Medicine For The Soul (Non) Re
Medicine For The Soul (Non) Re
RESEARCH ARTICLE
a1111111111 * [email protected]
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111 Abstract
a1111111111
Although the threat and uncertainty of the COVID-19 pandemic has become a significant
source of distress, using religion to cope may be associated with more positive health.
Given the severity and chronicity of the pandemic, religious individuals may also have relied
OPEN ACCESS on a variety of non-religious coping methods. Much of the existing COVID-19 research over-
Citation: Peneycad C, Ysseldyk R, Tippins E, looks the role of religious group membership and beliefs in relation to coping responses and
Anisman H (2024) Medicine for the soul: (Non) associated mental health, with an additional lack of such research within the Canadian con-
religious identity, coping, and mental health during text. Thus, this cross-sectional study investigated relations among religiosity, stressor
the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 19(1):
appraisals, (both religious and non-religious) coping strategies, mental and physical health
e0296436. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0296436 in a religiously-diverse Canadian community sample (N = 280) during the pandemic’s 2nd
wave from March to June 2021. Numerous differences were apparent in appraisal-coping
Editor: Ching Sin Siau, University Kebangsaan
Malaysia, MALAYSIA methods and health across five (non)religious groups (i.e., Atheists, Agnostics, “Spiritual but
not religious”, Christians, and those considered to be religious “Minorities” in Canada). Reli-
Received: May 18, 2023
giosity was also associated with better mental health, appraisals of the pandemic as a chal-
Accepted: December 13, 2023
lenge from which one might learn or grow, and a greater reliance on problem-focused,
Published: January 2, 2024 emotional-engagement, and religious coping. Moreover, both problem-focused and emo-
Copyright: © 2024 Peneycad et al. This is an open tional-engagement coping mediated the relations between religiosity and health. Taken
access article distributed under the terms of the together, this research has implications for individual-level coping as well as informing cul-
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
turally-sensitive public health messages promoting targeted self-care recommendations
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original with integrated religious or spiritual elements during times of threat and uncertainty, such as
author and source are credited. the COVID-19 pandemic.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting Information
files.
The health and fatality risks of COVID-19, heightened distress related to risk of infection, as
well as isolation due to government-imposed social distancing and lockdown measures have
resulted in poor mental health outcomes with lasting impacts world-wide [2–4].
The pandemic evoked a wide range of coping methods to deal with the ongoing threat [5,
6]. As with responses to a variety of stressors, some of these coping strategies have been more
adaptive (e.g., support-seeking, problem-solving) than have others (e.g., drug and alcohol use,
rumination) [7]. Moreover, the use of diverse stressor appraisal and coping strategies may vary
depending on an array of socio-demographic factors, group memberships, and identities [8–
10]. In the context of the present investigation, stressor appraisals refer to an individual’s eval-
uation of a stressful event, whereas coping strategies refer to an individual’s cognitions or
behaviours used to manage the stress [11]. This study investigated associations among self-
reported religious beliefs and identities, stressor appraisals, coping strategies, as well as mental
and physical health during the COVID-19 pandemic in a diverse community sample of partic-
ipants across Canada. In view of the effectiveness of religiosity as a coping strategy among
some individuals [12–15], it was anticipated that those who identified with a religious group
(compared to those who did not) would self-report more adaptive stressor appraisals, coping,
and health outcomes. Given associations among various appraisals and coping strategies with
health [16–18], it was also expected that appraisal-coping processes would mediate the rela-
tions between religiosity and self-reported health.
Method
Participants and procedure
A total of 392 people participated in an online study through Amazon Mechanical Turk
(MTurk)–a crowdsourcing website (http://www.mturk.com) and were compensated $3.00
CAD. After giving written informed consent, participants completed questionnaires assessing
religiosity, stressor appraisals, coping strategies, and general mental and physical health. The
study was available to people over 18 years of age living in Canada and IP addresses were col-
lected to determine geo-location. Responses included 142 from Ontario (Canada’s most
heavily populated province), 34 from Quebec, 32 from British Columbia, 24 from Alberta, 10
from Nova Scotia, 7 from New Brunswick, 6 from Manitoba, 4 from Saskatchewan, and 1
from Newfoundland and Labrador.
Following the removal of 112 incomplete or invalid survey responses, the final sample
totalled 280 participants (142 men, 137 women, 1 other; ranging in age from 18–72 years,
M = 37.35, SD = 11.19). This sample size provided 95% power to detect medium effect sizes
(two-tailed; [56]), based on our primary analyses (ANOVA and MANOVA) and the most con-
servative estimate is reported for conciseness. Data was collected between March 15th and June
4th, 2021, one full year into the COVID-19 pandemic. Approval from the Carleton University
Research Ethics Board-B (CUREB-B) was obtained prior to the commencement of data
collection.
A demographic questionnaire was also completed by participants, in which they self-identi-
fied their age, gender, ethnic/racial identity, religious affiliation, highest degree or level of edu-
cation, and average annual household income. Participants were then categorized into a (non)
religious group for our primary analyses of interest, with the remaining demographics treated
Measures
Religiosity. The Centrality of Religiosity Scale (CRS) [57] consisted of 13 questions to
measure the general intensities of five theoretically defined dimensions of religiosity. Each
item evaluated the frequency or intensity of the activation of religious constructs, rated on a
scale ranging from 1 (never/not at all) to 5 (very often/very much). These dimensions included
intellectual (e.g., “How often do you think about religious issues?”; 3 items; α = .86); ideology
(e.g., “To what extent do you believe in the existence of God or something divine?”; 3 items; α
= .93); public practice (e.g., “How often do you take part in religious services?”; 3 items; α =
.93); private practice (e.g., “How often do you pray?” 2 items; α = .82); and religious experience
(e.g., “How often do you experience situations in which you have the feeling that God or some-
thing divine intervenes in your life?”; 2 items; α = .94). A total religiosity score based on these
five dimensions was also computed (α = .91).
