Out
Out
A dissertation submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in
partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the
Department of Allied Health Sciences, Division of Speech and Hearing Sciences.
Chapel Hill
2006
Approved By
Copyright 2006 by
Roy, Vicky Poston
ii
ABSTRACT
VICKY POSTON ROY: Storybook Reading and the Addition of Toy Props:
The Effects on Child Engagement and Interest and the Interactions Between
Using a repeated-measures design, this study examined the addition of toy props during
storybook reading between caregivers and their young children 18-27 months of age. Twelve
demonstrating “low engagement” during storybook interactions and their caregivers were
child dyads were video-recorded on two occasions at their home reading two different
storybooks and engaging in a “favorite” activity. Toy props were included during one of the
The results of the study revealed significant group differences between prop
conditions for the total interaction time and the rate of child protesting behaviors.
Additionally a trend was identified between dyads in the high and low engagement groups
for both the total number of child verbal/vocal acts and total child gesture use. Specifically
there was a differential influence for the addition of toy props dependent on engagement
group membership with children in the low engagement group producing more verbal/vocal
iii
acts and gestures during the prop condition than the no prop condition. In contrast, children
in the high engagement group tended to produce few verbal/vocal acts and gestures during
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Many years ago, I left home hoping to find who I was meant to be. After a few years
and one degree, I set off on another journey still not quite sure what it was that God had
planned for me. At the end of that road and with another degree to boot, I once again
embarked on another educational journey, this time dragging my sweet husband-to-be along
for the ride. Many life experiences and unforeseen circumstances have brought me to the
place where I am now, but it has been the people who I have found, and in some cases those
who have found me, that I must thank for making me into the person I was always meant to
be.
I must first thank God for sending me the people in my life that have made this
journey possible. He has lovingly watched over me through the wonderful experiences and
To the many families who participated in my dissertation, I am truly grateful for their
time. Without each of these families, this study could not have come to fruition. I must also
thank the families I have worked with over the last four years – these families have solidified
for me my purpose in life and helped me to embrace a calling that I might not have heard if
To my dissertation advisor and mentor, Elizabeth Crais, thank you for your support
and guidance, for believing in me, and for allowing me the freedom and independence to do
what I needed to do to stay sane over the last few years. Your impeccable way with words
v
and ability to see the positive in all situations helped me to stay focused and to motivate me
in the end when at times it seemed like this journey would never end.
To Karen Erickson, thank you for always going above and beyond the call of duty.
You are amazing and I am honored to have worked with you. Thank you for always
introducing me as your colleague, for acknowledging everything that I took on over the last
four years, and for supporting me even when it wasn’t in your own best interest.
Vernon-Feagans, and Linda Watson, thank you for not allowing me to bite off more than I
could chew. This has been an enlightening experience and I hope to pay it forward in return.
To my fellow PhD colleagues Sally and Cara, thank you for providing Devin and me
with a family while we were so far away from our own. And to the boys, James and Chris,
To my friend and colleague Sonja who has listened, shared and most importantly
understood my challenges and triumphs like only another doc student could. Thank you for
listening and supporting, for proof reading and for editing, for picking me up from the
airport, and for promising to always room with me at ASHA! Thank you for being such a
great friend. I wish you many publications, lots of grant money, and most of all sanity in a
purposeful means to procrastinate. It has been a pleasure meeting, working, and learning
from each of you. Thanks for supporting and attempting to understand me. I am a better
vi
To my sisters, my mom and dad, my mother-in-laws, my father-in-law, my brothers,
the KOC, and the many wonderful unnamed friends and family who have supported, loved
Finally, and most importantly, to my sweet husband Devin. Six and half years ago the
Lord sent me an angel. Who would have ever thought that two people who were completely
not looking for each other could be so perfect for one another? Thank you for the unwanted
“pep talks” and the speeches about perseverance, determination, and good old hard work, for
procrastinate. Thank you for following me without asking any questions and for believing in
me when I wanted to sleep it all away. Thank you for pushing me even when I didn’t want to
be pushed. This would have never happened without your love and support. You have made
many sacrifices, both small and large, and I am forever grateful. Thank you. I love you very
much.
VPR
vii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Contents Page
ABSTRACT............................................................................................................................. iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS...................................................................................................... v
CHAPTER 1 ............................................................................................................................. 1
CHAPTER 2 ............................................................................................................................. 5
viii
Child Contributions to Storybook Reading......................................................................34
Potential Impact of Low Interest and Engagement during Storybook Reading. ........ 46
Research Questions............................................................................................................. 51
CHAPTER 3 ........................................................................................................................... 54
METHODS ............................................................................................................................. 54
Participants.......................................................................................................................54
Child Participants........................................................................................................ 55
Procedures........................................................................................................................60
Recruitment................................................................................................................. 60
Caregiver Questionnaires............................................................................................ 65
Language Assessment................................................................................................. 68
Storybooks .................................................................................................................. 69
ix
Inter-rater Reliability .......................................................................................................75
CHAPTER IV ......................................................................................................................... 79
RESULTS ............................................................................................................................... 79
Gender......................................................................................................................... 79
Book Influence............................................................................................................ 80
Book Order.................................................................................................................. 81
Prop Order................................................................................................................... 82
Since............................................................................................................................ 84
x
Total Interaction Time. ............................................................................................. 112
Study Limitations...........................................................................................................127
xi
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
3.1 Individual Scores for Four Children Demonstrating Delays on the ASQ ....................... 57
3.2 Matching Criteria for High and Low Engagement Groups ............................................. 59
4.1 Means and Standard Deviations for Average Subjective Rating of Engagement ............ 84
4.2 Means and Standard Deviations for Rate of Communicative Acts per Minute................ 84
4.3 Means and Standard deviations for the Average Subjective Rating of Engagement
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group. .................................85
4.4 Means and Standard Deviations for the Overall Rate of Communicative Acts during
the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group. ............................................87
4.7 Means and Standard Deviations for Total Child Communicative Acts During the Prop
and No Prop Book Conditions ................................................................................................ 95
4.8 Means and Standard Deviations for Child Verbal/Vocal Acts ......................................... 96
4.9 Means and Standard Deviations for Child Gesture Use ................................................... 97
4.10 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Verbal/Vocal Acts of the High and Low
Engagement Group During Their First and Second Book Reading Interaction. .................... 98
4.11 Means and Standard deviations for the Total Gesture Use During the Prop and No
Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group and Prop Order Presentation. .........................99
4.12 Means and Standard deviations for Overall Rate of Child Communicative Acts
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions. ....................................................................100
4.13 Means and Standard Deviations for Rate of Child Responses by Group and
Condition................................................................................................................................101
xii
4.14 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Child Protesting Behaviors during the
Prop and No Prop Book Conditions...................................................................................... 103
4.15 Means and Standard deviations for the Total Protesting Behaviors During the Prop
and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group and Prop Order Presentation. ..........104
4.16 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Interaction Time (in seconds) during
the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions.................................................................................105
4.17 Means and Standard Deviations for the Ratio of Caregiver Turns to Child Turns
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions. ....................................................................106
4.18 Means and Standard Deviations for Total Caregiver Turns During the Prop and No
Prop Book Conditions........................................................................................................... 107
4.19 Means and Standard Deviations for Average Subjective Ratings of Engagement
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions .....................................................................108
4.20 Means and Standard Deviations for the Average Subjective Rating of Engagement
of the High and Low Engagement Groups Based on Their First and Second Reading
Interaction ..............................................................................................................................110
4.21 Average Subjective Rating of Engagement Means and Standard Deviations for the
Low Engagement Group Only During the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Prop
Order Presentation. ................................................................................................................110
xiii
CHAPTER 1
Storybook interactions between caregivers and their children have been the focus
of an extensive body of research over the last three decades (Dickinson & McCabe, 1991;
Pellegrini, Brody, & Sigel, 1985; Whitehurst et al., 1994; Whitehurst et al., 1999) (See
Bus, van IJzendoorn, & Pellegrini, 1995; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998 for detailed
reviews). Research has documented the concurrent and predictive relationship between
aspects of storybook reading, and the language and literacy outcomes of young children
(Bus et al., 1995; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Moerk, 1985; Pellegrini et al., 1985).
children who are experiencing difficulties with language and literacy development (Snow
Reading as the “single most important activity for building the knowledge required for
1985 p.23). This statement has been supported by research documenting a positive
relationship between early storybook reading and later language and literacy development
(Bus et al., 1995; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Moerk, 1985; Pellegrini et al., 1985).
However, the Commission’s claim has received great attention in the literature (See
Allen, Cipielewski, & Stanovich, 1992; Bus et al., 1995; Cipielewski & Stanovich, 1992;
Senechal, Thomas, & Monker, 1995; Yarosz & Barnett, 2001) in part due to the
frequently cited review conducted by Scarborough and Dobrich (1994) indicating that
only a small proportion of the variance in later language and literacy skills can be
accounted for by the frequency with which children engage in storybook reading with a
In response to the Scarborough and Dobrich (1994), Bus and colleagues (Bus et
al., 1995) used available literature to conduct a meta-analysis that allowed for control of
sample size across previous studies. The results of the meta-analysis were similar in that
early storybook reading accounted for only a small proportion of the variance in later
language and literacy skills; however, the analyses also revealed a moderate effect size,
which Bus et al. concluded provided clear support for storybook reading as an important
contributing factor to later language and literacy development. Lonigan (1994) also
suggested that these initial small differences may be magnified over time and in turn may
Further debate has highlighted factors other than frequency of storybook reading
that may influence later literacy and language outcomes. These factors include the types
of interactions between caregivers and their children, the home literacy environment, and
the child’s interest and engagement in storybook reading activities (Bus et al., 1995;
DeBaryshe, 1995b). Despite the debate in the literature regarding the importance of the
frequency of storybook reading, most researchers generally accept the potential benefits
associated with shared storybook reading and the identified relationships between
storybook reading and later language and literacy development (Snow et al., 1998; Teale
& Sulzby, 1986). What remains unclear is the exact contribution of the varying
2
components of storybook reading that factor into the success of these types of caregiver-
child interactions and their relationship with later language and literacy outcomes.
later child development has examined varying factors of shared book reading. Such
factors as the frequency of home literacy practices and the quality of the interactions
between caregivers and their young children have been investigated in both the emergent
literacy field and the more general field of speech-language pathology (Bus et al., 1995;
Justice & Kaderavek, 2002; Lonigan, 1994; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994; Schuele &
van Kleeck, 1987; Snow et al., 1998). In contrast to the abundance of research available
examining the relationships between these related factors and their contributions to the
benefits related to storybook reading, another key factor, the child’s interest and
engagement in storybook reading, has received little attention in the literature to date.
by, storybook reading (Lonigan, 1994; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994; Snow et al., 1998;
Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002). Not only may increased child interest and
Schneider & Hecht, 1995), but also children who are interested and engaged are more
likely (than those who are not) to read independently for pleasure once they learn to read
(Baker, 2003; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). Furthermore, children who demonstrate early
interest and engagement in storybook reading and other literacy related activities are
3
throughout the early school years compared to their low-interest peers (Frijters, Barron,
& Brunello, 2000; Guthrie & Knowles, 2001; Olofsson & Niedersoe, 1999).
Unfortunately, many of the children who might benefit the most from the
potential positive influences of storybook reading (e.g. children with language and
literacy difficulties) have been described as less interested and engaged in this type of
interaction (Kaderavek & Sulzby, 1998a, 1998b; Morrow, 1983). Despite the growing
support for child engagement as a critical factor in the success of storybook reading
interactions, there are few studies that have attempted to better understand the
components that contribute to increases (or decreases) in child engagement and interest in
storybook reading. One potential strategy recommended in the clinical literature for
increasing child interest and engagement during storybook reading is the use of toy props
(Musselwhite & King-DeBaun, 1997). In spite of this unique and creative suggestion,
however, researchers have not examined the use of toy props and the impact on child
The current study aims to examine the influence of toy props during storybook
reading and their relationship with child engagement. Based on the notable contributions
children and its potential to greatly impact the development of language and literacy
skills of children experiencing difficulties with these areas, it is imperative that research
be conducted to shed light on different strategies and modifications that may promote
4
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
The review presented below aims to summarize the pertinent literature related to
storybook reading and its relationship to language and literacy development. In doing so,
reading and thus the identified impact of storybook reading on later language and literacy
development are discussed. A particular emphasis is placed on the role of child interest
and engagement and its potential contributions to the success of shared book reading
interactions. Finally, potential means for facilitating and promoting child interest and
engagement during storybook reading are discussed highlighting the need for empirical
contribute to child language and literacy development. In a seminal study, Hart and
Risley (1995) observed 42 families over a three-year period. Thirteen of the dyads were
as families from middle socioeconomic status, 13 were classified as families from lower
socioeconomic status and 6 families were classified as receiving welfare. The data
collected included monthly one hour observations over approximately 2 ½ years during a
variety of caregiver-child interactions including mealtime, play and other activities of
daily living.
experiences of children from varying socioeconomic levels, the results also highlight a
potential relationship between early differences in the quantity of language heard in the
home and later language development. The results of Hart and Risley’s study showed
significant differences between the groups in the amount of language heard per hour and
the total number of words heard by the children. Specifically, during the time the
children were between 11-18 months of age, the average child from professional
socioeconomic status heard a mean of 2,150 words and 642 utterances per hour with 75%
of these utterances being directly addressed to the child. The average child from middle
socioeconomic status (including children from both middle and lower socioeconomic
status) heard 1,250 words and 535 utterances per hour, with 60% of these utterances
addressed to the child. In contrast, the average child from a family receiving welfare
heard a mean of only 620 words and 394 utterances per hour with only approximately
50% of these utterances addressed to the child. The result of Hart and Risley’s study led
them to estimate that by age three, children from the professional families heard an
average of 30 million words, children from the working class families heard an average
of 20 million words, and children from families receiving welfare heard an average of
Given the differences noted in the early home experiences and the number of
words heard by each group of children, Hart and Risley examined the expressive
vocabularies of these children when they were 30 -36 months of age. The results
6
mirrored the findings presented earlier in that children from professional families
from families receiving welfare added an average of 357 words to their expressive
vocabularies (less than half the increase identified for children from professional
families). Hart and Risley concluded that the significant differences in early language
(1995) examining the relationship between the amount and quality of language heard
during the first 3 years of life and achievement scores when the children were 9-10 years
analysis revealed the quality of caregiver language during the first three years of life
accounted for 61% of the variance in the children’s scores on the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test – Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) and the Test of Language
Development (TOLD; Hammill & Newcomer, 1988). Additionally, the PPVT-R scores
collected for the children in the third grade from families receiving welfare revealed a
professional socioeconomic status. Thus the findings from this study provide clear
evidence that there is a significant relationship between the early experiences of children
during the first few years of life and their later achievement on formal assessments of
language.
