haddock, G., Bucci, S., Price, O., Lovell, K., Drake, R. J., Clayton, J., Penn, G., &
Edge, D. (2022). Exploring how to improve access to psychological therapies on acute mental health wards
from the perspectives of patients, families and mental health staff: qualitative study. BJPsych open, 8(4),
e112. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2022.513
6. Trémeau, F. (2006). A review of emotion deficits in schizophrenia. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience, 8:1,
7. Kohler, C. G., Walker, J. B., Martin, E. A., Healey, K. M., & Moberg, P. J. (2010). Facial emotion
8. Hoekert, M., Kahn, R. S., Pijnenborg, M., & Aleman, A. (2007). Impaired recognition and expression of
emotional prosody in schizophrenia: review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia research, 96(1-3), 135-145.
9. Virtanen M, Singh-Manoux A, Batty GD, Ebmeier KP, Jokela M, et al. (2017) The level of cognitive
function and recognition of emotions in older adults. PLOS ONE 12(10): e0185513.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185513
10. Zierhut M, Böge K, Bergmann N, Hahne I, Braun A, Kraft J, Ta TMT, Ripke S, Bajbouj
M, Hahn E. The Relationship Between the Recognition of Basic Emotions and Negative
11. van den Heuvel MP, Fornito A. Brain networks in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol
24500505.
12. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004).
13. Powell, R. C. (2017). Differentiation of Moods: as a Reflection of Ego Organization and Personality
Zsuzsa & Batki, Steven. (2019). Development of a Scoring Guide for the Positive and Negative
9854.5.2.61
15. Leontieva, L., Rostova, J., Tunick, R., Golovko, S., Harkulich, J., & Ploutz-Snyder, R.
16. Leontieva L, Golovko S. Construct Validity of the Pictogram Test for Diagnosis of Schizophrenia.
PMC8776534.
Appendix A (The Mood Differentiation Questionnaire):
These five specific questions rapidly provide valuable information suggesting the patient’s probable level
differentiated, poorly differentiated, and undifferentiated moods. While the time employed will vary from
patient to patient, generally it takes about seven minutes to administer the five questions and to listen
very closely to the answers. Please record the patient’s response with as much detail as possible. While
the content is important, the tone (paranoid, schizoid, antisocial, obsessive, dependent, avoidant,
I would like to ask you five very specific questions about your emotions. A lot of people have difficulty
with these five questions, but just do the best that you can. (Patients may be coaxed to say a bit more with
1. Do you ever take a look at yourself and say, “Gee, I guess I must be hungry?”
If so, how would you KNOW that you were feeling hungry or getting hungry?
That the feeling you were feeling must be hunger- and not something else?
2. Do you ever take a look at yourself and say, “Gee, I guess I must be anxious?”
If so, how would you KNOW that you were anxious or getting anxious?
That the feeling you were feeling must be anxiety- and not something else?
3. Do you ever take a look at yourself and say, “Gee, I guess I must be depressed.”
If so, how would you KNOW that you were depressed or getting depressed?
That the feeling you were feeling must be depression- and not something else?
4. Do you ever take a look at yourself and say, “Gee, I guess I must be angry?”
If so, how would you KNOW that you were angry or getting angry?
That the feeling you were feeling must be anger- and not something else?
5. Do you ever take a look at yourself and say, “Gee, I guess I must be afraid?”
If so, how would you KNOW that you were afraid or getting afraid?
That the feeling you were feeling must be fear- and not something else?
Appendix B (Scoring guide for grading patient’s responses to the Mood Differentiation
Questionnaire):
-Directly addresses the question of how the patient personally knows when the specific
mood.
-Not completely perfect response that may have some inaccuracy in abstraction or may
be otherwise idiosyncratic.
-Treatment response: Focuses on the action the person takes upon sensing that the mood
is present.
be feeling.
6-7: Incorrect/psychotic response, demonstrating undifferentiated mood.
-Unable to address the question how they know that a certain mood is present. Patients
may deny experiencing the mood at all and may otherwise be too distracted or
Directly addresses the question of how the patient personally knows when the specific mood
incompleteness in abstraction; sole focus on the action the person takes upon sensing that the
feeling.
Unable to address the question how they know that a certain mood is present. Patient may
say “I don’t know” or deny experiencing the mood at all. Patient also may provide
completely incoherent or unrelated answer.
*If the answer has multiple levels of response, give the higher score as long as it constitutes a
significant part of the answer. For example, if an answer demonstrates both normal/neurotic (1) and