Stressor appraisals. Stressor appraisals in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic were
evaluated on seven dimensions [58], each consisting of four items rated on a scale ranging
from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). These dimensions included threat appraisals (e.g., “How
threatening is this situation?”; 4 items; α = .75; challenge appraisals (e.g., “Is this going to have
a positive impact on me?”; 4 items; α = .65); centrality appraisals (e.g., “Does this situation
have important consequences for me?”; 4 items; α = .88); appraisals of the situation as control-
lable-by-self (e.g., “Do I have what it takes to do well in this situation?”; 4 items; α = .86); con-
trollable-by-others (e.g., “Is there help available to me for dealing with this problem?”; 4 items;
α = .85); and uncontrollable by anyone (e.g., “Is this a totally hopeless situation?”; 4 items; α =
.80). The final dimension reflected the general stressfulness of the pandemic (e.g., “To what
extent do I perceive this situation as stressful?”; 4 items; α = .78).
Coping. Coping with the COVID-19 pandemic was assessed with the short-version Sur-
vey of Coping Profile Endorsements [59], comprising 30-items, reflecting 15 distinct coping
strategies (e.g. cognitive restructuring, rumination, social-support seeking, denial, etc.). Partic-
ipants rated each item on a scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (totally). To reduce the num-
ber of coping dimensions a principal components analysis was conducted, which resulted in
four factors. These factors included emotional-engagement coping (e.g., “I told others that I was
really upset”; α = .81); emotional-avoidance (e.g., “I avoided thinking about the problem”; α =
.66); problem-focused coping (e.g., “I made plans to overcome my concerns or the problem”; α
= .70); and religious coping (e.g., “I turned to God or my faith”; single item).
Mental & physical health. Mental health was assessed with a widely used, global single-
item measure [60], which asked participants to rate their current mental health ranging from 1
(poor) to 5 (excellent). To include a more comprehensive view of health, physical health was
also assessed with a single-item measure [61], which asked participants to rate their current
physical health ranging from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent).
Statistical analyses
A series of analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and multivariate analyses of variances (MANO-
VAs) were conducted to examine potential differences in appraisals, coping, mental and physi-
cal health, and religiosity among the five (non)religious groups. Analyses assessing (non)
religious group differences and relations among stressor appraisals, coping, mental and
physical health, and religiosity that included age, gender, race, education, and income as covar-
iates yielded identical patterns of significant results. However, given that some participants
chose not to report all demographics, to maximize sample size, the analyses presented here are
those that include (non)religious group as the sole predictor.
Correlations then assessed relations among the variables of interest, and mediation analyses
were conducted to determine whether appraisal-coping processes accounted for the relations
between religiosity and health (i.e., mental and physical). To assess these multiple-mediation
models, we conducted regression-based bootstrapping analyses with 95% confidence intervals
(CIs) using Hayes’ (2022) PROCESS macro (Model 4) [62]; this analysis provides evidence of
mediation if the significant direct effect between the predictor and outcome variable is reduced
when the mediator(s) is included, and the 95% CI does not include zero.
Results
Demographics
Participants in the present study were diverse in terms of gender, age, education, income, eth-
nic, racial, and (non)religious identity. Table 1 presents demographics of this sample as a func-
tion of self-identified (non)religious group membership as well as total numbers across all
(non)religious categories.
As seen in Table 3, not surprisingly, Christians and religious “Minorities” reported practic-
ing religious coping significantly more than Atheists (ps < .001), Agnostics (ps < .001), and
those who were Spiritual (p = .002 and p < .001, respectively). Interestingly, religious “Minori-
ties” (p < .001) and those who were Spiritual (p = .053), reported significantly more emo-
tional-engagement coping compared to Atheists. Finally, Atheists reported significantly less
problem-focused coping compared to religious “Minorities” (p = .001), Christians (p = .041),
and those who were Spiritual (p = .044).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.t002
“Minorities” (p = .012), and Atheists (p = .050). Also, Christians self-reported significantly bet-
ter mental health than those who were Spiritual (p = .032). However, there were no significant
differences in self-reported mental health among Christians, Atheists, or religious
“Minorities”.
An ANOVA conducted to assess differences in self-reported physical health, revealed a rela-
tively modest non-significant effect, F(4,275) = 2.211, p = .068, n2 = .031, across (non)religious
groups. Specifically, Atheists reported marginally better physical health compared to religious
“Minorities” (p = .089). Clearly, the links to physical health were appreciably weaker than
those related to self-reported mental health.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.t003
Table 4. Mental and physical health (Means, SDs) as a function of (non)religious group.
Atheist Agnostic Spiritual Christian Religious “Minority” F (4,275) η2
(n = 69) (n = 45) (n = 53) (n = 69) (n = 44)
Mental Health 3.26a (0.99) 2.73b (1.09) 2.91a,b (0.92) 3.43a,c (0.95) 3.18a (1.00) 5.34*** .072
Physical Health 3.57a (0.931) 3.31a (0.925) 3.41a (0.649) 3.23a (0.912) 3.14a (0.795) 2.21 .031
***
p < .001; Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p < .05
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.t004
having the lowest levels of overall religiosity (M = 1.50, SD = 0.58), followed by Agnostics
(M = 1.69, SD = 0.49), Spirituals (M = 2.12, SD = 0.72), Christians (M = 2.68, SD = 1.08), and
religious “Minorities” (M = 3.09, SD = 0.99).
Table 5. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among religiosity, stressor appraisals, & health.