7
Storybook Reading Provides a Facilitating Setting
The potential impact of differences in the amount and type of language to which
children are exposed has previously led researchers to identify different contexts of
caregiver and child interactions that may contribute to increases in language and literacy
development. A variety of contextual settings have been examined but the most common
settings have been storybook reading (Deloache & DeMendoza, 1987; Moerk, 1985;
Ninio & Bruner, 1978; Pellegrini et al., 1985; Snow & Goldfield, 1983) and play
(Girolametto, 1988; Girolametto, Weitzman, Wiigs, & Pearce, 1999; Kaderavek &
Sulzby, 1998b). Mealtime and dressing have also been examined as contexts that have
the potential to benefit language and literacy development. However, these contexts have
received less attention in the research to date and are therefore not reviewed in detail
here.
examined 63 mother-child dyads within these contexts. Thirty of the dyads were from
working class families and the remaining were from upper-middle class families.
Working class was defined as completing high school with no further education except
positions. Upper-middle class was defined as completing at least two years or more of
language behaviors of the mothers that were measured included mean length of utterance
(MLU), speech rate, total number of root words, number of topic continuing replies,
8
behavioral directives. These language behaviors were derived from videotaped samples
of the dyads collected during mealtime, dressing, book reading, and toy play.
The results indicated that child-directed speech during toy play included the
highest rate of directives and the lowest rate of conversation eliciting utterances. In
shared book reading, the directives observed during toy play may have a negative effect
on child language development (Hart & Risley, 1995; Nelson, 1973). In mealtime
contexts, the rate of maternal speech was even lower, yet the rate of conversation-
eliciting utterances was the highest (along with dressing). Maternal speech was different
from all other contexts for dressing in that it had the least lexical complexity.
Interestingly, given the focus of the current study, shared book reading (as opposed to the
other three contexts) had the greatest lexical diversity, the greatest syntactic complexity,
and the highest rate of topic-continuing replies. Each of these characteristics of maternal
language use have been found to relate positively with a variety of measures of child
Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). The results from Hoff-Ginsberg support the notion that
In comparing storybook reading and free play contexts, Jones and Adamson
(1987) examined the interactions between 32 children age 18-23 months and their
storybook reading and in free play. The results of this study indicated that the type of
activity greatly influenced the language used by the dyad. Specifically, book reading was
9
found to involve more metalinguistic language use and increased the interpretability of
the children’s communicative intent as compared to the free play task. Additionally,
mothers used more referential speech and less language aimed at regulating the behavior
Given the findings from these studies, there is some indication that storybook
reading provides a unique opportunity for language and literacy learning not necessarily
afforded during these other contexts. As noted, maternal speech during book reading has
also been found to be structurally more complex and often includes a larger vocabulary
than mother-child interactions in other contexts. Book reading interactions also typically
include more questions and labeling behaviors as well as fewer directives (Hoff-
frequently occurring routine in the lives of many families (e.g., Trelease, 2001) and thus
successful storybook interactions are potentially great. Therefore, storybook reading was
chosen as the context to be investigated in the current study because of the evidence
suggesting that this activity typically involves richer language input (compared to the
language input during other contexts) believed to be conducive to language and literacy
development.
(1978a). This theory is built on the premise that individual intellectual development
cannot be understood without reference to the social milieu in which the child is
10
embedded. According to Vygotsky, children acquire knowledge through social
where the child’s problem solving can be guided by a more knowledgeable other who can
structure and model the appropriate solution to the problem. Cognitive processing
therefore occurs first on a social plane where it is internalized and transformed to form an
individual plane (Rogoff, 1990). Shared storybook reading is a prime example of this
type of social context interaction between a caregiver and a young child. Through
storybook reading interactions, the child’s cognitive development, language and literacy
skills are enhanced in interaction with a more knowledgeable other. Vygotsky’s (1978)
storybook interactions by providing a known context within which the caregiver can
important context for language and literacy development through her notion of “guided
children and their caregivers are able to construct “bridges from the child’s present
understanding and skills to reach new understandings and skills and arrange and structure
a child’s participation in activities with dynamic shifts over development in the child’s
responsibilities” (Rogoff, 1990, p8). The context surrounding shared storybook reading
offers a natural framework for the child’s role as an apprentice in guided participation by
providing a setting for children to share in an experience that they might not be able to
participate in without the guided support of a more knowledgeable reader. The guided
11
participation allows children to function at the edge of their competence but still remain
caregivers consciously and unconsciously support their children, while also challenging
them to become more competent in the shared book reading interaction, cognitive
Storybook reading provides a child with an opportunity to learn through social interaction
with a more knowledgeable other through a process of ongoing scaffolding and support
from the caregiver. This process is adjusted by the caregiver to match the child’s zone of
potentially beneficial context for learning comes from Arnold Sameroff (1993; 1995).
The transactional developmental model takes into account both the individual as well as
important to the success of storybook reading given that it allows consideration of the
child, the caregiver, and the interaction between the two when examining such an
the child and the dynamic experiences afforded by his or her caregiver/s within a given
social context. The transactional model gives equal emphasis to the role of the caregiver
12
as well as the role of the child. As suggested by Sameroff and Fiese (1990), development
interactions, it is important to examine both the individual and the social context in which
context for language and literacy development, research over the last 20 years has aimed
to document the relationships between early storybook reading and later literacy and
language development (Bus et al., 1995; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Moerk, 1985;
Pellegrini et al., 1985). Storybook reading is believed to facilitate early episodes of joint
attention between caregiver and child by providing a mutual focus of attention in which
caregivers are able to expand and extend the child’s language through the use of their
own language and gestures (Moerk, 1985; Neuman, 1996). The mutual focus of attention
achieved during storybook reading is important for vocabulary mapping as well as other
areas of language development (Morales et al., 2000). Storybook interactions are also
separate meaning from context. Through storybook reading children are first introduced
to unique contextual aspects of the interaction. These contextual aspects involve the part
of the story that is directly observable in the illustrations. With increasing development
on the part of the child, caregivers are able to use storybooks to help the child move
beyond the here and now through interpretation and inferences. The process of
13
decontextualization is believed to be a critical component of symbolic development and
therefore is important for many aspects of language and cognitive growth (Fenson, 1986).
and literacy including receptive language, vocabulary, emergent literacy skills, and later
reading and academic success (Bus et al., 1995; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Justice,
Meier, & Walpole, 2005; Moerk, 1985; Phillips & McNaughton, 1990; Snow et al.,
children new ideas and concepts (Arnold, Lonigan, Whitehurst, & Epstein, 1994).
During storybook reading, caregivers have been found to use a variety of strategies to
help scaffold learning (Pellegrini et al., 1985). A caregiver’s behaviors and the
corresponding talk that is evoked during storybook reading make interactions centered
around storybooks an ideal learning environment for language and literacy skills
(DeTemple, 2001).
Results of studies examining children’s language and literacy skills have indicated
strong stability in the rankings of children in these areas across the age span (Dickinson
& McCabe, 2001; Sulzby, 1985). Therefore, children who score low on measures of
language and literacy skills in kindergarten/first grade relative to their peers typically
continue to score low on language and literacy measures later in school (Dickinson &
McCabe, 2001). This finding suggests that the individual differences which are apparent
Evidence also suggests that early storybook reading is a strong predictor of later
reading achievement (Bus et al., 1995). Unfortunately there has not been a consensus
regarding exactly which contributing factors of shared book reading are the most
14
important to later language and literacy success. This is partly due to the fact that
children come to school with various amounts of previous exposure to book reading.
However, the later language and literacy outcomes for children with varying experiences
are clearly different, warranting further investigation into the factors that contribute to
factors. Two key factors that can be identified in the literature are the ability of
caregivers to provide appropriate opportunities and support for their children during
storybook interactions and the children’s ability to demonstrate their current skill level
(Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994). Further, these factors have a transactional relationship
development (Sameroff & Fiese, 1990). For example, the impact of a caregiver’s
shared storybook interactions (Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998). Through this transactional
interaction, a caregiver has the potential to influence his or her child, as well as the child
having the potential to impact the caregiver. Furthermore, both partners in the shared
storybook interaction are influenced by the nature of their interactions and the framework
the influence of the environment and the dynamic interactions between the child and the
15
organized, interrelated systems with each system having the potential to influence both
within and across systems. The effects of this transactional process are cumulative and
have increasing influence over each other over time, demonstrating continuities in
influences language and vice versa, and these influences are stable across the age span.
storybook reading, a number of factors have been considered (for review see Bus et al.,
1995; Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994). These factors not
only contribute to the success of caregiver-child storybook interactions, they also have
been shown to influence later language and literacy development. Specific contributions
associated with the caregiver and social context of the interaction include the frequency
of storybook reading, the benefits of repeated readings of the same book, the quality of
the home literacy environment, and the important role of the primary caregivers, teachers,
and other care providers. Each of these contributions will be reviewed focusing on the
potential role each factor plays in the success of storybook reading and thus its impact on
reading and the positive influence of successful interactions on later language and literacy
development (Bus et al., 1995; Lonigan, 1994; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994; Senechal,
LeFevre, Hudson, & Lawson, 1996; Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002). For example,
Scarborough, Dobrich, and Hager (1991) examined the frequency of adult reading,
16
caregiver-child reading, and child solitary reading in the home through interviews and
questionnaires collected when the children were 30, 36, 42, and 48 months of age. A
total of 45 families were included in the study with 22 caregivers identified as poor
typical readers with at least 1 family member identified as a poor reader, and 22
family (see Scarborough, 1989, 1990 for a full description of the design and sampling
methods). The study involved an initial interview with caregivers about their current
addition, the caregivers completed short questionnaires regarding the frequency of book
The results indicated that children who were later identified as poor readers
participated in substantially fewer book reading experiences than children who were
identified as typical readers. As the authors note, the identified differences were not
related to caregiver reading ability, but something inherent in the child that was
contributing to how often the caregiver and child engaged in shared book reading.
In a later study, Debaryshe (1995) examined the relationship between shared book
reading experiences and the child’s early oral language skills. In this study, Debaryshe
factors of interest included age at which mothers began reading to their children, the
frequency of home reading sessions, the number of different stories read per week, and
the frequency of library visits. Child receptive and expressive language skills were
17
Multiple regression analyses were conducted and revealed that frequency of
storybook reading was more strongly correlated with receptive language skills than
expressive language, with home reading exposure accounting for 35% of the variance in
receptive language scores on the Reynell. Additionally, the age at which mothers began
reading to their children was found to be the most robust predictor of oral language skills.
children’s early literacy related skills (Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002). As a part of
dyads engaging in storybook interactions in their homes right before the children entered
kindergarten. Each dyad read a familiar book chosen by the caregiver and an unfamiliar
book provided by the researcher. In addition, caregivers reported on the frequency with
which their children engaged in reading activities on a 4-point scale (0 = none, 1= less
than 1x per week, 2= multiple times each week, 3= almost daily). Multiple regression
analyses revealed strong correlations between frequency of reading in the home and
orientation towards print and phonological awareness. Correlations were not found
Although evidence has supported the importance of the frequency that caregivers
engage in storybook reading with their young children and its relationship to later
development, there has been some debate over the exact contribution of frequency when
Scarborough and Dobrich (1994) cited previously, the frequency of storybook reading
only accounted for a small amount of the total variance associated with literacy outcomes
18
accounted for approximately 8% of the overall variance in later school achievement and
only about 7% of the variance associated with emergent literacy skills and later language
outcomes. The authors concluded that although the frequency of storybook interactions
between children and their caregivers is indeed associated with later language and
the same studies included in Scarborough and Dobrich’s (1994) review. Their results
indicated a more moderate relationship between frequency of storybook reading and later
language and literacy outcomes. The effect sizes were strongest for language skills
(d=.67), with slightly smaller effect sizes for reading and emergent literacy (d = .55 and d
= .58, respectively). Bus and colleagues (1995) suggested that differences between these
and previous results are due to the fact that the quantitative meta-analysis used to
is not necessarily the major component contributing to the benefits associated with
storybook reading. As pointed out in the literature (Bus et al., 1995; Scarborough &
Dobrich, 1994), frequency may in and of itself be dependent on other factors such as the
storybook reading that has been examined in the literature is the benefit of repeated
same story and pictures over and over again (DeTemple, 2001; Moerk, 1985). By
19
repeatedly reading familiar books, children are able to participate in the interaction by
filling in familiar words and eventually taking on more of the responsibility of the book
incorporate previous adult models into their own speech, extending their language
abilities (Snow & Goldfield, 1983). Furthermore, caregivers have been found to use
more high level talking strategies during repeated readings of storybooks compared to
initial readings. During initial readings, caregivers appear to be checking the child’s
comprehension of the story (Crowe, 2000). In doing so, caregivers typically ask more
yes/no type questions and questions that request a label from the child. In contrast,
during subsequent readings of the same book, caregivers encourage their children to
This type of talk is consistent with DeTemple’s (2001) notion of immediate and
the here and now and is therefore contextual in nature. Immediate talk includes labeling,
yes/no questioning, demonstrating a skill (e.g., counting or pointing), and the familiar
“fill in the blank” routine where caregivers provide a partial statement with the
expectation that the child will fill in the missing information. In contrast, non-immediate
talk encourages children to make connections to their own background knowledge and
that because non-immediate talk refers to information that is not immediately available in
the pictures or environment, this type of language typically involves extended discourse
which is more implicit and complex than the language involved in immediate talk. Thus,
20
non-immediate talk (and the child’s responses to it) is believed to be a higher level of
language use compared to immediate talk and is often observed during repeated readings
early literacy measures and non-immediate talk to be highly positively associated with
later language and literacy skills. Specifically, caregivers who used a high percentage of
immediate talk during storybook interactions had children who had lower scores on
measures of early literacy skills. On the other hand, caregivers who used more non-
immediate talk and encouraged their children to think beyond what was illustrated in the
pictures, had children who scored higher on language and literacy measures. Further,
storybook readings because the language and context of the story are more familiar.
Caregivers are therefore no longer checking for story comprehension but are attempting
to extend their child’s knowledge by relating the story to personal experiences and asking
the child to draw on previous readings to make predictions (McDonnell, Friel-Patti, &
In a more recent study, McDonnell and her colleagues (2003) examined the
interactions between caregivers and their children during repeated storybook readings
over a three-week period. The dyads were videotaped during storybook interactions on
four occasions reading the same book. The results indicated that the caregivers continued
21
utilizing increasingly more sophisticated language strategies. Interestingly, the results
also indicated that caregivers became less active participants in the storybook interaction
as children became more familiar with the target book; thus, a shift in the balance in
participation between the caregiver and child was noted. These results, coupled with the
results from DeTemple (2001), support the idea that repeated readings of a familiar
storybook evoke increasingly more sophisticated language from both reading partners
and therefore are an important contributing factor to the associated benefits of storybook
reading.
In contrast to repeated readings of the same book, first time readings may provide
opportunities for immediate language building and examining the inherent interest and
engagement of the child. In addition, it may be expected that children who are interested
and engaged in first time readings may be more likely to seek out additional opportunities
for reading and thereby become further engaged, thus demonstrating the transactional
nature of these interactions. If caregivers can promote the initial (and continued) interest
and engagement of their child in shared storybook interactions, then the child is more
likely to benefit from the higher-level and non-immediate kinds of strategies used by
factors contribute to successful initial readings and what benefits might be associated
with identifying strategies for increasing this success. Therefore, studying initial
readings and strategies to enhance the engagement and interest in those interactions, may
interactions.