Variable M SD 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Religiosity 2.19 0.99 .003 .25** .07 .08 .03 .07 .10 .14* -.04
Stressor Appraisals
2. Threat 2.99 0.83 __ .02 .68** -.22** -.14** .45** .75** -.26** -.30**
3. Challenge 2.66 0.78 __ __ .22** .51** .39** .06 .15* .15* .09
4. Centrality 3.24 0.91 __ __ __ -.02 -.04 .24** .62** -.24** -.29**
5. Controllable-by- 3.46 0.84 __ __ __ __ .56** -.16** -.13* .35** .22**
self
6. Controllable-by-others 3.14 0.89 __ __ __ __ __ -.22** -.09 .24** .08
7. Uncontrollable 2.31 0.89 __ __ __ __ __ __ .34** .03 -.06
8. Stressfulness 2.99 0.84 __ __ __ __ __ __ __ -.29** -.22**
Health
9. Mental 3.18 1.00 __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ .37**
10. Physical 3.35 0.85 __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __
*
p < .05
**
p < .01
***
p < .001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.t005
-.13, -.03, and problem-focused coping, B = .07, SE = .02, 95% CI = .03, .11, did not overlap
zero (Fig 1).
When the potential mediating roles of coping strategies were assessed in the (non-signifi-
cant) relation between religiosity and physical health, the direct relation (c path; [62]), B = -.03,
SE = .05, p = .50, remained non-significant (c’ path), B = .03, SE = .08, p = .76. However, as
with mental health, the 95% CIs for the mediated paths through both emotional-engagement
(B = -.05, SE = .02; CI = -.09, -.02), and problem-focused coping (B = .04, SE = .02; CI = .01,
.08), did not overlap zero (Fig 2). As a mediation model may involve indirect effects between
two variables even in the absence of direct effects [63], emotional-engagement and problem-
focused coping appeared to indirectly link religiosity and physical health. Taken together, the
use of emotional-engagement and problem-focused coping strategies appeared to fully account
for the tendency for more religious individuals to report greater mental health, as well as indi-
rectly link religiosity with greater physical health.
Table 6. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among religiosity, coping strategies, & health.
Variable M SD 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Religiosity 2.19 0.99 .28** .23** .11 .82** .14* -.04
Coping Strategies
2. Problem-focused 2.23 0.79 __ .39** .44** .22** .15* .04
3. Emotional-engagement 1.42 0.68 __ __ .39** .29** -.22** -.24**
4. Emotional-avoidance 1.95 0.79 __ __ __ .07 -.003 -0.8
5. Religious coping 0.81 1.31 __ __ __ __ .13* -.09
Health
6. Mental 3.18 1.00 __ __ __ __ __ .37**
7. Physical 3.35 0.85 __ __ __ __ __ __
*
p < .05
**
p < .01
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.t006
Fig 1. The mediating role of coping in the relation between religiosity and mental health.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.g001
Discussion
The aim of this research was to examine potential differences in how (non)religious groups
appraised and coped with the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as to assess potential relations
among religious beliefs, stressor appraisals, coping strategies, and mental and physical health
in a religiously-diverse Canadian sample during the pandemic. Given that many countries dif-
fered in their approach to dealing with COVID-19, Canada’s pandemic response was also dis-
tinct in its implementation on several factors. Some of these included stay-at-home and face-
masking requirements, hospital and policy responses, and vaccine rollout, which in turn,
affected the severity of COVID-19 case and death counts. At the time of data collection, stay-
at-home requirements were still in effect, thousands of people were hospitalized with COVID-
19, with over 3000 new cases being reported each day [55]. Thus, the COVID-19 pandemic
was a unique circumstance in which to examine the role of (non)religious group identity on
stressor appraisals, coping, and mental health within a Canadian population. Several variations
emerged across the (non)religious groups in stressor appraisals, coping strategies, and health.
While these differences did not always delineate a clear pattern, Atheists predictably reported
the lowest levels of religiosity, followed by Agnostics, participants who identified as “Spiritual
but not religious”, Christians, and religious “Minorities” reported the highest levels. Religiosity
Fig 2. The mediating role of coping in the relation between religiosity and physical health.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0296436.g002
essentially appeared to serve as a proxy for increasing engagement with one’s religious group
and belief system (or lack thereof). Moreover, when relations among religiosity, appraisal-cop-
ing processes, and health, as well as the potential mediating roles of appraisals and coping were
examined, our findings are consistent with the suggestion that greater religious beliefs were
tied to better well-being (see [64]) both directly and indirectly through (non-religious) emo-
tional-engagement and problem-focused coping strategies during the pandemic.
an increased tendency to appraise the pandemic as a challenge from which one might learn
and grow. Earlier research had provided evidence that challenge appraisals are strongly associ-
ated with more positive emotional reactions and greater use of cognitive strategies, such as
engaging in positive self-statements and active coping [67]. Furthermore, consistent with pre-
vious research [48, 67], our findings revealed that an increased tendency to appraise the pan-
demic as a challenge was associated with more positive mental health (although appraisals did
not statistically mediate this relation). It is possible that people greater in religiosity may have
been more likely to appraise the pandemic as a challenge given that many religions promote
the belief that there is purpose in difficult circumstances because a transcendent force is in
control of such situations [68]. Of course, within the religious “Minorities” group, some indi-
viduals identified as belonging to a religion that upholds belief in a singular God (i.e., Bahá’ı́,
Jewish, Muslim, or Sikh) which may influence differences in appraising the pandemic as a
challenge, compared to those who identified with a religion in which there are multiple gods
(i.e., Hindu) or no gods at all (i.e., Buddhist). Nonetheless, religious “Minorities” and Chris-
tians were significantly more likely to view the pandemic as a challenge when compared to
Atheists. While we found no evidence of significant associations between religiosity and other
types of stressor appraisals (e.g., threat, centrality), these results support the notion that reli-
gious individuals may have additional capacity to view stressors—including the COVID-19
pandemic—as a challenge rather than as a threat.
In addition to problem-focused coping, religiosity was associated with greater use of emo-
tional-engagement coping in the present study, which can include strategies such as emotional
expression and emotional social support-seeking [77, 78]. According to our analyses of group
differences, this relation appeared to be driven largely by those who identified as belonging to
a religious group that is considered a “Minority” in Canada (i.e., Bahá’ı́, Buddhist, Hindu, Jew-
ish, Muslim, and Sikh). Although emotional-engagement styles of coping have previously been
associated with higher levels of well-being [69, 73, 77, 78] our study revealed that emotional-
engagement coping was instead associated with more negative mental and physical health out-
comes. Although our emotional-engagement factor included strategies such as emotional-
expression and social support-seeking, which could have beneficial actions [54, 77, 78], it also
included rumination, other-blame, and wishful thinking—strategies that are generally consid-
ered to be less “adaptive” in coping with stress [79, 80]. In fact, rumination itself has been asso-
ciated with diverse psychological disturbances and predicted the later development of
depression [81]. In this way, our findings are consistent with recent research in which people
with greater religiosity were more likely to “emotionally overreact” and engage in negative
coping behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic [82].
While some religious individuals may have engaged in more positive emotional-engage-
ment coping strategies, such as seeking social support, others may have relied on less construc-
tive emotional strategies to deal with the pandemic. Indeed, greater religiosity has been
associated with some unreasonable behavioural coping responses to deal with the COVID-19
pandemic (e.g., hoarding toilet paper), which was also linked with increased emotionality [82].
Moreover, the religious “Minorities” group included participants who identified as belonging
to diverse religions, thus differences in religious beliefs as well as differences in the interpreta-
tions and practice of those beliefs (e.g., communal worship), may influence the use of more
positive or negative emotional-engagement coping strategies. For example, individuals who
identified as belonging to religions that observe daily and weekly times of worship (i.e., Jewish
and Muslim), may be more likely to benefit from consistent social support-seeking compared
to those who identified as belonging to religions that observe less frequent and structured
times of communal worship (i.e., Bahá’ı́, Buddhist, Hindu, and Sikh). Indeed, greater religious
participation has long been associated with increased social support [83–85]. Nonetheless,
despite higher levels of religiosity being accompanied by greater emotional-engagement cop-
ing, which was in turn associated with more negative mental and physical health outcomes,
religiosity was significantly associated with more positive mental health. Thus, rather than act-
ing as a direct mediator, emotional-engagement coping appeared to have a suppressor effect
[63, 86] in the (positive) relation between religiosity and mental health, such that this relation
might have been even stronger (and the relation between religiosity and physical health might
have been significant) had it not been for the emotion-focused coping in which religious indi-
viduals were more likely to engage.
individuals is that seemingly random events can hold purpose or meaning [33, 67] and are in
control by a more powerful transcendent force [88], the benefits of religiosity may also come
from the adoption of specific appraisals of stressors (e.g., viewing events as challenges rather
than threats) and endorsing effective problem-focused coping methods. In the same vein, cog-
nitive restructuring (i.e., reframing one’s interpretation of a situation) may include an element
of religion or spirituality, such as prayer and/or mediation, thereby harnessing the benefits typ-
ically inherent to both active problem-focused and religious coping together.
Despite the use of problem-focused coping, the present study also revealed the tendency for
religious individuals to use emotional-engagement coping, which may be associated with
more negative health outcomes. Thus, it may also be prudent for public health messages to
caution against the use of certain emotional-engagement coping strategies, such as rumina-
tion, denial, or disengagement. While such messaging would be relevant for both religious and
non-religious individuals alike, it may be especially pertinent for religious communities in
which it would be advantageous to help steer them away from negative religious coping strate-
gies (e.g., anger at God/gods), which have been associated with more negative health outcomes
[89]. As religious individuals tend to have formal support networks through their religious
organizations and/or communities [75], promotion of this unique resource may help them to
cope in more effective ways with both emotional and tangible outcomes.
Non-religious individuals who lack religious communities may likewise benefit from
encouragement to engage in social support-seeking and emotional expression with other posi-
tive communities in which they may be involved [9]. Indeed, even without considering reli-
gious identity, greater social connectedness during pandemic lockdown periods was associated
with lower levels of perceived stress, worry, burnout, and fatigue [90, 91]. Moreover, as better
mental health was associated with the use of challenge appraisals and problem-focused coping
when our study sample was examined as a whole (i.e., including both religious and non-reli-
gious individuals), the benefits of engaging in such strategies likely extend within and beyond
religious communities. Indeed, given the broad distress created by the COVID-19 pandemic,
it has been noted that the pandemic may be thought of as a collective trauma [92–94]. As such,
appraising the pandemic as a challenge (rather than a threat) may be associated with post-trau-
matic growth (i.e., significant positive change arising from major life struggles; [95–97]).
Given the potentially long-standing mental health implications of the pandemic, it is impor-
tant that accessible, tailored, and culturally-sensitive public health initiatives adequately
address the needs of people with diverse backgrounds and beliefs.
Limitations
Like most research, this study has several limitations. First, the religious “Minority” group
comprised members from several different religions due to the relatively small number of
respondents who identified as belonging to one of a variety of religious groups considered to
be “Minorities” in Canada. Thus, there may well be important differences (e.g., individualistic
vs. collectivist attitudes or practices; [87]) between these religious groups that were not
detected in the current study. Nonetheless, our inclusion of these broad and often under-rep-
resented religious groups extends previous research that has in many cases considered Chris-
tians alone. Indeed, despite having the least representation within our sample (15.7%),
religious minorities were nonetheless over-represented compared to the national average of
these groups combined (10.4%; [98]). Likewise, Atheists, Agnostics, and “Spirituals” were
over-represented in the current study, perhaps due to the use of MTurk as a data collection
tool (which often elicits younger, more highly-educated participants; [99]), thereby offering a
valuable opportunity with our screening and validity checks in place; [100] to compare
responses across these growing (non)religious groups. Relatedly, as a result of the sample sizes
across (non)religious groups, we were unable to adequately assess interactions with gender.