22
Quality of the Home Literacy Environment. An additional factor that may
contribute to storybook reading and its impact on language and literacy skills is the
quality of the home literacy environment (Bus et al., 1995; Snow et al., 1998; Whitehurst
& Lonigan, 1998). The home literacy environment includes measures such as the
number of children’s books available in the home, the availability of other literacy
materials in the home, and the amount of time other people in the household engage in
literacy activities (DeTemple, 2001). Many researchers have suggested that these
environmental factors influence the child’s motivation and interests in (and opportunities
for) literacy activities and therefore have the potential to influence language and literacy
development (Justice & Kaderavek, 2002; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994). In addition,
these measures have been linked to later vocabulary development and measures of
emergent literacy skills (Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1992; Payne, Whitehurst, & Angell,
1994; Roberts, Burchinal, & Durham, 1999; Roberts, Jurgens, & Burchinal, 2005;
Anderson-Yockel and Haynes (1994) examined the reading habits and attitudes of
10 African-American mothers and 10 White mothers, both from working class families.
The results indicated minimal differences between the African-American and White
mothers’ behaviors, as well as their beliefs and attitudes regarding storybook interactions.
Specifically, mothers in both groups were very similar in that they owned a library of
children’s books, read to their children on a regular basis, and perceived storybook
interactions to be important to their child’s development. The only differences that were
identified between the two groups of mothers were related to the frequency of reading.
White mothers reported reading to their children more frequently (than the African-
23
American mothers) as well as being read to more frequently when they were children.
The White mothers also reported reading books for pleasure more often than the African-
American mothers. It should be noted that no differences were found between the
language measures of the children examined in this study except for the African-
American children produced more spontaneous verbalizations and the White children
In looking at a composite measure of both the frequency of book reading and the
home literacy environment, Bus and colleagues’ (1995) review of previous literature
found these two factors to be associated with later language and literacy outcomes.
Specifically, children who were read to more often and had greater home literacy support
responsiveness and support of the home environment was found to be the strongest
predictor of children’s language and early literacy skills. The researchers followed 72
African American children beginning in their first year of life and continuing until the
daily literacy practices at 18, 30, and 42 months of age and again right before entry into
kindergarten. Dyads were also observed engaging in shared book reading interactions
when the children were 2, 3, and 4 years of age. Measures of the quality and
responsiveness of the home environment were collected at 9, 18, 30, and 42 months of
age using the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory
24
The global measurement of the home environment predicted receptive vocabulary
at 3 years of age, and early literacy skills and receptive and expressive language at 4
years of age and entry into kindergarten. The home environment was found to contribute
to language and early literacy development above and beyond specific literacy practices
storybook reading, and the quality of the interactions between caregivers and their young
children.
experiences of children with various backgrounds and therefore contribute to the impact
of storybook reading on later language and literacy development. Although the majority
of caregivers agree that storybook reading is important to the academic success of their
children, a large percent of caregivers are unclear as to exactly how their children learn to
read (Paratore, 2002). Dickinson and McCabe (2002) suggested that the academic
backgrounds can be attributed to a “mismatch” between the language used in the child’s
home environment and the language used in the school setting. These differences were
suggested to make it difficult for some children to understand the ways in which teachers
are asking them to use language. Additionally, evidence suggests that differences in the
early language and literacy experiences among children from various backgrounds may
also make it difficult for teachers to provide appropriate scaffolding to support the
learning of these children (Vernon-Feagans, 1996). Therefore, the child’s early home
experiences and literacy exposure are equally important when considering the success of
storybook interactions and their impact on language and literacy development. Although
25
there is increasing information available regarding differences that may occur among
that may occur for children from similar backgrounds, particularly related to their interest
Caregiver’s Role During Storybook Reading. The role of the caregiver during
storybook interactions has also been considered key to successful interactions and to
(Anderson-Yockel & Haynes, 1994; Bus et al., 1995; Haynes & Saunders, 1998;
Pellegrini et al., 1985; Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994). Caregivers have been observed to
progressively increase the complexity of the language strategies they incorporate during
storybook interactions over time (Deloache & DeMendoza, 1987). This progression
occurs as the child’s developing language and literacy skills increase, indicating that
caregivers are adjusting their input to match the perceived increases in the level of their
child’s abilities (Pellegrini et al., 1985). This notion of “parent attunement” is consistent
Vygotsky’s theory proposes that optimal learning occurs when input is provided within
this zone of proximal development. Therefore, the caregiver’s ability to ”attune” to their
child’s current skills and provide scaffolding to help children achieve success at slightly
higher levels is believed to facilitate language and literacy development (Pellegrini et al.,
1985). Interactions during storybook reading are ideal situations for providing this
optimal support.
26
As discussed earlier, caregivers of typically developing children focus initially on
the obvious aspects of the story depicted in the illustrations, utilizing increased amounts
become more capable, they begin to take on more of the responsibilities by initiating
more interactions, commenting, and pointing (McDonnell et al., 2003). With increasing
sophistication on the part of the child, caregivers begin to utilize more challenging
language strategies encouraging their children to make predictions and infer meaning.
Caregivers then “adapt, extend, clarify, and at times disregard the print” (Crowe, 2000 p
506) during storybook interactions in response to the behaviors of their children. In the
review by Scarborough and Dobrich (1994), caregivers’ behaviors before and after
language and literacy skills. However, as indicated by the authors, this finding warrants
additional research. Given that caregivers’ behaviors are key to the success of storybook
interactions, the following section reviews the literature related to attempts to modify
storybook reading and language and literacy as well as the predictive relationships
between storybook reading and later academic success, often results in the
and atypical development. The popular press has suggested that storybook reading is a
critical component of early child development and thus many caregivers seek
opportunities to engage in storybook reading with their young children (Trelease, 2001).
27
Furthermore, storybook reading seems particularly well-suited for children who are
experiencing difficulties with early language and literacy development given the inherent
opportunities for vocabulary building, the shared focus of attention, and the potential for
scaffolding in order to match the interaction to the child’s abilities and interest (Justice &
Kaderavek, 2002; Schuele & van Kleeck, 1987; Snow et al., 1998).
caregivers, early care and education providers, and teachers to use specific language
development in young children (Arnold et al., 1994; Whitehurst et al., 1994; Whitehurst
et al., 1999). The main purpose of dialogic reading is to shift the control of the
interaction from the caregiver to the child. The primary role of the adult during dialogic
reading is to serve as active listener and facilitator. By asking questions and adding
complexity of their own language use. Specific dialogic reading strategies such as
increasing the use of wh-questions, use of open-ended question forms, and avoiding
single response question forms such as yes/no questions, have been taught to caregivers
and other providers to effectively help them modify their language input to their children
Recent attention has also been given to training parents and other care providers
to focus on print-referencing skills during storybook reading. For instance, Justice and
her colleagues (Ezell & Justice, 2000; Justice & Ezell, 2000, 2002) trained caregivers to
point to the words in the book, highlight letters and sounds, and acknowledge a variety of
28
concepts about print while interacting with their children during storybook reading.
facilitating strategies, the majority of the print-referencing studies have also focused on
the caregiver’s contributions to the interaction with little or no direct attention given to
A further focus has been on modifying the literacy environment for young
and 23 were considered high proficiency readers. The study involved increasing the
access to literacy activities by providing each family with a total of 12 storybooks and the
opportunity to meet with a “book club” once a week for 12 weeks. Neuman defined
“access” not only as the acquisition of books but also as the opportunity to discuss the
books in a group format which allowed the caregivers to identify different strategies for
interacting with their children during storybook reading. The meetings of families and
professionals focused on discussing the book and its relation to each family. Neuman
quality of interactions between caregivers and their children of low and high reading
discussion regarding each book’s topic, caregivers were observed to increase their use of
Other research has focused on the support provided by preschool and kindergarten
teachers as well as early care and education providers during the early years of
29
development (Dickinson & McCabe, 2001). Research suggests that quality daycare and
preschool environments have the potential to act as a buffer against the negative effects
of other environmental factors such as low income (Whitehurst et al., 1994), low
caregiver literacy levels (Neuman, 1996), limited home literacy support (Dickinson &
McCabe, 2001), and other child characteristics (Vernon-Feagans, Hurley, & Yont, 2002).
Whitehurst and colleagues (1994) found that the teachers in their study were able to
Whitehurst and his colleagues (1999) provided storybook reading training to Head Start
teachers. The results of this study support the findings of Whitehurst et al (1994) in that
positive effects were also observed from the emergent literacy intervention. Other
studies have found similar results (Burchinal, Lau, & Sparling, 1994) indicating that high
quality storybook interactions in daycare and school have beneficial effects on the
quality care outside of the home for children who are at risk for literacy failure.
Unfortunately there are many barriers to the potential benefits of storybook reading in
classroom and daycare settings. For example, Whitehurst and others (1994) reported that
none of the teachers who participated in their study continued the small group reading
sessions after terminating the study. As indicated by Dickinson and McCabe (2001), the
degree to which teachers have the time and resources to incorporate these recommended
30
Vernon-Feagans (1996) also reported some potential challenges when
determining the particular components of storybook reading in early care and educational
Intervention Project that was initiated in 1972. This project was one of the first
The Abecedarian children were primarily from African American low income
backgrounds and the Mainstream control children were primarily white children from
program where they received fulltime high quality childcare services (n = 57) or the non-
interactions between children in both groups during storybook reading as well as the
The results indicated that although teachers were not responding differently to the
children, per se, they were responding differently to the types of responses that the
children gave during storybook interactions. Specifically, when a child responded with
an irrelevant or incorrect answer, the teachers were more likely to ignore this response
question, the teachers were more likely to restructure their question or encourage the
child to try again. Unfortunately, the Abecedarian children were twice as likely to
provide an irrelevant response as opposed to no response when they did not know an
ignored their responses rather than restructuring the question or encouraging the child to
31
try again. As a result, the two groups of children benefited differentially from the
storybook interactions. Although it is important to think about the reasons for the
differences noted for the child responses, it was evident that children influenced the type
The cumulative findings from these studies indicate that caregivers can be trained
to take advantage of the opportunities that occur during storybook reading, and in turn,
guiding parents and other care providers to increase specific language facilitating
strategies such as the strategies targeted by Whitehurst and his colleagues have been
intervention when considered within the context of the family. It is also reasonable to
conclude that scaffolding techniques during storybook interactions have the potential to
impact language and literacy development if teachers are trained to provide children with
responses of the child. Unfortunately this is not always the case and individual
differences between children upon school entry may be exacerbated (Dickinson &
McCabe, 2001). As indicated by Mahoney and Wheedon (1997), programs that enable
development and well-being are an important focus of intervention for young children.
studies have also focused on children with language impairments and children with
developmental disorders (Bellon, Ogletree, & Harn, 2000; Crowe, Norris, & Hoffman,
32
2000; Ezell, Justice, & Parsons, 2000; Light, Binger, & Kelford Smith, 1994; McNeill &
developing children, the majority of studies addressing children with disabilities have
storybook reading. One of Whitehurst and colleagues’ (Whitehurst et al., 1991) first
dialogic reading intervention studies involved children with specific expressive language
impairment. Crain-Thoreson and Dale (1999) and McNeill and Fowler (1999) also
provided training to caregivers of preschool age children with mild to moderate language
delays. The findings from these studies indicate that caregivers can be trained to increase
their use of language facilitating strategies during storybook reading, resulting in notable
increases in a variety of child behaviors (e.g. total number of words produced, ratio of
effectiveness of storybook reading as a context for parent training with 6 caregivers and
their children with language impairments. Caregivers were trained to use interaction
patterns during storybook reading that evoked attention to the pictures, requested
information, responded to the child’s question, and/or acknowledged the child’s response.
words, and number of different words produced during storybook reading as a result of
caregiver training.
language and literacy development of children who have a diagnosis of autism spectrum
disorder. Kirchner (1991) described a case study of a 4-year-old child with Asperger
33
reciprocal book reading. The results indicated that the child was able to use language in
increasingly novel ways after the introduction of the storybooks. Bellon and colleagues
(2000) also examined the effect of adult scaffolding during repeated storybook reading
with one child diagnosed with high functioning autism. The results of these studies were
similar to previous research indicating that adult scaffolding is effective in increasing the
verbal output of children with disabilities. Although limited in scope, the available
research suggests that storybook reading provides a context within which caregivers can
successfully learn to facilitate language and literacy development in both children who
experience difficulties in these areas as well as children who are typically developing.
One of the issues, however, that is not as clear is the degree to which certain
characteristics of the child such as language skills contribute to the child’s interest and
engagement in shared storybook reading and how these factors may be transactionally
is the influence of the child. Although limited in scope, contributions specific to the child
have been identified including the child’s ability to participate and demonstrate their
understandings during storybook reading and the child’s interest and engagement during
Studies have found that although the majority of caregivers are able to judge
appropriately their child’s current level of functioning, this is not always the case
34
(Pellegrini, McGillicuddy-Delisi, Sigel, & Brody, 1986). Although there are probably
many factors contributing to the difficulty in estimating a child’s current functioning, the
child’s ability to respond to and engage with the caregiver can be assumed to impact
example, Crowe (2000) found that the caregivers in her study were underestimating the
abilities of their children with a diagnosis of autism and therefore not providing adequate
scaffolding to support their children during storybook interactions. Other studies have
indicated that children with expressive language delays and/or other developmental
disabilities may have difficulty demonstrating their skills (Kaderavek & Sulzby, 1998b;
Marvin & Mirenda, 1993; Marvin & Wright, 1997) and therefore their caregivers have a
difficult time providing scaffolding that is within their zone of proximal development.
Further characteristics that have been highlighted in the literature include the
child’s motivation and interest surrounding storybook reading. In fact, Scarborough and
Dobrich (1994) suggested that the child’s motivation and interest may be more influential
than the frequency and quality of storybook interactions. In addition, the work of Justice
and Kaderavek (2002) has indicated that a large proportion of children with disabilities
do not enjoy storybook interactions. These authors argue that motivation is critical to the
influences, the child’s motivations and internal rewards for such an interaction are likely
to influence the impact storybook reading will have on the overall benefits of these
interactions. Given the goal of the current study, it is important to examine the available
literature regarding the impact of child engagement and interest on the success of
storybook reading.
35
Engagement and Interest in Storybook Reading. Child interest and engagement
the benefits associated with storybook reading. Engagement has been defined in the early
education literature as “the amount of time children spend interacting appropriately with
the environment at different levels of competence” (McWilliam & Bailey, 1992, p.234).
Although engagement during storybook reading has only recently received attention in
the literature (Baker, Scher, & Mackler, 1997), the importance of engagement with the
childhood learning (Kruif & McWilliam, 1999; McWilliam & Bailey, 1992) with high
& Butts, 1984). Researchers have suggested that engagement is essential to learning
(McWilliam & Bailey, 1992) and thus one can assume that engagement may contribute
significantly to the success of interactions between young children and their caregivers.
In research examining storybook reading, the term “engagement” has been used in
include directing one’s gaze toward the book and turning the pages. The terms “interest”
and/or “motivation” have typically been used to refer to the child’s intrinsic behaviors
and enjoyment of storybook reading and are most often measured by caregiver report or
self-report (Baker & Scher, 2002; Lonigan, 1994; Senechal et al., 1996). The majority of
studies that refer to the child’s motivation to participate in either independent or shared
reading have focused on older children (kindergarten and beyond) (Baker, Mackler,
Sonnenschein, & Serpell, 2001; Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997). This is primarily due to the
36
difficulties in reliably measuring motivation in young children. Therefore, when
inferred to represent the young child’s interest and motivation to partake in such
activities.