Given previous research suggesting that women tend to report poorer mental health (e.g.,
[101]) and greater religiosity (e.g., [102]) compared to men, as with most research, future stud-
ies would benefit from a larger sample of participants. Nonetheless, our patterns of findings
held constant even when gender (along with age, race, education, and income) was included as
a covariate.
Additionally, mental and physical health were each measured using a single, self-reported
item (notwithstanding the documented predictive value of these single-item measures; [103]).
Likewise, (non)religious group identity and affiliation was based on a single question as part of
the demographic questionnaire, but without a related measure to evaluate the strength of this
non(religious) identity. Despite this, our study did include a more precise measure of religios-
ity, which seemed to capture the relative strength of the identity across the groups (perhaps
with the exception of highly-identified Atheists; [104]). A final limitation of this study is the
inability to compare our results to those from a pre-pandemic context, which may have pro-
vided insight into specific changes in the relations among religiosity, stressor appraisals, cop-
ing, and health outcomes that may have occurred as a result of the pandemic. Although our
study yielded potentially important patterns among religiosity, appraisal-coping processes and
health, given the correlational nature of the data, causal relationships cannot be definitively
determined.
Conclusions
The results of the present study revealed numerous differences in appraisal-coping methods
and health across (non)religious groups. However, the clearest patterns emerged between reli-
giosity and health, where both problem-focused and emotional-engagement coping mediated
these relations. These findings suggest that greater religious belief may contribute to better
mental and physical health, at least in part, through using adaptive (non-religious) coping
strategies. Accordingly, this research may have implications for coping at the individual level,
as well as informing culturally-sensitive public health messages promoting targeted self-care
recommendations with integrated religious or spiritual elements during times of threat and
uncertainty, such as the COVID-19 pandemic or future health crises.
Supporting information
S1 Checklist. STROBE statement—checklist of items that should be included in reports of
observational studies.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Renate Ysseldyk.
Data curation: Claire Peneycad, Emily Tippins.
Formal analysis: Claire Peneycad, Emily Tippins.
Funding acquisition: Renate Ysseldyk.
Investigation: Claire Peneycad, Emily Tippins.
Methodology: Claire Peneycad, Emily Tippins.
Project administration: Emily Tippins.
References
1. World Health Organization | WHO. WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard [Internet]. [cited 2022
Jan 10]. Available from: https://covid19.who.int/.
2. Dozois DJA, Mental Health Research Canada. (2021). Anxiety and depression in Canada during the
COVID-19 pandemic: A national survey. Can Psychol, 2021; 62(1):136–142.
3. Park CL, Finkelstein-Fox L, Russell BS, Fendrich M, Hutchison M, Becker J. Americans’ distress early
in the COVID-19 pandemic: Protective resources and coping strategies. Psychol Trauma. 2021; 13(4):
422–431. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000931 PMID: 33507795
4. Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, Xu Y. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among
Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommendations. Gen Psychiatr.
2020; 33(2):e100213. https://doi.org/10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213 PMID: 32215365
5. Budimir S, Probst T, Pieh C. Coping strategies and mental health during COVID-19 lockdown. J Ment
Health. 2021; 30(2):156–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638237.2021.1875412 PMID: 33502917
6. Chow SK, Francis B, Ng YH, Naim N, Beh HC, Ariffin MAA, et al. Religious coping, depression and
anxiety among healthcare workers during the COVID-19 pandemic: A Malaysian perspective. Healthc
(Amst). 2021; 9(1):79. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare9010079 PMID: 33467744
7. Park CL, Russell BS, Fendrich M, Finkelstein-Fox L, Hutchison M, Becker J. Americans’ COVID-19
stress, coping, and adherence to CDC guidelines. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2020; 35(8):2296–2303.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-020-05898-9 PMID: 32472486
8. Draru MC, Lopatofsky T, Karimi K, Gautier ML. Understanding coping mechanisms during the COVID-
19 pandemic: A case study of stakeholders of the African Sisters Education Collaborative. Rev. Relig.
Res. 2022; 64(2):325–342. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13644-022-00488-z PMID: 35431345
9. Haslam SA, Jetten J, Postmes T, Haslam C. Social identity, health and well-being: An emerging
agenda for applied psychology. Appl. Psychol. Int. Rev. 2009; 58(1):1–23.
10. Ysseldyk R, Talebi M, Matheson K, Bloemraad I, Anisman H. Religious and ethnic discrimination: Dif-
ferential implications for social support engagement, civic involvement, and political consciousness. J.
Soc. Political. Psychol. 2014; 2:347–376.
11. Folkman S. Stress: Appraisal and coping. 2013. p. 1913–1915.
12. Fatima S, Arshad M, Mushtaq M. Religious coping and young adult’s mental well-being during Covid-
19: Testing a double moderated mediation model. Arch Psychol Relig. 2022;1–20.
13. Mahamid FA, Bdier D. The association between positive religious coping, perceived stress, and
depressive symptoms during the spread of coronavirus (COVID-19) among a sample of adults in Pal-
estine: A cross-sectional study. J Relig Health. 2021; 60(1):34–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-
020-01121-5 PMID: 33389439
14. Pargament KI, Smith BW, Koenig HG, Perez L. Patterns of positive and negative religious coping with
major life stressors. J Sci Study Relig. 1998; 37(4):710–724.
15. Park CL, Holt CL, Le D, Christie J, Williams BR. Positive and negative religious coping styles as pro-
spective predictors of well-being in African Americans. Psychol Relig Spiritual. 2018; 10(4):318–326.
https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000124 PMID: 30505376
16. Finkelstein-Fox L, Park CL. Control-coping goodness-of-fit and chronic illness: A systematic review of
the literature. Health Psychol Rev. 2019; 13(2):137–162. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.