Although the observable behaviors of joint attention and engagement and interest
in storybook reading appear to overlap during the early stages of child development, the
reading go beyond the ability to sustain joint attention and direct and follow the gaze of
another. Engagement and interest in storybook reading during the early years of
toward literacy related activities (Guthrie & Knowles, 2001). For the purposes of this
study, joint attention was conceptualized as the ability to participate in shared interactions
with a caregiver. On the other hand in very young children, engagement, and more so
The distinction between these terms is important as each contribute to the success of
shared storybook reading. Although all of these factors deserve attention, the current
interactions with a caregiver and their active efforts to do so. These factors will be
Evidence suggests that early engagement and interest in storybook reading are
important contributing factors to the benefits associated with shared storybook reading
37
and its relationships with later reading achievement (Lonigan, 1994; Scarborough &
Dobrich, 1994). Early interest in reading has been linked to the frequency of independent
reading in later childhood (Baker, Dreher, & Guthrie, 2000; Baker et al., 1997; Baker &
Wigfield, 1999; Snow et al., 1998) which has been linked to improved reading
achievement scores and later language and literacy outcomes (Anderson, Wilson, &
Fielding, 1988; Baker et al., 1997; Cipielewski & Stanovich, 1992). Specifically,
Anderson and colleagues (1988) found that the amount of independent reading engaged
in between the second and fifth grade accounted for 16% of the variance in fifth grade
graders regarding their children’s attitudes and behaviors toward literacy activities. The
results indicated a correlation between the composite score for the children’s literacy
related attitudes and behaviors, and their concurrent reading scores. Although only
minimal research was conducted following the results of Almy’s findings, there has been
recent attention in the research literature regarding the relationship between child interest
and engagement in early reading, and later reading achievement (Arnold et al., 1994;
Baker et al., 2001; Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1992; DeBaryshe, 1995b; Frijters et al., 2000;
Morrow, 1983; Senechal et al., 1996; Wells, 1985). Arnold and colleagues (1994)
suggest a relationship between the age when caregivers begin reading to their children
and the child’s interest and motivation to engage in these types of interactions
independently as well as with an adult. Other findings suggest that the frequency of early
storybook reading is also related to child interest and engagement (Lonigan, 1994). In
the review cited previously by Scarborough and Dobrich (1994), child interest in literacy
38
accounted for approximately 14% of the variance in children’s language and literacy
outcomes. It should be noted that this correlation is higher than the one reported for
frequency of storybook reading, which accounted for only about 8% of the variance in
influence regarding frequency and child engagement. Further, there is little information
available as to additional strategies or techniques that may enhance a child’s interest and
engagement in shared storybook reading. Thus, studies that may shed light on these kinds
interactions.
Measuring Child Engagement and Interest. Our knowledge base regarding child
engagement during storybook reading and the impact on later language and literacy
development is limited primarily due to the fact that there are very few instruments
available for measuring engagement and interest that are appropriate for very young
children (Baker & Scher, 2002). To date, the majority of studies examining child interest
and engagement have relied on caregiver report. For example, Sonnenschein and
Munsterman (2002) asked caregivers how frequently their child read books with others at
home. Roberts and colleagues (2005) interviewed the participating mothers in their study
when their children were 18, 30, and 42 months and when their children entered
kindergarten to rate whether the children enjoyed being read to on a five point scale ( 1=
not at all, 2 = a little, 3 = pretty much, 4 = very much, and 5 = loves it). However, the
majority of caregiver reporting methods to date only include a few specific questions
related to child interest and engagement and are dependent on the caregiver’s own
perceptions.
39
With older children (kindergarten and beyond), researchers have used self-report
measures. Specifically, Baker and Scher (2002) asked children to choose which of two
stuffed animals they were most like, with one animal portrayed as demonstrating a
negative attitude toward the literacy related questions and one animal portrayed as
demonstrating a positive attitude. Similar self reporting methods were used by Frijters,
Barron, and Brunello (2000). In this study children were presented with two pictures of
children engaging in similar literacy activities. One picture showed a child with a happy
face and the other picture showed a child with a sad face. Both pictures were exactly the
same with the exception of the expression on the child’s face. For example, one picture
includes a child sitting reading a book alone. For this picture the researcher would say to
the child, “This girl likes to look at books by herself” while pointing to the happy picture.
The examiner would then point to the picture of the girl looking sad and say “This girl
does not like to read books by herself.” The child was then asked which girl they were
most like. Depending on the child’s response (e.g. child pointed to happy face), the
examiner would then say, “Do you like to look at books by yourself a little or a lot?”
Both approaches appeared to successfully differentiate children who did and did not have
an interest in reading.
storybook reading such as choosing to look at books, directing gaze at the book, pointing
to pictures in the book, and turning pages (Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1992; Morrow,
1983). Active Learning Theory proposes that learning is an active process that requires
acceptance of knowledge that exists out in the world; rather, learning requires that the
40
learner actively engages with their environment. This theory also supposes that active
learning does not necessarily require physical activity but is reliant on actively engaging
the brain during an activity. Therefore, it might be assumed that more subtle behavioral
indicators may serve as indication that the child is engaged but not overloaded by the
cognitive demands of the interaction itself. Active learning may be seen in children
during shared storybook interactions in many forms, both physically and mentally. The
child who points to pictures on request or answers questions during storybook reading, as
well as the child who shows engagement and interest in a book by attending and listening
challenging” (Carlton, 1996). Mastery motivation is believed to be the basis for later
intrinsic motivation and has been found to predict later cognitive development (Messer,
et al., 1986). Intrinsic motivation is believed to facilitate greater learning and academic
success for elementary and junior high school students (Gottfried, 1985). This is
assumed to be due to the fact that intrinsically motivated individuals tend to be more
actively involved in their learning and have been found to use strategies that promote
Therefore attempts to identify strategies that may increase a child’s interest and
41
Mastery motivation is believed to develop across three age ranges: birth to 9
months, 9-24 months, and 24-36 months (Barrett and Morgan, 1995). At birth, infants
are seen to have an undifferentiated need for competence and through development
of competence and individual persistence and preference for challenge and/or novelty
(White, 1959). If we assume that this process is the same for building the intrinsic
motivation to engage in shared book reading and later independent reading, then research
motivation during storybook reading with children much younger than the children in the
Senechal and colleagues (Senechal et al., 1995) who examined the differences between
children’s incidental acquisition of new vocabulary words during storybook reading when
study involved 32 four- and five-year-old children. Children were randomly assigned to
one of two book reading conditions. In condition 1, children listened passively to one
book read three different times on three different occasions. In condition 2, children were
asked a series of questions during the book reading interaction that required them to
actively participate by pointing or labeling the pictures. In this condition, the adult asked
a “what” or “where” question following each targeted word. Pretest and posttest
measures were collected for comprehension and production vocabulary for targeted
words. The results of this study indicated that children who actively participated in the
book reading interactions by pointing to (M = 4.2) and or labeling the words (M = 4.0)
42
understood and produced significantly more new words than children who simply
Finally, one preliminary study utilized a subjective rating scale to rank the quality
1998a). This particular study compared the child’s engagement during storybook reading
with a caregiver, to caregiver-child interactions during play. The results identified a set
of children who demonstrated “low orientation” or limited engagement that was specific
to the context of storybook reading. These children were described as reluctant and/or
resistant to engage in the book reading interaction, and if able to engage, demonstrated
minimal interest in the book. Taken together, the studies reviewed thus far support the
hypothesis that child interest and engagement in storybook reading is an important factor
to consider when examining the benefits of storybook reading and the later relationships
with language and literacy development of young children. However, less information is
available about the early development of interest and engagement in storybook reading in
Early Foundation for Interest and Engagement. Early child interest and
storybook interactions and a predictor of later reading skills (Scarborough & Dobrich,
1994; Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002). Given that these influences can be assumed
43
Arnold and colleagues (Arnold et al., 1994) suggest that many typically
family, and developmental factors. The interview also included specific questions
regarding the literacy attitudes and behaviors of the child. Over the course of the study,
caregivers completed a diary indicating their daily literacy practices with their child. The
diary elicited a variety of information including ratings for their child’s interest and
enjoyment in literacy related activities using a 5-point scale. The researchers found that
all of the children had extensive experience with storybook reading by the time they were
2 ½ years of age. Moreover, those children whose caregivers reported beginning to read
with them prior to 6 months of age were more likely to enjoy storybook reading and
demonstrate greater interest and engagement than children whose caregivers reported
From these findings, it is reasonable to assume that many children are developing
at least a foundation for interest (or lack of interest) in storybook reading interactions
before they are 2 ½ years of age. Unfortunately, there is limited research available
examining these early interactions between caregivers and their very young children and
the factors contributing to early child engagement and interest in storybook reading. For
this reason, it is critical to examine the storybook interactions of very young typically
developing infants and toddlers in order to better understand the factors that contribute to
an early foundation for interest and engagement as well as identify potential strategies for
44
One of the foundational skills for building interest and engagement may be joint
attention. There is a large evidence base supporting a link between early joint attention
skills and later language development. Joint attention, defined as the ability to follow and
direct the attention of others (Baldwin, 1995), typically develops within a relatively brief
period between 9 and 15 months of age (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). During
with evidence supporting the theory that joint attention skills are precursors to language
development (Markus, Mundy, Morales, Delgado, & Yale, 2000; Morales, Mundy, &
Rojas, 1998; Morales et al., 2000; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Sigman & Ruskin,
1995; Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). Specifically, lexical development, receptive language
skills, and expressive language abilities have all been found to correlate positively with
early measures of joint attention (Markus et al., 2000; Morales et al., 1998; Morales et al.,
Storybook reading is an ideal context for the development of joint attention skills
because it provides multiple opportunities for both caregiver and child to direct and
respond to the gaze of another. Storybooks provide a concrete representation that can be
examined and discussed by the caregiver-child dyad for extended periods of time as well
as be returned to for multiple viewings (Moerk, 1985). Joint attention skills are also
unique opportunity for providing young children with multiple experiences with new
words (Markus et al., 2000; Morales et al., 2000). Evidence suggests that the majority of
45
mothers’ language during storybook reading with very young typically developing
children relates to vocabulary development through picture description and labeling, and
the use of simple wh-question forms (Moerk, 1985; Ninio & Bruner, 1978). Joint
attention skills are critical in order for a child to be able to take advantage of the multiple
When thinking specifically about storybook reading, the ability to sustain joint attention
is only one of the factors contributing to the success of the interaction. Preliminary
research suggests that there are children who are capable of demonstrating joint attention
for extended periods of time when engaged in certain types of activities (e.g. play);
however, these same children demonstrate limited interest and engagement during
storybook reading (Kaderavek & Sulzby, 1998a). Thus the study of means to motivate
joint attention and therefore increase the interest and engagement during storybook
interactions.
Children who experience difficulties with language and literacy development are at
particular risk for low interest and engagement in storybook reading. Morrow (1983)
examined the storybook interest of 172 kindergarten children of which 75 children were
identified as exhibiting high literacy interest and 98 were identified as exhibiting low
interest in literacy related activities. The findings indicated that the majority of children
in the high interest group demonstrated higher pre-literacy skills compared to children in
the low interest group. Evidence from Kaderavek and Sulzby (1998a) suggests that
although many typically developing children are highly engaged by storybook reading,
46
there are many children with language impairments who demonstrate limited interests in
participating in such interactions. Specifically, Kaderavek and Sulzby found that almost
half of the children with language impairments in their study exhibited low interest in
storybook interactions whereas all of the typically developing peers demonstrated high
rates of interest and engagement during storybook reading. Baker and colleagues (2000)
developing children as well as children with language impairments, it has also been
when the child is uninterested and difficult to engage may have unanticipated negative
the child’s limited interest by persisting in an activity that is unpleasant for the child
(Baker, 2003; Baker et al., 2001; Frijters et al., 2000; Kaderavek & Sulzby, 1998a). This
persistence has the potential to begin a negative feedback loop in which the child, for
whatever reason, is uninterested in storybook reading and yet the caregiver continues in
the interaction, potentially increasing the disinterest and/or avoidance on the part of the
child and decreasing the likelihood that the child will engage in independent storybook
reading later in life (Scarborough & Dobrich, 1994). Two situations are therefore
created: struggling readers who have difficulty understanding print and therefore choose
to avoid reading situations, and avoidant readers who do not experience difficulty with
the act of reading per se but who choose to not engage in later independent book reading
due to a lack of motivation and interest (Baker, 2003; Dreher & Baker, 2003). Therefore,
47
research aimed at identifying strategies that have the potential to increase child interest
and engagement during storybook reading, and thus decrease the likelihood of triggering
strategies have been highlighted throughout the literature as facilitating engagement and
interest in storybook reading. DeBaryshe (1995b) suggests that reading storybooks aloud
with young children may promote child engagement and interest in storybook reading
thus increasing the frequency with which children seek out caregivers to engage in
Additionally, repeated storybook reading and the caregiver’s ability to adapt the language
input during storybook reading interactions may also facilitate engagement and interest in
this type of activity (Martinez & Roser, 1985; Pierce & McWilliams, 1993).
visual props during circle time. Visual props included individual or sequenced pictures
such as photos or hand-drawn pictures, objects such as puppets and toy props, and
pictures of repetitive actions such as clapping hands or stomping feet. Using a single
subject multiple baseline design, Danko trained classroom teachers to incorporate visual
supports (including props) into their regular circle time activities. The study involved
three children with a diagnosis of autism and their teachers. The results from this study
active circle-time participation and attentional circle time behaviors. Danko concluded
that the addition of visual supports and props during circle time enhanced child
48
engagement. Although not specific to storybook reading, the results of this study support
the theory that the addition of visual supports can significantly improve the quantity and
(ALS) on the verbal output of children with autism spectrum disorder. ALS is based on a
milieu teaching approach in which facilitators incorporate the use of visual symbols into
their language output. For example, a facilitator might say “Let’s open the book” while
pointing to picture symbols that represent “open” and “book” (Beukelman & Mirenda,
1998). ALS incorporates modeling as the primary method of teaching and encourages
children to actively engage with their communication systems for the purpose of
increasing the child’s active participation and engagement. The results of Dexter’s study
indicated that the children in his study benefited from the incorporation of visual symbols
during storybook reading. Five of the six children demonstrated observable gains in their
communication use.
The specific use of toy props during storybook reading has been highlighted in the
applied literature as one method for facilitating and promoting child engagement and
Musselwhite & King-DeBaun, 1997) have recommended the use of toy props during
However, despite these recommendations, there is no known study that has empirically
49
reading. Although not specifically focused on incorporating toys during storybook
reading, the previously cited study by Kaderavek and Sulzby (1998a) did examine the
during toy play with a caregiver. This study involved 10 children identified as typically
developing and 10 children meeting the criteria for specific language impairment (SLI).
The results indicated that all of the typically developing children demonstrated high
interest in storybook reading whereas only 60% of the children with SLI showed high
interest and the remaining 40% demonstrated decreased interest in such activities.