1560229 PMID: 30558475
17. Rafferty KA, Billig AK, Mosack KE. Spirituality, religion, and health: The role of communication,
appraisals, and coping for individuals living with chronic illness. J Relig Health. 2015; 54(5), 1870–
1885. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-014-9965-5 PMID: 25341570
18. Taha S, Matheson K, Cronin T, Anisman H. Intolerance of uncertainty, appraisals, coping, and anxiety:
The case of the 2009 H1N1 pandemic. Br J Health Psychol. 2014; 19(3):592–605. https://doi.org/10.
1111/bjhp.12058 PMID: 23834735
19. Bergman YS, Cohen-Fridel S, Shrira A, Bodner E, Palgi Y. COVID-19 health worries and anxiety
symptoms among older adults: The moderating role of ageism. Int Psychogeriatr. 2020; 32(11):1371–
1375. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610220001258 PMID: 32613924
20. Yip P, Cheung YT, Chau PH, Law YW. The impact of epidemic outbreak. J Crisis Interv Suicide Prev.
2010; 31(2).
21. Kharroubi S, Saleh F. Are lockdown measures effective against COVID-19? Front Public Health.
2020; 8:549692. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2020.549692 PMID: 33194950
22. Sprang G, Silman M. Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents and youth after health-related disasters.
Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2013; 7(1):105–110. https://doi.org/10.1017/dmp.2013.22 PMID:
24618142
23. Bankier-Karp AL, Shain M. COVID-19’s effects upon the religious group resources, psychosocial
resources, and mental health of Orthodox Jews. J Sci Study Relig. 2022; 61(1):197–216.
24. Janssen F, Bänziger S, Dezutter J, Hutsebaut D. Religion and mental health: Aspects of the relation
between religious measures and positive and negative mental health. Arch Psychol Relig. 2005; 27
(1):19–44.
25. Litalien M, Atari DO, Obasi I. The influence of religiosity and spirituality on health in Canada: A system-
atic literature review. J Relig Health. 2022; 61(1):373–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-020-01148-
8 PMID: 33409859
26. Schnabel L, Schieman S. Religion protected mental health but constrained crisis response during cru-
cial early days of the COVID-19 pandemic. J Sci Study Relig. 2022; 61(2):530–543. https://doi.org/10.
1111/jssr.12720 PMID: 34230686
27. Shiah YJ, Chang F, Chiang SK, Lin IM, Tam WCC. Religion and health: Anxiety, religiosity, meaning of
life and mental health. J Relig Health. 2015; 54: 35–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-013-9781-3
PMID: 24132457
28. Magin ZE, David AB, Carney LM, Park CL, Gutierrez IA, George LS. Belief in God and psychological
distress: Is it the belief or certainty of the belief? Relig. 2021; 12(9): 757.
29. Park CL. Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its
effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol Bull. 2010; 136(2):257–301. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0018301 PMID: 20192563
30. Héliot Y, Gleibs IH, Coyle A, Rousseau DM, Rojon C. Religious identity in the workplace: A systematic
review, research agenda, and practical implications. Hum Resour Manag J. 2020; 59(2):153–173.
31. Mavor K, Ysseldyk R. A social identity approach to religion: Religiosity at the nexus of personal and
collective self. In: Vail K, Routledge C, editors. The Science of Religion, Spirituality, and Existentialism.
Elsevier Academic Press; 2020. p. 187–206.
32. Muldoon OT, Haslam SA, Haslam C, Cruwys T, Kearns M, Jetten J. The social psychology of
responses to trauma: Social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses. Eur
Rev Soc Psychol. 2019; 30(1):311–348.
33. Park CL. Religiousness/spirituality and health: A meaning systems perspective. J Behav Med. 2007;
30(4):319–328. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-007-9111-x PMID: 17522971
34. Ysseldyk R, Matheson K, Anisman H. Religiosity as identity: Toward an understanding of religion from
a social identity perspective. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2010; 14:60–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1088868309349693 PMID: 20089847
35. McCullough ME, Willoughby BLB. Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explana-
tions, and implications. Psychol Bull. 2009; 135(1):69–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014213 PMID:
19210054
36. Hackney CH, Sanders GS. Religiosity and mental health: A meta-analysis of recent studies. J Sci
Study Relig. 2003; 42(1):43–55.
37. Balbuena L, Baetz M, Bowen R. Religious attendance, spirituality, and major depression in Canada: A
14-year follow-up study. Can J Psychiatry. 2013; 58(4): 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1177/
070674371305800408 PMID: 23547646
38. Dilmaghani M. Importance of religion or spirituality and mental health in Canada. J Relig Health. 2018;
57(1):120–135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-017-0385-1 PMID: 28315988
39. Merath K, Kelly PE, Hyer JM, Mehta R, Agne JL, Deans K, et al. Patient perceptions about the role of
religion and spirituality during cancer care. J Relig Health. 2020; 59(4):1933–1945. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10943-019-00907-6 PMID: 31482443
40. Peres MFP, Kamei HH, Tobo PR, Lucchetti G. Mechanisms behind religiosity and spirituality’s effect
on mental health, quality of life and well-being. J Relig Health. 2018; 57(5):1842–1855. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s10943-017-0400-6 PMID: 28444608
41. Ysseldyk R, Matheson K, Anisman H. Coping with identity threat: The role of religious orientation and
implications for emotions and action intentions. Psychol Relig Spiritual. 2011; 3(2):132–148.
42. Burker EJ, Evon DM, Sedway AJ, Egan T. Religious and non-religious coping in lung transplant candi-
dates: Does adding god to the picture tell us more? J Behav Med. 2005; 28(6):513–526. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10865-005-9025-4 PMID: 16237611
43. Pargament KI. The Psychology of Religion and Coping: Theory, Research, Practice. Guilford
Press; 1997.
44. Spilka B, Ladd KL. The psychology of prayer: A scientific approach. Guilford Press; 2012.
45. Exline JJ, Park CL, Smyth JM, Carey MP. Anger toward God: Social-cognitive predictors, prevalence,
and links with adjustment to bereavement and cancer. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011; 100(1):129–148.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021716 PMID: 21219078
46. Ano GG, Vasconcelles EB. Religious coping and psychological adjustment to stress: A meta-analysis.
J Clin Psychol. 2005; 61(4):461–480. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20049 PMID: 15503316
47. Koenig HG. Religion and mental health: Research and clinical applications. London: Academic
Press. 2018.
48. Maltby J, Day L. Religious orientation, religious coping and appraisals of stress: Assessing primary
appraisal factors in the relationship between religiosity and psychological well-being. Pers Individ Dif.
2003; 34(7):1209–1224.
49. Park CL. Exploring relations among religiousness, meaning, and adjustment to lifetime and current
stressful encounters in later life. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2006; 19(1):33–45.
50. Bentzen JS. In crisis we pray: Religiosity and the COVID-19 pandemic. Centre for Economic Policy
Research, COVID Economics 20. 2020. 14824.
51. Pirutinsky S, Cherniak AD, Rosmarin DH. COVID-19, mental health, and religious coping among
American orthodox Jews. J Relig Health. 2020; 59(5):2288–2301. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-
020-01070-z PMID: 32705481
52. Agadjanian A, Siekierski K. Religion and pandemic: Shifts of interpretation, popular lore, and practices.
An introduction. Entangled Relig. 2022; 12(3).
53. Baker JO, Martı́ G, Braunstein R, Whitehead AL, Yukich G. Religion in the age of social distancing:
How COVID-19 presents new directions for research. Sociol Relig. 2020; 81(4):357–370. https://doi.
org/10.1093/socrel/sraa039 PMID: 34191911
54. Upenieks L. Religious/spiritual struggles and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: Does “talking
religion” help or hurt? Rev Relig Res. 2022; 64(2):249–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13644-022-00487-
0 PMID: 35431346
55. Mathieu E, Ritchie H, Rodés-Guirao L, Appel C, Giattino C, Hasell J, et al. “Coronavirus Pandemic
(COVID-19).” OurWorldInData.org, 2020, https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus
56. Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A, Lang GA. (2009) Statistical power analyses using G-power 3.1: Tests
for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods 41: 1149–1160. https://doi.org/
10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 PMID: 19897823
57. Huber S, Huber OW. The centrality of religiosity scale (CRS). Religions. 2012; 3(3):710–724.
58. Peacock EJ, Wong PP. The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cogni-
tive appraisal. Stress Med. 1990; 6(3):227–236.
59. Matheson K, Anisman H. Systems of Coping Associated with Dysphoria, Anxiety and Depressive Ill-
ness: A Multivariate Profile Perspective, Stress. 2003; 6:3:223–234.
60. Ahmad F, Jhaji AK, Stewart DE, Burghardt M, Bierman AS. Single item measures of self-rated mental
health: A scoping review. BMC Health Serv Res. 2014; 14(1): 398. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6963-
14-398 PMID: 25231576
61. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. J
Health Soc Behav. 1997; 38(1):21–37. PMID: 9097506
62. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. Guilford Publica-
tions; 2022.
63. Agler R, De Boeck P. On the interpretation and use of mediation: Multiple perspectives on mediation
analysis. Front Psychol. 2017;8.
64. Hoogeveen S, Sarafoglou A, Aczel B, Aditya Y, Alayan AJ, Allen PJ, et al. A many-analysts approach
to the relation between religiosity and well-being. Relig Brain Behav. 2022;1–47.
65. Anisman H, Merali Z. Understanding stress: Characteristics and caveats. Alcohol Research & Health:
Alcohol Res Health. 1999; 23(4):241–249.
66. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kelsey RM, Leitten CL. Subjective, physiological, and behavioural effects of
threat and challenge appraisal. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993; 65(2):248–260.
67. Wang Y, Jackson T, Cai L. Causal effects of threat and challenge appraisals on coping and pain per-
ception. Eur J Pain. 2016; 20(7):1111–1120. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.835 PMID: 26773726
68. Koenig HG. Religion, spirituality, and health: The research and clinical implications. ISRN Psychiatry.
2012; 278730:1–33. https://doi.org/10.5402/2012/278730 PMID: 23762764
69. Mueller PS, Plevak DJ, Rummans TA. Religious involvement, spirituality, and medicine: Implications
for clinical practice. Mayo Clin Proc. 2001; 76(12):1225–1235. https://doi.org/10.4065/76.12.1225
PMID: 11761504
70. Ehring T, Quack D. Emotion regulation difficulties in trauma survivors: The role of trauma type and
PTSD symptom severity. Behav Ther. 2010; 41(4):587–598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.
004 PMID: 21035621
71. Gotmann A, Bechtoldt MN. Coping with COVID-19 –Longitudinal analysis of coping strategies and the
role of trait mindfulness in mental well-being. Pers Individ Dif. 2021; 175:110695 https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.paid.2021.110695 PMID: 33531724
72. Krägeloh CU, Chai PPM, Shepherd D, Billington R. How religious coping is used relative to other cop-
ing strategies depends on the individual’s level of religiosity and spirituality. J Relig Health. 2012; 51
(4):1137–1151. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-010-9416-x PMID: 21042859
73. Molen KV, Fogarty K, Mcgrady M, Lagerwey M. Religious problem-solving styles and self-efficacy with
problem-focused coping in a faith-based poverty alleviation programme. Ment Health Relig Cult. 2020;
23(10):888–901.
74. Chao RCL. Managing stress and maintaining well-being: Social support, problem-focused coping, and
avoidant coping. J Couns Dev. 2011; 89(3): 338–348.
75. Kühle L, Larsen TL. ‘Forced’ online religion: Religious minority and majority communities’ media usage
during the COVID-19 lockdown. Religions. 2021; 12(7):496.