Moreover, of particular significance was the finding that all of the children with SLI
interactions involving storybook reading. The authors indicated that these results suggest
that a general low level of engagement was not the primary factor contributing to the
differences between the groups. More particularly, these children demonstrated low
engagement specific to storybook reading. However, Kaderavek and Sulzby did not
explore whether the toys could be used to enhance child engagement during the shared
book reading interactions. The results of this study coupled with the recommendations
for inclusion of toy props during storybook reading warrant further research examining
the potential benefits of toy props during storybook reading with caregivers and their
In summary, storybook reading with young children has been linked to positive
language and literacy outcomes. A child’s interest and engagement during storybook
reading has been suggested to play a critical role in the success of these types of
50
interactions. Storybook reading interest and engagement has been found to correlate with
later language and literacy development as well as to be predictive of the frequency with
which a child engages in independent reading during the early school years. However,
very few studies have attempted to examine potential strategies aimed at fostering child
interest and engagement during storybook reading interactions between caregivers and
their very young children. The current study aimed to examine the addition of toy props
to storybook reading interactions and their influence on the interest and engagement of
toddlers 18 to 27 months of age. Because the majority of the storybook literature focuses
on preschool age children and older, this study provides important initial information that
and their very young children as well as strategies to facilitate a positive foundation of
interest and engagement in storybook reading during the early years of a child’s
development.
Research Questions
The purpose of this study was to determine whether the addition of toy props
interactions. In addition the study explored whether the addition of toy props had a
identified as “low interest” storybook reading partners. The research questions and
1. Does the addition of toy props during storybook interactions increase child
communicative acts (per minute) produced by the child during the storybook interaction?
51
It was hypothesized that the addition of toy props would positively affect child
2. Does the addition of toy props during storybook interactions increase the total time (in
seconds) that the caregiver and child participate in the book reading interaction?
It was hypothesized that the addition of toy props would result in an increase in
the total amount of time (in seconds) the caregiver and child participated in the
storybook interaction.
3. Does the addition of toy props during storybook interactions alter the balance of
It was hypothesized that the addition of toy props during storybook interactions
would significantly shift the balance of participation between the caregiver and
the child).
4. Does the addition of toy props during storybook interactions increase child
It was hypothesized that the addition of toy props would have a positive effect on
5. Does the addition of toy props during storybook interactions have a differential effect
52
It was hypothesized that both groups of children would benefit from the addition
of toy props; however, it was expected that the benefits would be significantly
greater for the children identified as “low interest” partners compared to their
53
CHAPTER 3
METHODS
The purpose of the current study was to determine whether the addition of toy
Additionally, the current study aimed to identify whether a differential effect of toy prop
use would be seen for children identified by the caregivers as demonstrating high
reading. The section that follows will detail the methods used in the current study.
Participants
investigations when power is set at .80. Using standard conventions, alpha was set at .05.
Given the expectation that the addition of toy props would significantly increase the
amount of child and adult behaviors produced during the storybook interactions, it was
expected that there would be large differences between the storybook conditions (props
vs. no props). Therefore, the analysis for sample size was based on an expectation of a
Another consideration in the power analysis was the degree to which the measures
would be correlated across storybook conditions. There is some indication from previous
research (Anderson-Yockel & Haynes, 1994; Hayden, Reese, & Fivush, 1996; Haynes &
Saunders, 1998; Justice & Kaderavek, 2003; Reese, Cox, Harte, & McAnally, 2003) that
the correlations among measures would be moderate to high. Assuming there would be a
moderate correlation (.50) among the measures across storybook conditions, the sample
size needed would be 13 participants per group. Assuming there would be a high
correlation (.80) among the measures across storybook conditions, the sample size
needed would be 8 participants per group. Thus, it was estimated that a sample size
between 8-13 participants per group would be adequate for the investigation.
Child Participants. All of the children met the following criteria: a) between the
ages of 18 and 27 months of age at the time the storybook readings took place (M= 21.7
months; SD= 2.61), b) no parental concerns regarding hearing status, and c) developing
Materials section). All of the children lived at home with both caregivers. Twelve
children were the family’s only child, 9 children had one sibling, and 3 children had two
siblings. Ten were cared for in the home, whereas 10 were in childcare outside of the
home approximately 10-30 hours per week, and 4 children were in childcare for over 30
None of the families reported any current concerns regarding their child’s hearing.
Twelve of the children had ear infections in the last year with only 8 of these children
reportedly having an ear infection in the last 6 months. Of the twelve families reporting
frequent ear infections in the past, 6 of the children reportedly recovered with completion
of a full round of antibiotics and 6 children received P.E. tubes followed by complete
recovery.
55
In an effort to document that each child in the study was within normal limits for
speech and language development, three standardized measures were administered during
the first home visit. These measures are described in detail in the Materials section. The
standard score on the Auditory Comprehension subtest of the PLS-4 was 111 (SD = 11.2)
with a range from 81-127 and the mean standard score on the Expressive Communication
subtest was 114 (SD = 11.6) with a range from 91-147. The MacArthur-Bates
development. The raw scores on the CDI ranged from 26-570 (M = 221; SD = 174.6)
and the percentile rankings ranged from the 20th -95th (M = 51st; SD = 25).
None of the caregivers reported any concerns regarding their child’s overall
development. In order to ensure that all of the children participating in the current study
were within normal limits for overall development, the age-appropriate Ages and Stages
Questionnaire (ASQ; Squires, Potter, & Bricker, 1999) was completed by each caregiver.
development in five areas: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, and
personal-social. All of the children were within normal limits on all subtests with the
solving area of development and one child exhibited a borderline delay in fine motor and
personal/social development and a moderate delay for gross motor development. These
56
four children were included in the sample because they were equally distributed among
the two engagement groups, were all within the normal range on the PLS-4, and were all
above the 20th percentile on the CDI. The scores for these four children are presented in
Table 3.1 with each child’s score in the delayed area, the cutoff score provided by the
ASQ system, respective PLS-4 standard score for both the receptive and expressive
Table 3.1 Individual Scores for Four Children Demonstrating Delays on the ASQ
Personal/Social 35 35
Gross Motor 15 35
mothers except for two children whose fathers completed the study (one in each
surrounding small towns. All dyads were Caucasian with the exception of three families.
Of these three, one family was Asian, one family was Hispanic, and one family was of
mixed ethnicity (African-American and Caucasian). All of the caregivers had completed
at least some college, with the majority of caregivers reported the completion of a college
57
professional degree (n = 7). Nine of the families earned an average family income of
$50-80,000 annually and 11 families earned over $100,000 annually. Four families
averaged less than $40,000 annually, but three of these families included at least one
Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b) completed at the onset of the study
(details in Materials section). All of the dyads were matched on child age (within 30
days of each other) and caregiver annual income and education level. Nine of the dyads
were matched on gender. The remaining three dyads consisted of male/female matches
with the female participant being in the high interest group for each dyad. Education
level was defined by highest level of schooling completed by the participating caregiver.
Family annual income level was estimated to the nearest $5,000 increment. These
definitions were modified from criteria used by DeBaryshe, (1995b) and Goodwyn,
Acredolo, & Brown, (2000). See Table 3.2 for participant matching descriptive
information.
58
Table 3.2 Matching Criteria for High and Low Engagement Groups
High ICLQ1 Age2 Income3 ED4 Sex Child Low Grp ICLQ Age Income ED Sex Child
Grp Care5 Care
1
Average Rating on Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire (range 1-5);
2
Age in months; days;
3
Annual Family income 2 = 25–29,999, 3 = 30-34,999, 4 = 35,000-39,000, 7 = 50-54,999, 9 = 60–64,999, 11 = 70-74,999,
12 = 75-79,999, 13 = 80-84,999, 17 = over 100,000
4
Education level: 3 = some college, 4 = College Graduate, 5 = Graduate or Professional School;
5
H = Home, D = Childcare Center
Table 3.3 provides the range of scores on the language assessments with the mean and
standard deviation for the two engagement groups. The groups do not differ significantly for the
child’s age in months or the average score on the auditory comprehension subtest of the PLS-4.
As expected, the two groups differ significantly (p. < 05) on their ratings on the Individual Child
Literacy questionnaire. However, unexpected group differences were also noted for expressive
language abilities with the high engagement group performing higher on both the expressive
MacArthur-Bates CDI4 –
(Low ) Raw Score* 26-288 129.5 96.1
Percentile* 20-50th
(High) Raw score 47-570 312 190.7
Percentile 30-95th
1
Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire, 2Preschool Language Scale – 4 Auditory
Comprehension Subtest, 3Preschool Language Scale– Expressive Communication Subtest,
4
MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories – Words and Sentences
* Significant Group differences (p =< .05)
Procedures
Recruitment. Recruitment took place over three phases. In Phase I, local childcare
program directors within the Raleigh/Durham/Chapel Hill, North Carolina area were contacted to
60
participate in the recruitment phase of the study. A general description of the study was
presented to each facility director and program consent was obtained. Each consenting childcare
director was asked to identify classroom childcare providers at their facility who serve children
within the study age range (18-27 months). On a day and time convenient to the childcare
director, the researcher arranged a brief meeting with each childcare teacher to explain the
purpose of the study and provide a brief description of family involvement. Teachers who
consented to distribute the study description letter also agreed to collect the signed consent forms
for the researcher. Letters providing a description of the study and requirements for
participation, a Caregiver-Child Consent Form and a Contact Information Form were sent home
with each child in the classroom who met the age criteria (See Appendix A-C). The researcher
returned to each facility at least two times following the letters being sent home to collect signed
consent forms and provide a brief reminder flyer for each child who had not yet returned a
consent form. The reminder flyer provided a brief description of the study and an email address
In phase II, the researcher contacted by phone each family who returned a consent form
to discuss the details of the study and make arrangements to send the family an Individual Child
Literacy Questionnaire (Appendix D; described in Materials section). Within one week, the
researcher either dropped off an Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b) and self-
addressed stamped envelop for each consenting family at the child’s childcare center or mailed
the questionnaire and envelop directly to the family. In the case where the questionnaire was
dropped off at the childcare center, all childcare providers provided verbal consent for the
researcher to send the information home with each child whose caregiver returned a consent
form. Caregivers were asked to complete the form and return it to the researcher at their earliest
61
convenience. The Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire requested information regarding the
child’s literacy attitudes and behaviors and was used to determine group membership (high vs.
low engagement).
In Phase III of recruitment, all of the information collected thus far was compiled in order
to identify 12 caregiver-child dyads for both the “high engagement” and “low engagement”
participant groups. The first 12 participants whose average score on the Individual Child
Literacy Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b) was less than or equal to 3.5 were included in the “low
engagement” participant group. The first 12 participants whose average score on the Individual
Child Literacy Questionnaire was greater than or equal to 3.75 were included in the “high
engagement” participant group. Toward the end of the recruitment phase only children and
families who met the inclusion criteria and could be matched with a dyad whose data had already
All dyads that consented to participate but were not included in the study were notified
via email or phone and thanked for their willingness to participate. Reasons for exclusion from
the study were as follows: dyad did not meet the storybook engagement rating cut-off required
for inclusion, the researcher was unable to recruit a dyad match (based on child age or gender,
caregiver education level and/or annual family income), dyad was not among the first 12 dyads
to meet criteria and complete the data collection process, and/or dyad did not meet the age
inclusion requirement.
A total of 12 dyads were recruited from local childcare facilities. The remaining 12
dyads were recruited through word of mouth through other participants in the study as well as
friends/colleagues of the researcher who work with families of young children. The recruitment
62
process was exactly the same for these families with the exception of the method of initial
contact.
Pre-testing and Observations. Once group membership was determined, each caregiver
was then contacted to set up appointments to complete the home portion of the study. Caregivers
identified two time periods within a two-week time frame in which the researcher came to their
home to complete a set of forms and questionnaires, administer the child language assessment,
and observe and videotape the caregiver and child engaging in a “favorite” activity and reading
During the initial phone contact, the researcher asked the caregiver to identify an activity
that was one of the child’s “favorites”. The criteria defining a “favorite” activity were as
follows: 1) an activity in which both the caregiver and child typically engaged in together, 2) an
activity that the caregiver was willing to allow the researcher to video tape for approximately 3-5
minutes, 3) an activity that took place indoors, and 4) an activity that did not involve gross motor
movements (e.g., running around the house, climbing on furniture). To aid caregivers in
choosing an activity, the researcher suggested using a toy that the child was interested and easily
engaged in such as play doh, puzzles, cars, trains, or pretend play. Caregivers were asked to
choose something other than storybook reading. Dyads engaged in a variety of play activities
including block play, ball play, puzzles, and simple pretend play with various dolls, small people
During the first home visit, each caregiver completed the following questionnaires
(2) Caregiver Literacy Questionnaire (Appendix F), (3) Storybook Familiarity Rating Form
(Appendix G), (4) Child Hearing Status Form (Appendix H), (5) Caregiver-Child Activity
63
Questionnaire (Appendix I), (6) MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory -
Words and Sentences Form (Fenson et al., 1992), and (7) Ages and Stages Questionnaire
(Squires et al., 1999). In addition, the researcher administered the Preschool Language Scale– 4
(PLS-4; Zimmerman et al., 2002) and videotaped the dyad engaging in a “favorite” shared
activity for approximately 3-5 minutes. At the end of the first home visit, caregivers were asked
During the second home visit, all dyads participated in two videotaped storybook
interactions. One storybook interaction included toy props and the other included the book only.
The order of book conditions was counterbalanced across participants with the exception of one
dyad in the “low engagement” group and two dyads in the “high engagement” group. For the
dyad in the low engagement group, the child protested the initial no prop storybook condition
and therefore the caregiver was instructed to read the book with props first and then read the
book without props second. The two dyads in the high engagement group were intended to be in
the prop book condition first, but the caregivers read the books without incorporating the props at
all and were therefore requested to read the second book with the props. All storybook and
“favorite” activity interactions took place in the dyad’s home in a room of the caregiver’s choice
that allowed for adequate space and lighting for the video-recording. The preferred choice for
most dyads was the living room although one dyad chose to read in the child’s bedroom, another
chose to read in the family playroom, and a third chose to sit on the back deck outside. Dyads
Two cameras were utilized in order to capture the entire scene. The first camera, aimed
at capturing the frontal view, was set up on a tripod directly in front and at eye level to the dyad
(approximately 4-6 feet away depending on the available space). The second camera, used to
64
capture the dyad’s nonverbal behaviors, was also set up on a tripod either to the side or behind
the dyad (depending on the location of the dyad and the setup of the room). Both cameras were
monitored by the researcher throughout the interactions in order to make necessary adjustments.
For the no-prop storybook condition, caregivers were provided with a book only and
instructed to read the storybook as they typically would do. For the storybook condition that
included toy props, the dyad was presented with four or five toy props (depending on the book)
relevant to the storyline. Each book contained the same toy pig and chicken whereas the other
toys changed depending on which book was read with props (plastic balloons and flowers for the
“Friends” book and a toy Velcro apple, one small plastic knife, and a plastic plate for the
“Apple” book). Caregivers were encouraged to incorporate the toys into the storybook
interaction using the following instructions and examples: “For this storybook, it is very
important that you attempt to use the toy props during your book reading interaction. For
example, you may make a noise for an animal and pretend it’s walking across the book. You
might also reenact a part of the story using the toys. You are welcome to use the toys in anyway
you feel comfortable but please remember to try to use the toys in some way while you are
reading and looking at the book.” At the end of the second home visit, caregivers were asked to
whether the storybook readings were similar to or different from the caregiver and child’s typical
book reading interactions. The first home visit lasted approximately 60 minutes and the second
Study Materials
before the storybook reading sessions (See Appendix A – I for copies of each form). Most
65
families were able to complete all of the forms and questionnaires during the first home visit.
However, if a caregiver chose to not complete the forms and instead watched the child
assessment or did not have enough time to complete everything, the forms were left with the
caregiver and collected during the second home visit. The specific forms and questionnaires
included:
number as well as the participating child’s name, sex, and date of birth.
child’s familiarity with 8 storybooks using a Likert scale from 1-5 with a rating of
5 being “very familiar”. Additional qualifiers for each rating were provided on
the form. For this study, unfamiliar was defined as “never seen nor read before”
and familiar was defined as “having read the book with the child on many
occasions”.
6) Child Hearing Status Form –child’s current hearing status and history of ear
infections.
66
together, and the caregiver’s estimate of the amount of time (per episode) the
8) Post “Favorite” Activity Rating Form (Roy, 2006) - caregiver’s rating of the
“favorite” activity interaction with the child, based on how typical the interaction
questions regarding the child’s behaviors during the “favorite” activity. The rating
form was completed by each caregiver at the end of the first home visit.
9) Post Storybook Interaction Rating Form (Roy, 2006) –caregiver’s ratings of each
everyday storybook interactions with the child. This form includes a total of 8
questions regarding the child’s behaviors and attitudes. The rating form was
10) Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ; Squires et al., 1999). Caregivers
completed the ASQ in order to document the developmental status of each child.
The ASQ questionnaire system is divided into intervals ranging from 4-60 months
of age. Caregivers were provided with the appropriate age interval questionnaire
divided into five subtests (communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem
child’s natural environment and were encouraged to attempt to elicit any response
that they were unsure about whether the child could make. The caregiver
indicated “yes,” “sometimes,” or “not yet” for each question. As indicated in the
67
scored as five points, and a “not yet” response is scored as zero. A total for each
subtest is calculated by adding the score for each question; the maximum score
for each subtest is 60 points. The composite score in the ASQ reflects the child’s
average score across the five subtests. The ASQ manual provides different cutoff
points for typical developmental status for each age range examined. For most
1992) - The CDI questionnaire (Words and Sentences Form) is a caregiver report
children age 16-30 months. The Words and Sentences form has been proven to
between 16-30 months of age (Dale, Bates, Reznick, & Morisett, 1989; Fenson et
al., 1994). This form consists of 680 words, divided into 22 categories that
include the following: sound effects and animal sounds, animals, vehicles, toys,
food, clothing, body parts, household items, furniture and rooms, outside things,
places to go, people, games and routines, action words, descriptive words, words
about time, pronouns, question words, prepositions and locations, quantifiers and
articles, helping words, and connecting words. The CDI also includes a section
for how children use words and a section on sentences and grammar that includes
al., 2002) - The PLS-4 assesses the receptive and expressive language skills of children birth to 7
68
years of age. It was standardized on over 1500 children representative of the U.S. population and
on over 300 children within the age range included in the current investigation. Acceptable
inter-rater, test-retest, and internal consistency reliability levels and concurrent validity are
reported in the test manual. The PLS-4 assesses a relatively brief but balanced sample of
language behaviors. The PLS-4 was administered by the primary researcher who is also a
licensed speech-language pathologist in the state of North Carolina. The PLS-4 was
utilizing a variety of sources (e.g., librarian and other professional recommendations, children’s
book publisher recommendations, leveled book catalogs and recommended book lists, previous
research, and personal judgment). An attempt was made to identify books that were less well-
known to account for potential variability of exposure across dyads, as well as identify books
that were a part of a “series” so that some of the toy props could remain the same across the two
During the initial pilot testing, two books, Pepo and Lolo and the Red Apple (Larranaga,
2004a) and Pepo and Lolo are Friends (Larranaga, 2004b), were identified from the original 8
and determined to be “very unfamiliar”. To verify that each child had not had any previous
exposure to these books, a familiarity rating was completed by each caregiver during the first
home visit using the researcher developed Storybook Familiarity Rating Form (Appendix G).
Both books were rated by all caregivers as “very unfamiliar”. Table 3.4 provides descriptive
69
Table 3.4 Descriptive Information for the Two Storybooks
In order to address the research questions, the following behaviors and measures were
engagement during the “favorite” activity was to verify that membership in the “low
engagement” group was based on low engagement specific to storybook reading and not a more
score was calculated utilizing a 5-point Likert scale modified from Kaderavek and Sulzby
(1998a). The subjective rating involved an interval rating system where a subjective rating was
made at each 15sec interval of the Total Interaction Time (defined below) for both interaction
contexts (storybook and “favorite” activity). All ratings were averaged across the Total
Interaction Time and a rating between 1 and 5 was assigned to each interaction. See Appendix L
for additional details regarding the rating criteria and examples of possible engagement and
disengagement behaviors.
Total Interaction Time. During the storybook interactions, coding of the interaction
began when the caregiver made an attempt to engage the child in the book or when the caregiver
or child acknowledged the book (e.g., opened book, made a comment related to the book).
70
Because some caregivers tended to play with the toys first without reference or notice of the
book, interaction time did not start at the first mention of the toys or when either the child or
adult engaged with the toys in some way. The interaction was considered terminated when the
caregiver or child closed the book and/or indicated in some way (verbally or non-verbally) that
the book interaction was over (regardless of whether they continued to play with the toys).
For the “favorite” activity, coding of the interaction began when the caregiver and/or
child made a verbal comment or nonverbal gesture/action initiating the activity. Coding was
terminated after the dyad had participated in the “favorite” activity for at least five minutes.
Child Communicative Acts. The following definitions were used to identify a child
communicative act. These measures were coded from the caregiver-child storybook interactions
and were utilized in order to calculate the proportion of each type of communicative act, the
child’s rate of communicative acts, and total number of communicative acts based on the Total
Interaction Time. These behaviors were modified from the child behaviors coded in Anderson-
3b) Response to adult request for information/action – any child response (i.e., verbal,
contrast, responses to caregiver’s attempts to gain the child’s attention which were
followed by the child visually and/or physically orienting toward the caregiver were not
coded as responses (Ex. Mom: “Look, at this.” Child – turns to look at the book.).
3c) Gesture - any spontaneous gesture or imitation of an adult’s gesture such as pointing
and acting on the book (e.g., turning the page, patting the book).
71
Only child behaviors that were intentional in nature and served a communicative function were
coded. The following definition was utilized in determining if a behavior was intentional and
communicative:
aware of the effect that a signal will have on his listener and persists in that behavior until
the effect is obtained or failure is clearly indicated” (Bates, 1979; p36). Expectancy of a
1992).
Child Protesting Acts. The total number of child behaviors that were in protest to the
storybook interaction was calculated. Protesting behaviors included crying, whining, pushing the
book away, attempting to leave the interaction, squirming to leave the interaction (regardless of
whether the attempt to leave was successful), and throwing the book. Crying and whining were
coded as one protesting behavior unless separated by a period of 5 seconds or more of no crying.
Statements from the child such as “No book” or comments about being “all done” with the book
prior to completion of the story were also coded as child protesting behaviors. Attempting to
close the book towards the end of the interaction but prior to finishing the book was not coded as
a protesting behavior unless accompanied by some negative indication that the child was trying
behaviors during the Total Interaction Time that were vocal, verbal, gestural, and/or a
72
Rate of Child Communicative Act (ROC). The rate of child communicative acts was
measured by the mean number of communicative acts displayed per minute for the Total
system described by Tannock (1988) and McDonnell & Friel-Patti (2003) was utilized.
Conversational control refers to the speaker’s tendency to direct the flow of extratextual
interactions (McDonnell et al., 2003). This measure was included in order to determine the
nature of participation of the caregiver and child. The following 3 measures were calculated from
6a. Total number of child turns per interaction – total number of child turns within each
interaction.
6b. Total number of adult turns per interaction - total number of adult turns within each
interaction.
The following definitions were utilized in determining the three measures of conversational
individual that are not separated by a communicative act from the partner nor by a pause
the termination of the partner’s communicative turn during which the individual could,
73
c) An initiation is defined as any instance where the speaker leads the interaction,
including the introduction of a new topic, an attempt to elicit completion of text, initiation
of routines such as counting, and maintenance of a topic with no intervening turn by the
d) A response is defined as any response to the speaker with the same topic, completion
Caregiver Prop Use. In order to determine the caregivers’ use of the toy props, the
following behaviors were coded from the Total Interaction Time. The total number of prop use
a) Labels toy prop – Caregiver provides a name for the prop (e.g., This is a bear, an
b) Animates the toy prop - Caregiver makes a noise for a toy or makes the toy do an
action (e.g., makes the pig jump, makes a noise for the chicken)
c) Relational toy prop use – Caregiver relates the toy prop to another toy, the book, the
child, or themselves (e.g., makes the pig eat the apple in the book, makes the pig jump on
d) Requests - Caregiver requests information or an action from the child (e.g., “Give the
piggy a bite.” “You cut the apple.” “Can you find the chickie.”). Attention getters such
as “look,” “See,” and ‘lookit” were not viewed as active requests for the child to
e) General prop use – Any prop use that could not be coded in the first four categories
but was clearly an attempt by the caregiver to include the props in the storybook
74
interaction (e.g., standing the pig or chicken up on its feet, squeezing the chick or pig to
f) Other – Narrating the child’s toy prop use and making a direct connection between the
toys and the book were also coded as prop use behaviors (e.g., “You are cutting that
Data Management
connecting the camera through a firewire connection) and compressed into computer format in
order to aide transcription and coding. All storybook and “favorite” activity interactions for each
dyad were copied onto an external hard drive due to the size of each file. For each storybook
reading interaction video file, the audio track was extracted using QuickTime movie software.
The audio files were then transcribed using Transcriber Software. Transcriber is free,
downloadable software that is user friendly and allowed for ease in transcribing the linguistic
data for the study. All interactions were transcribed verbatim using the Transcriber Software and
later exported into the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts software (SALT; Miller &
Chapman, 2004). SALT was utilized to facilitate and check coding of verbal and nonverbal
behaviors. Transcription followed the guidelines outlined by Miller and Chapman (2004).
Video interactions were viewed in order to code non-verbal behaviors as well as to check
transcription.
Inter-rater Reliability
The primary researcher coded the entire data set independently and three research
assistants were recruited and trained to code different aspects of the coding system for reliability
purposes. One research assistant was trained by the primary researcher to use the Transcriber
75
Software, transcribe the storybook interactions, and code the caregiver and child turns and child
responses. The second research assistant was trained to rate both the storybook reading clips and
the favorite activity clips using the subjective engagement scale. This researcher was also
trained to calculate the total interaction times for the storybook reading interactions. The third
research assistant was trained to code the caregiver prop use behaviors.
Each research assistant met with the primary researcher individually on at least two
occasions. During these meetings, the rules for coding were discussed and a sample video clip of
a child not included in the study was coded by the researchers. After the initial training session
was completed, the research assistant independently coded an additional practice clip for their
respective behaviors and compared their results to the primary researcher’s original coding. This
process was continued until the research assistant and primary researcher were in agreement for
at least 80% of possible agreements during training. During the initial training period, the
original coding system was modified as necessary based on disagreements between coders until a
consensus was met and final criteria determined. Once the criteria was determined, all data were
Interrater agreement estimates were calculated for 12 (25%; 6 with and 6 without props)
video clips randomly selected from the 48 total storybook reading video clips collected, with the
exception of the caregiver prop use codes which were only coded for the 6 of the 24 storybook
clips that included the toy props. Interrater reliability was also calculated for 4 (16%) of the
favorite activity video clips for the subjective rating of engagement only.
A point by point agreement was calculated to determine reliability for each coded
behavior by dividing the total number of agreements by the total number of agreements plus
disagreements and multiplying by 100 to gain a percent. For the child and caregiver turns, there
76
were 837 (84.3%) agreements out of a total 993 possible agreements. For child responses, there
were 34 (92%) agreements out of a total 37 possible agreements and for caregiver total prop use,
there were 47 (90.4%) agreements out of a total 52 possible agreements. For the total interaction
time, there was a total of 1612 seconds in agreement between the two coders out of a total 1674
Coding for the subjective ratings of engagement involved a slightly more complex
process. Because of the subjective nature of the engagement rating scale, it was determined that
all of the video clips would be independently coded by both the primary researcher and the
research assistant trained to code the engagement ratings. An initial set of criteria was
established prior to coding any data included in the current study. These criteria were based on
previous research and a sub-sample of pilot data collected at the onset of the study. All the
storybook clips were rated by the primary researcher with modifications made to the original
criteria based on any segments that could not be clearly rated given the original criteria
descriptions. This iterative process continued until the entire data set had been rated by the
primary researcher. Next, the research assistant trained to code the subjective ratings of
engagement coded a set of practice video clips that were not included in the final data set. These
ratings were compared to the primary researcher’s ratings, with each disagreement being
discussed and a decision made based on the consensus of the two coders. At this point, the
primary researcher summed the decisions into a final set of coding definitions and both coders
rated all of the video clips independently. Twelve of these video clips were randomly selected to
For the storybook reading subjective ratings of engagement, there were 76 (72%) out of a
possible 104 agreements when calculated using exact agreements. The reliability increased to
77
101 (97%) agreements out of a possible 104 agreements when calculated for agreements within
one point of the other coder (See Appendix L for description of 5 point criterion rating scale).
For the subjective ratings of engagement during the favorite activity, there were 30 (74%)
agreements out of 39 possible agreements when determined for exact agreements and 39 (100%)
agreements when calculated using agreements within 1 point of the second coder.
78
CHAPTER IV
RESULTS
The results presented below focus on the preliminary analyses conducted to determine
whether group differences based on gender, book influence, prop order and interaction order
might have influenced any observed differences between the groups. Descriptive statistics are
then provided for the questionnaire data collected as part of the current study including
frequency counts for dyads in both the high and low engagement groups. Finally, the question of
whether the addition of toy props during storybook reading influenced caregiver-child
interactions is explored.
Gender. Due to the potential for gender differences and the fact that three pairs of dyads
were not matched based on the child’s gender, making the number of boys (n =15) included in
the study greater than the number of girls (n = 9), it was important to look for any differences
that may be attributed to possible gender influences. A series of independent t-tests were
conducted to determine if mean group differences were present based on gender. No significant
differences were identified between males and females based on age in months (t [22] = -1.378,
p=.182), standard scores on the receptive and expressive communication subtests of the PLS-4 (t
[22] = 1.867, p=.075 and t [22] = 1.082, p=.291, respectively), and raw scores and percentile
p=.281 and t [22] = 1.551, p=.135, respectively). Additionally, group differences were not found
between males and females on their average rating on the Individual Child Literacy
Questionnaire (t [22] = .462, p=.649) or their average subjective rating of engagement during the
favorite activity (t [22] = .899, p= .379). Based on these findings, gender was excluded from the
statistical model and was not assumed to play a significant role in the results reported in the
sections to follow.