76. Shakespeare-Finch J, Obst PL. The development of the 2-way social support scale: A measure of giv-
ing and receiving emotional and instrumental support. J Pers Assess. 2011; 93(5):483–490. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.594124 PMID: 21859288
77. Carver CS. Active Coping; 2014. p. 16–19.
78. Jacobi CJ, Vaidyanathan B, Andronicou M. Mental health correlates of sharing private problems in
congregations during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Sci Study Relig. 2022; 61(2):553–563.
79. Fariddanesh M, Rezaei AM. Predicting the happiness of adolescents based on coping styles and reli-
gious attitudes. J Relig Health. 2019; 58(2):537–553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-017-0497-7
PMID: 29022238
80. Hong RY. Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and
coping behavior. Behav Res Ther. 2007; 45(2):277–290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.006
PMID: 16635479
81. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008; 3
(5):400–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x PMID: 26158958
82. Kranz D, Niepel C, Botes E, Greiff S. Religiosity predicts unreasonable coping with COVID-19. Psychol
Relig Spiritual. 2020; 15(1).
83. Ellison CG, George LK. Religious involvement, social ties, and social support in a Southeastern com-
munity. JSSR, 1994; 33(1):46–61.
84. Hastings OP. Not a lonely crowd? Social connectedness, religious service attendance, and the spiri-
tual but not religious. Soc Sci Res. 2016;(57):63–79.
85. Bradley CS, Hill TD, Burdette AM, Mossakowski KN, Johnson RJ. Religious attendance and social
support: Integration or selection? Rev Relig Res. 2020; 62(1):83–99.
86. MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression
effect. Prev Sci. 2000; 1(4):173 https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1026595011371 PMID: 11523746
87. Saroglou V, Clobert M, Cohen AB, Johnson KA, Ladd KL, Van Pachterbeke M, et al. Believing, bond-
ing, behaving, and belonging: The cognitive, emotional, moral, and social dimensions of religiousness
across cultures. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2020; 51(7–8):551–575.
88. Kay AC, Gaucher D, McGregor I, Nash K. Religious belief as compensatory control. Pers Soc Psychol
Rev. 2020; 14(1):37–48.
89. Grubbs JB, Exline JJ, Campbell WK. I deserve better and god knows it! Psychological entitlement as a
robust predictor of anger at God. Psychol Relig Spiritual. 2013; 5(3):192–200.
90. Nitschke JP, Forbes PAG, Ali N, Cutler J, Apps MAJ, Lockwood PL, et al. Resilience during uncer-
tainty? Greater social connectedness during COVID-19 lockdown is associated with reduced distress
and fatigue. Br J Health Psychol. 2021; 26(2):553–569. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12485 PMID:
33099800
91. Yıldırım M, Çiçek İ, Şanlı ME. Coronavirus stress and COVID-19 burnout among healthcare staffs:
The mediating role of optimism and social connectedness. Curr Psychol. 2021; 40(11):5763–5771.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01781-w PMID: 33935470
92. Muldoon OT. Collective trauma. In Jetten J, Reicher SD, Haslam SA, Cruwys T, editors. Together
Apart: The Psychology of COVID-19. SAGE Publications; 2020. p. 69–72.
93. Stanley BL, Zanin AC, Avalos BL, Tracy SJ, Town S. Collective emotion during collective trauma: A
metaphor analysis of the COVID-19 pandemic. Qual Health Res. 2021; 31(10):1890–1903. https://doi.
org/10.1177/10497323211011589 PMID: 33980096
94. Watson MF, Bacigalupe G, Daneshpour M, Han W, Parra-Cardona R. COVID-19 interconnectedness:
Health inequity, the climate crisis, and collective trauma. Fam Process. 2020; 59(3):832–846. https://
doi.org/10.1111/famp.12572 PMID: 32589267
95. Finstad GL, Giorgi G, Lulli LG, Pandolfi C, Foti G, León-Perez JM, et al. Resilience, coping strategies
and posttraumatic growth in the workplace following COVID-19: A narrative review on the positive
aspects of trauma. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021; 18(18):9453. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph18189453 PMID: 34574378
96. Waters L, Cameron K, Nelson-Coffey SK, Crone DL, Kern ML, Lomas T, et al. Collective wellbeing
and posttraumatic growth during COVID-19: How positive psychology can help families, schools, work-
places and marginalized communities. J Posit Psychol. 2021; 1–29.
97. Yan S, Yang J, Ye M, Chen S, Xie C, Huang J, et al. Post-traumatic growth and related influencing fac-
tors in discharged COVID-19 patients: A cross-sectional study. Health Psychol. 2021;12. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.658307 PMID: 34122242
98. Statistics Canada. Religion in Canada. October 28, 2021. Available from: https://www150.statcan.gc.
ca/n1/pub/11-627-m/11-627-m2021079-eng.htm
99. Castille CM, Avet S, Daigle K. Comparing MTurk and the US population’s occupational diversity. PLoS
One. 2019; 15(5):e0216512.
100. Chmielewski M, Kucker SC. An MTurk crisis? Shifts in data quality and the impact on study results.
Social Psychol Pers Sci. 2020; 11(4):464–473.
101. Salk RH, Hyde JS, Abramson LY. Gender differences in representative national samples: Meta-analy-
ses of diagnoses symptoms. Psychol Bull. 2017; 143(8):783–822.
102. Baker JO, Whitehead AL. Gendering (non)religion: Politics, education, and gender gaps in secularity
in the United States. Soc Forces. 2016; 94(4):1623–1645.
103. Idler EL. Religion and aging in the global context of secularization: Patterns, processes, conse-
quences. In Ferraro KF, Carr D, editors. Handbook of aging and the social sciences. 9th ed. Aca-
demic Press; 2021. p. 55–268.
104. Mackey CD, Silver CF, Rios K, Cowgill CM, Hood RW. Concealment of nonreligious identity: Exploring
social identity threat among atheists and other nonreligious individuals. Group Process Intergroup
Relat. 2021; 24(5):860–877.