Book Influence. In order to assess whether props had a differential effect across the two
books (Apple book vs. Friend Book), dyads were separated into two groups based on which book
they received with props. The two groups consisted of 13 dyads who read the Apple book with
props (7 High, 6 Low) and 11 dyads who read the Friends book with props (5 High, 6 Low). A
series of repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted using the following dependent variables
collected during the prop and no prop conditions: total interaction time, subjective rating of
engagement, total number of verbal acts, total number of gestures, total number of responses,
total number of caregiver turns, total number of protesting behaviors, total number of
communicative acts, rate of communicative acts per minute, and the ratio of caregiver to child
turns.
The results of these analyses did not reveal any statistically significant findings regarding
which book was read with props. However, a trend was identified for two dependent variables
(p = .08 and .07, respectively). Examination of the means and standard deviations revealed a
trend favoring the “Apple” book. Specifically, within the prop condition, dyads that read the
“Apple” book with props had longer interactions (M = 173, SD = 59.2) than dyads that read the
“Friends” book with props (M = 140, SD = 35.4). Within the no prop condition, dyads that read
the “Apple” book without props (M = 133.23, SD = 31.6) also tended to have longer interactions
than dyads who read the “Friends” book without props (M = 117, SD = 42.3). A similar trend
was identified for the total number of caregiver turns with caregivers who read the “Apple” book
80
with props taking more turns than caregivers who read the “Friends” book with props. However,
it should be noted that these differences did not reach significance at the .05 level.
Book Order. The order in which the dyad read each book, regardless of whether or not
the book was accompanied with props, was also examined to determine whether the quality of
the storybook interaction was dependent on it being the first or second reading interaction. The
data were reorganized to represent the dyad’s behaviors during their first book reading
interaction versus their second book reading interaction. There was no distinction made between
props versus no props for this set of analyses. Simple paired sample t-tests were conducted to
examine whether mean differences were present. The dependent variables that were examined
included: total interaction time, subjective rating of engagement, total number of verbal acts,
total number of gestures, total number of responses, total number of caregiver turns, total number
of protesting behaviors, total number of communicative acts, rate of communicative acts per
minute, and the ratio of caregiver to child utterances. The results indicated no significant
differences between any of the dependent variables when the data were categorized for the book
the dyad read during the first interaction versus the book they read during the second interaction.
Total Prop Use. Total prop use was calculated to determine whether there were
differences between the caregivers across engagement groups and their use of the toy props
during the prop condition book reading interaction. The total number of prop use behaviors for
the low engagement group was 255 (M = 21.3, SD = 15.5). The total number of prop use
behaviors for the high engagement group was 228 (M = 19, SD = 15.5). An independent samples
t-test was conducted to examine differences between the total number of prop use behaviors
between the caregivers in the high and low engagement groups. The results revealed no
significant differences between the groups, indicating both sets of caregivers tended to use the
81
toy props to the same degree (t [22] = .356, p= .725). It should be noted that limited prop use
was typical of a total of 9 caregivers (6 high, 3 low) who demonstrated less than 9 toy prop
behaviors (M = 5.8, SD = 1.7), whereas the remaining 15 caregivers used an overall mean of 28.7
prop behaviors (SD = 12.9). Of the 9 caregivers who demonstrated less than 9 total prop
behaviors, 4 received the props during their first book reading interaction and 5 received the
Prop Order. The order in which caregivers were provided props to use during their
storybook interactions was also examined to determine whether there was an effect of prop
presentation. For these analyses, group membership was based on whether the child received
props during the first or second storybook reading interaction. Despite careful planning and
counter-balancing, unequal dyads were represented in the conditions. The primary reasons
included the child protesting during the initial interaction without props and the parent
continuing on to attempt the book with props, and the caregiver initially not using the props
despite specific instructions to include them during the first book reading interaction so the prop
condition was attempted again during the second interaction. A total of 7 dyads in the low
engagement group and 4 dyads in the high engagement group read the book with props first, and
5 children in the low engagement group and 8 children in the high engagement group read the
book with props second. A series of repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted with the
following dependent variables during the prop and no prop conditions: total interaction time,
subjective rating of engagement, total number of verbal acts, total number of gestures, total
number of responses, total number of caregiver turns, total number of protesting behaviors, total
number of communicative acts, rate of communicative acts per minute, and the ratio of caregiver
to child utterances. Initially, the series of repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted with
82
dyads from both engagement groups combined into two groups: those reading the book with
props first (n = 11) and those reading the book with props second (n = 13).
The analyses revealed a significant main effect of prop order on the average subjective
rating of engagement (F [1,22] = 5.857, p=.02) and the rate of communicative acts per minute
independent-samples t-tests were conducted. Significant main effects of prop order were
observed for the subjective rating of engagement (t [22], = -2.551, p = .02) and the rate of
communicative acts per minute (t [22] = -2.056, p=.05) during the no prop condition only.
Specifically, children who received props first tended to have a lower subjective rating of
engagement (M = 3.54, SD = 1.1) and a lower rate of communicative acts per minute (M = 9.9,
SD 6.4) during the second book reading interaction without props when compared to children
who read the book without props first (subjective rating of engagement, M = 4.44, SD = .56; rate
differences between either group (props first vs. props second) during the prop condition with
both groups receiving relatively high average ratings of subjective engagement and producing
relatively similar rates of communicative acts. However, when children read the book with the
props first, their subsequent reading without props showed significantly lower subjective ratings
of engagement and lower rates of communication than when the no prop condition came first and
the props were second. Table 4.1 and 4.2 provide the means and standard deviations for the
average subjective rating of engagement and rate of communicative acts per minute during the
prop and no prop book reading condition for dyads receiving the props first and dyads receiving
props second.
83
Table 4.1 Means and Standard Deviations for Average Subjective Rating of Engagement
Table 4.2 Means and Standard Deviations for Rate of Communicative Acts per Minute
Since the dyads were divided into two groups based on when they read the book with
props and were not differentiated based on engagement group for these analyses, it is possible
that the uneven representation of dyads from each engagement group influenced these findings.
It is therefore informative to examine the descriptive statistics for each engagement group based
on when they read the book with props. Table 4.3 and 4.4 provide the means and standard
deviations for the average subjective rating of engagement and the overall rate of communicative
acts for the high and low engagement groups, respectively. Data are presented for the 7 dyads in
the low engagement group and the 4 dyads in the high engagement group who read the book
84
with props first, as well as data for the 5 dyads in the low engagement group and 8 dyads in the
high engagement group who read the book with props second.
Table 4.3 Means and Standard deviations for the Average Subjective Rating of Engagement
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group.
Recall that the overall trend for the group receiving the props first indicated a decrease in
subjective rating from the prop to no prop condition. Additionally, there were no statistically
significant differences identified between the two groups for either dependent variable during the
prop condition. When examining the descriptive statistics, the general trend for the dyads in
both engagement groups who received props first is similar to the overall trend for the groups
when they are combined favoring the prop condition. However, an opposite trend was identified
for the dyads in the low engagement group who received the props second compared to the
overall trend for the combined dyads from the two engagement groups who received props
85
second. For these 5 dyads in the low engagement group, a general decrease was noted in
subjective rating of engagement from the no prop condition to the prop condition. Specifically,
the average subjective rating of engagement for the 5 children in the low engagement group who
received props second decreased from 4.7 to 4.0 when props were added. This was not the case
for the 7 children in the low engagement group who received props first (4.4 props and 3.5 no
props) or both subgroups of children in the high engagement group (High/Props first = 3.9 props
and 3.6 no props; High/Props second = 4.6 props and 4.3 no props).
Similarly, when the descriptive data for the overall rate of communicative acts were
examined separating the two groups who received props first and props second into subgroups
based on their engagement group membership, differences were identified. Recall again that the
overall trend for the combined group receiving props first was a decrease from the prop
condition to the no prop condition. However, when subdivided, the high engagement group who
received props first actually demonstrated an increase from the prop to no prop condition.
Additionally, although the group statistics for the engagement groups combined indicated a
significant difference between the combined groups during the no prop condition only when they
received props first compared to when they received props second, the descriptive statistics
suggest that this difference may be primarily accounted for by the dyads in the high engagement
group. The average difference for the no prop condition between the high engagement dyads
receiving props first compared to the high engagement group receiving props second was 11.1,
whereas the difference for the low engagement group was only 5.1.
86
Table 4.4 Means and Standard Deviations for the Overall Rate of Communicative Acts during
the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group.
Caregiver Literacy Questionnaire. To collect data on the home literacy practices of the
dyads, a caregiver literacy questionnaire was completed by each participating caregiver. All but
one caregiver in the sample reported beginning to read to their child by the time the child was 12
months of age, with over half of the dyads starting to read at or before 6 months of age (n = 19).
All but three of the families reported reading to their children 6 or more times a week, with each
reading session lasting at least 3-5 minutes. All of the families except one reported having more
than 16 different children’s books in their home, with 12 families reporting more than 50
different children’s books in the home. All of the families reported having a variety of different
children’s literacy materials in their homes with most indicating that they had alphabet books (n
87
= 20), single word and sentence books (n = 22), interactive books (n = 22) and counting or
number books (n = 21). Most families also reported having adult literacy materials including
phone books (n = 21), cookbooks (n = 22), dictionaries or encyclopedias (n = 19), and adult
books (n = 22) and 12 families reported receiving a weekly or daily newspaper. Ten families
indicated that they visit both the library and bookstore at least once a month, whereas 14 families
determine approximately how long their child typically engaged in a range of play activities. All
of the children reportedly engaged in block play, with 20 children participating for at least 2-5
minutes. The same was true for outside play with 23 out of 24 children engaged in outside play
for over five minutes at a time, with one child in the low engagement group participating for
approximately 2-5 minutes. Twenty-one children engaged in toy play for at least 2-5 minutes,
with one child in the high engagement group engaging in toy play for 1-2 minutes and 2 children
in the low engagement group engaging in toy play for less than 1 minute at a time. Eighteen
caregivers reported that their child participated in some type of pretend play for at least 2-5
minutes, with 2 caregivers in the high engagement group and 4 caregivers in the low engagement
group reporting that their children were engaging in this type of play for less than 2 minutes.
Finally, in terms of amount of time spent in a single reading activity, all of the children
reportedly engaged in some type of storybook reading, with the majority of children engaging for
at least 2-5 minutes at a time (n =23) and only one child (in the low engagement group) engaging
the “favorite” activity interaction in order to verify that group membership in the high and low
88
engagement groups was not related to the child’s overall ability to engage in sustained
there were differences between the high engagement and low engagement groups’ abilities to
engage during a preferred play activity. This test was not significant indicating that the two
groups were equally able to engage in a sustained interaction with a caregiver (t [22] = -1.132,
p= .27). Table 4.5 shows the range of scores for both engagement groups with group means and
standard deviations.
Table 4.5 Subjective Rating of Engagement By Group During the “Favorite” Activity
After completing the favorite activity, caregivers were asked to complete a four question
Post Favorite Activity Rating Form (Roy, 2006) regarding their perceptions of how typical the
play interaction was compared to other play interactions. The majority of caregivers rated the
child’s interest and engagement in the play interaction as “somewhat typical” or better (high
group n = 11, low group n =12) with only one caregiver rating the play interaction as “very
different”. The child in this dyad was in the high engagement group and the caregiver reported
that she is “usually more engaged in play, and leads the play experience.” A similar pattern was
noted for active involvement during the play interaction with the majority of the caregivers rating
their child’s active involvement as typical (high group n = 8, low group n = 7). There were three
caregivers of children in the low engagement group and five caregivers of children in the high
engagement group who rated their child as “less active than usual” in the play interaction with
89
one caregiver of a child in the low engagement group rating the child as “more active than
usual.” All caregivers rated their child as exhibiting typical alertness or better, with the
exception of two caregivers of children in the low engagement group who indicated that their
each storybook interaction was typical of previous book reading interactions were collected at
the end of the second home visit. Frequency counts indicated that the majority of caregivers
rated the prop book reading condition (20/24) and the no prop book reading condition (19/24) as
typical or better. Two caregivers in the low engagement group and two caregivers in the high
engagement group rated the prop condition interaction as “very different” from other book
reading interactions, and three caregivers in the low engagement group and two caregivers in the
high engagement group rated the no prop book reading interaction as “very different”. Of the
caregivers who perceived at least one of the book reading interactions to be different from
typical reading interactions, only two caregivers (one high engagement dyad and one low
engagement dyad) perceived both interactions to be very different from other book reading
interactions. The majority of caregivers perceived their child to be “interested and engaged”
during the prop book condition (20/24) with slightly fewer caregivers rating their child’s interest
and engagement during the no prop condition as typical or better (16/24). Of those caregivers
rating the interactions as different, three caregivers (one high, two low) perceived their child to
be less interested and engaged during both interactions. One caregiver in the high engagement
group perceived their child to be less engaged and interested only during the prop condition
interaction, whereas five caregivers (4 high, 1 low) perceived their child as less interested and
engaged only during the no prop condition. Similarly, most caregivers perceived their child’s
90
“active involvement” during the prop book reading condition (17/24) and the no prop condition
(15/24) as typical or better. Three caregivers (one high, two low) perceived their child to be less
active than usual during both book reading interactions, whereas 4 caregivers perceived their
child to be less active during the prop condition only and 6 caregivers perceived their child to be
less active during the no prop condition only. Finally, overall alertness was rated by all but two
caregivers as typical or better, with one child in each engagement group perceived to be less alert
than usual (i.e., sleepy). Table 4.6 provides the frequency of caregiver ratings of interactions
during the prop and no prop book reading conditions for the high and low engagement groups.
91
Table 4.6 Caregivers’ Ratings on the Post Storybook Rating Form
Low High
Prop Book Condition Group* Group*
How typical would you rate your child’s somewhat or very typical 10 10
storybook interaction? very different 2 2
How would you rate your child’s interest and typical interest or better 10 10
engagement in the book reading interaction? less interested & engaged 2 2
Low High
No Prop Book Condition Group Group
How typical would you rate your child’s Somewhat or very typical 10 9
storybook interaction? Very different 2 3
How would you rate your child’s interest and typical interest or better 9 7
engagement in the book reading interaction? less interested & engaged 3 5
Low High
Overall Impressions Group Group
caregivers and their very young children, a series of repeated-measures analysis of variance
design is useful when thinking about data in terms of a “three dimensional structure” (p 573).
The participants or n make up one dimension, the dependent variables make up another
dimension, and the different time points (in this case book reading conditions) make up the third
92
dimension. Analysis of variance is based on certain assumptions made about the population and
parameters related to the dependent variable. There are essentially two assumptions underlying
the analysis of variance for repeated measures designs: homogeneity of variance and normality
(Howell, 2002).
In order to determine whether the data collected for the current study met all the
assumptions, Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance was included in all of the analyses that
are presented below. There were no significant values identified across all analyses and
therefore the error variance for each dependent variable was considered to be equal. In regards to
normality, the values for skewness and kurtosis indicated that the data were not equally
distributed and therefore do not meet the assumption of normality. According to Howell (2002),
violations of the assumptions. Additionally, Howell notes that this is “especially true for the
normality assumption” (p 340). Therefore, given that each sample included an equal number of
participants and there were no missing data points, a violation of the normality assumption was
In examining the influence of toy props, the goal of the current study was twofold. First,
the researcher aimed to determine whether the addition of toy props during storybook reading
increased child communicative behaviors and positively impacted caregiver behaviors. Second,
the research aimed to identify whether the addition of toy props had a differential effect based on
whether children were identified by their caregivers as demonstrating high or low engagement
during shared storybook interactions. In the results that follow, the latter research aim was
examined as a part of each individual analysis conducted on the following dependent variables:
total child communicative acts, rate of communicative acts (per minute), total time (in seconds)
93
that the caregiver and child participated in the book reading interaction, the balance of
participation between the caregiver and child as measured by the ratio of caregiver turns to child
turns during the interaction time, and the subjective rating of engagement during storybook
interactions.
Total Child Communicative Acts. To examine the potential influence of toy props during
analysis of variance was conducted with engagement group as the between subjects factor (i.e.,
low versus high) and the book condition (i.e., props vs. no props) serving as the within subject
factor. For each child, the total number of communicative acts was calculated during both the
prop and no prop book reading conditions by summing the number of child verbal/vocal acts, the
number of child gestures, and the number of child responses during each book reading condition.
A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to investigate if a main effect for prop condition, a
main effect of engagement group, and/or a potential interaction between engagement group and
For the total number of child communicative acts, the ANOVA revealed no significant
main effects for prop condition (F [1,22] = .056, p=.815), or engagement group membership (F
was identified for the total communicative acts, F [1,22] = 4.32, p= .05). The children in the low
engagement group produced more total communicative acts in the prop condition (M = 36.5)
than in the no prop condition (M = 25.9). This was in opposition to the high engagement group
who produced fewer communicative acts during the prop condition (M = 27.3) than during the no
prop condition (M = 35.7). Table 4.7 provides group frequency counts, means and standard
94
Table 4.7 Means and Standard Deviations for Total Child Communicative Acts During the Prop
and No Prop Book Conditions
The results of the analysis of variance take into account group membership and prop
condition, but it is possible that the unequal number of dyads from each engagement group that
received props first and second confounded the results. There were 7 dyads in the low
engagement group who completed the prop condition first. When these dyads completed the
second reading without props, they experienced a decrease of 8.6 in their total communicative
acts. The 5 dyads in the low engagement group who completed the no prop condition first, did
not experience a decrease in that their total communicative acts remained relatively the same
during both reading interactions (props mean = 32.2; no props mean = 31.4). This suggests more
of a general prop condition effect for the low engagement dyads in that children produced, on
average, more total communicative acts during the prop condition than the no prop condition.
This was not the case for the dyads in the high engagement group who on average produced
more total communicative acts in their first reading interaction regardless of prop presentation.
This look at the descriptive data suggests that the significant group-by-prop condition interaction
was confounded by the unequal representation of the two engagement groups in the two prop
condition orders.
95
In order to determine whether the addition of toy props increased the number of each
different type of child communicative acts, total frequency counts for child verbal/vocal
communicative acts, child gestures, and child responses were calculated during both book
to determine whether group differences existed. The ANOVAs were not significant for any of
the analyses. However, there was a trend toward a differential effect of toy prop use on
storybook reading for children identified as demonstrating high vs. low engagement. The two
significance for the total child verbal/vocal acts (F [1,22] = 4.03, p = .057) and for the child’s
total gesture use (F [1,22] = 3.803, p = .064). Specifically, children in the low engagement
group tended to produce more verbal/vocal communicative acts and use more gestures during the
prop book reading condition then during the no prop condition. The opposite was found for
children identified as demonstrating high engagement during storybook reading. This group
tended to use fewer verbal/vocal acts and gestures during the prop book condition than the no
prop condition. Table 4.8 and 4.9 provide means and standard deviations for the two dependent
Table 4.8 Means and Standard Deviations for Child Verbal/Vocal Acts
96
Table 4.9 Means and Standard Deviations for Child Gesture Use
Again, given the uneven number of dyads in each engagement group who received the
book with props first versus those who received the props during their second interaction, the
differences identified in the group statistics may be confounded by the order of prop presentation
specific to the engagement group. A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to determine
whether a main effect of reading interaction was present. The results of this analysis indicate no
reading interaction order by engagement group interaction or main effect of engagement group
membership. However, a trend was identified in the data for the children in both engagement
groups to produce more verbal/vocal acts during their first reading interaction compared to their
second reading interaction regardless of prop presentation (F [1,22] = 3.935, p = .06). This was
not the case for gesture use or responses. Table 4.10 provides the means and standard deviations
for the high and low engagement group based on their first and second reading interaction
97
Table 4.10 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Verbal/Vocal Acts of the High and Low
Engagement Group During Their First and Second Book Reading Interaction.
In addition, if the data are examined further and the two engagement groups are divided
into subgroups based on when they received props (props first vs. props second), a difference is
noted for the 7 dyads in the low engagement group who received props first to produce more
verbal/vocal acts during their first interaction which included props than the second interaction
which did not include props (p = .06). Again, this is not the case for the entire low engagement
group as the 5 dyads in the low engagement group who received props during their second
reading interaction did not differ significantly when comparing the prop and no prop conditions.
The impact of the larger, props first group clearly influence the trend in descriptive data which
show an increase in number of verbal/vocal acts when props are present (mean for no props =
13.8, mean for props = 15.0). For the high engagement group, there are no significant
differences in the total number of verbal/vocal acts produced during the prop and no prop
reading conditions between the 4 dyads receiving props first and the 8 dyads receiving props
second. However, there seems to be a first vs. second reading interaction trend in that dyads in
the high engagement group produced more verbal acts during their first reading interaction vs.
their second reading interaction regardless of prop presentation. Table 4.11 provides the means
98
and standard deviations for the subgroups of children in each engagement group who received
Table 4.11 Means and Standard deviations for the Total Gesture Use During the Prop and No
Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group and Prop Order Presentation.
Overall Rate of Communicative Acts. A repeated measures ANOVA was also conducted
to examine the rate of child communicative acts per minute during the prop and no prop book
reading interactions. The rate of communicative acts was determined by dividing the total
number of communicative acts by the total interaction time for each book reading condition. The
results of the repeated-measures ANOVA did not reveal any significant differences. Table 4.12
provides group means and standard deviations for the rate of child communicative acts. As can
99
be seen, although the differences were not significant, the means parallel the results for other
variables.
Table 4.12 Means and Standard deviations for Overall Rate of Child Communicative Acts
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions.
In addition to the child’s total communicative acts and rate of communicative acts, rates
for each individual child’s communicative acts were calculated. Repeated-measures ANOVAs
were run for the rate of each type of communicative act (i.e., verbal/vocal acts, gestures, and
responses). For these analyses, the between subject factor was engagement group and the rate of
each communicative act during the prop and no prop book reading condition was the dependent
variables. Results indicated no significant findings for the rate of verbal/vocal child acts per
minute or the rate of child gesture use per minute. However, the ANOVA revealed a significant
main effect for engagement group on the rate of child responses (F [1,22] = 6.501, p = .018).
Using Cohen’s d to determine the magnitude of the effect, the effect size of engagement group
on the rate of child responses was d = .775. According to Cohen’s (1988) criteria, this is
between a medium (d =.50) and a large (d = .80) effect size. Children in the high engagement
group produced significantly more responses to the caregiver’s requests for information or action
than children in the low engagement group. There was no main effect of prop condition or
significant engagement group-by-prop condition interaction. Table 4.13 provides the group
100
Table 4.13 Means and Standard Deviations for Rate of Child Responses by Group and
Condition.
Again, given the unequal representation of dyads receiving props first vs. those who
received props second in the two engagement groups, additional examination of the descriptive
data is useful in understanding the results for the rate of each communicative act examined
above. When the data are examined for an overall book reading interaction effect, there are no
statistically significant differences between the dependent variables based on reading interaction
order and no significant differences were identified for the individual low and high engagement
groups when examined separately. However, it was noted that the descriptive data support the
trend identified thus far showing a higher rate of communicative acts during the first book
reading interaction compared to the second book reading interaction regardless of prop
presentation. This further examination of the data indicated a similar trend of an influence of the
first book reading interaction over the second book reading interaction with children in both
groups producing higher rates of communicative acts during their first reading interactions.
However, the individual subgroup of high engagement dyads did not follow this same trend.
Visual inspection of the data indicated that the children in the low group produced higher rates of
communicative acts during the first read and children in the high engagement group produced
101
Child Protesting Behaviors. Child protesting behaviors were also coded during both
book reading conditions. Child protesting was not considered in the original hypotheses
regarding the influence of toy props; however, after examining the data it was clear that this
measure was necessary to fully describe the book reading interactions. When examining the
descriptive data, there were 6 children who protested during the prop condition compared to 12
children who protested during the no prop condition. The range of total protesting behaviors
during the prop condition was 0 to 7 and the range of total protesting behaviors during the no
prop condition was 0 to 20. There were two children in the data set identified as outliers with
total protesting scores of 17 and 20 during the no prop condition. Interestingly, these children
[1,22] = 4.78, p = .04), but no main effect of engagement group or engagement group-by-prop
condition interaction. The main effect of prop condition revealed a medium effect size of .663
when calculated using Cohen’s d. Table 4.14 presents the mean and standard deviation for each
group indicating a significant effect of prop condition with children protesting significantly less
during the storybook interaction that included toy props than during the interaction that did not
include props.
102
Table 4.14 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Child Protesting Behaviors during the
Prop and No Prop Book Conditions
It is also interesting to note that when the data are examined further to consider the
potential influence of unequal representation of children from each engagement group in each
prop presentation condition, the children in the low engagement group present a very interesting
profile. Table 4.15 provides the means and standard deviations for the total protesting behaviors
during the prop and no prop book conditions by engagement group and prop order presentation.
When the group means were examined, the 7 children in the low engagement group who
received props first protested significantly more on average during the no prop condition. In
contrast, the five children who were in the no prop condition first produced fewer protesting
behaviors when compared to their prop condition interaction. This pattern was also observed for
the four children in the high engagement group who received props first. Therefore, children in
the low engagement group tended to produce more protesting behaviors during the second book
reading interaction regardless of prop presentation. The prop presentation along with reading
interaction order may have played a role for some of the children. As can be seen in the data,
children in the low engagement group protested more during the second reading interaction
regardless of prop presentation but the protesting was dramatically increased when the first
reading interaction included props. Additionally, a similar impact was noted for the high
103
engagement group with children protesting more when props were not included but this was
dramatically increased when the no prop condition was the second book reading interaction.
Table 4.15 Means and Standard deviations for the Total Protesting Behaviors During the Prop
and No Prop Book Conditions by Engagement Group and Prop Order Presentation.
Total Interaction Time. Total interaction times during both prop conditions were
The results indicated a main effect of prop condition for the total interaction time (F [1,22] =
6.89, p = .02), with dyads interacting for longer periods of time during the prop condition than
during the no prop condition, regardless of engagement group membership. The effect size for
the main effect of props was .713 indicating a medium to large effect size. Group total time in
seconds, means and standard deviations can be found in Table 4.16. There was no main effect of
104
Table 4.16 Means and Standard Deviations for the Total Interaction Time (in seconds) during
the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions
Given the unequal sample sizes of the subgroups of dyads (high vs. low receiving props
first vs. second), it was again considered important to examine the data to determine if these
potential confounds were impacting the current findings. The results indicated clear support for
a main effect of prop condition with individual means for the subgroups following the same
trend. Specifically, for both engagement groups, dyads on average spent more time in the book
interactions when props were included compared to the time spent in book interactions that did
not include toy props. Therefore, the data for the individual groups support the findings for the
Balance of Participation. The balance of participation between the caregiver and child
was calculated for both prop book reading conditions. The ratio was calculated by dividing the
total number of caregiver turns by the total number of child turns for each storybook interaction.
Group means and standard deviations are provided in Table 4.17. The repeated-measures
ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of prop condition (F [1,22] = 1.385, p = .252), no
significant main effect of engagement group (F [1,22] = 1.825, p .190) and no significant
engagement group-by-prop condition interaction (F [1,22] = 1.272, p = .272). The mean ratio
for caregiver to child turns during the prop condition for the low engagement group was 3:1.
105
This was found to be similar to the ratio for the low engagement group during the no prop
condition. Therefore the balance of participation was not impacted by the addition of toy props.
Although not statistically significant, when examining the mean ratio of caregiver-child
turns for the high engagement group, a different pattern was observed. The average ratio of
caregiver to child turns during the prop condition for the high engagement group was similar to
that of the low engagement group at 3:1. However, this ratio increased during the no prop
condition to 5:1 suggesting that caregivers were taking more responsibility for the interaction
during the no prop condition than during the prop condition. It should be noted that a visual
inspection of the descriptive data identified a large standard deviation for the high engagement
group during the no prop condition. This was due to one dyad in the high engagement group.
When this dyad was removed from the data set and the repeated-measures ANOVA re-run, the
mean for the high engagement group during the no prop condition lowered to 3.99 (SD = 3.2).
This is only slightly higher then the other groups but the standard deviation for this group
Table 4.17 Means and Standard Deviations for the Ratio of Caregiver Turns to Child Turns
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions.
Caregiver Participation. Caregiver turns were also coded in order to determine the
degree to which each caregiver participated in the storybook interactions. Utterances that
contained direct lines from the text were excluded from this code as it was assumed that this
106
would be relatively equal across groups. However, utterances that contained a repetition of the
text that was clearly functioning as the caregiver’s turn, apart from direct verbatim reading (e.g.,
used for emphasis or used in conjunction with a demonstration with the toy props), were
included as caregiver turns. Means and standard deviations for each group are provided in Table
4.18. A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no main effect of prop condition (F [1,22] = .450,
p = .509) and no main effect of engagement group (F [1,22] = .000, p =.989). This analysis also
indicating that the caregivers in both the high and low engagement group participated similarly
Table 4.18 Means and Standard Deviations for Total Caregiver Turns During the Prop and No
Prop Book Conditions
When the descriptive data were examined to determine whether the book reading
interaction order impacted the caregivers’ participation, the results indicated that there were no
statistically significant differences between the caregiver’s turns during their first book reading
interaction compared to their second book reading interaction. It should be noted, however, that
the descriptive means support the general trend in that caregivers in both engagement groups
tended to produce more turns during the first book reading interaction compared to the second
107
book reading interaction. However, when the engagement groups were examined individually,
Average Subjective Rating of Engagement. The average subjective rating of the child’s
engagement was used as the dependent variable. The means and standard deviations for each
group can be found in Table 4.19. The results of this analysis indicated there was no significant
main effect for prop condition (F [1,22] = 1.485, p=.236) and no significant main effect of
engagement group (F [1,22] = .147, p = .705). In addition, there was no interaction identified
between engagement group and prop condition (F [1,22] = .052, p=.821) during the storybook
readings. However, the individual means follow the trend in favor of the prop condition.
Table 4.19 Means and Standard Deviations for Average Subjective Ratings of Engagement
during the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions
Given the unequal representation of dyads in the high and low engagement group in the
book and prop conditions and the hypothesis that props would increase the child’s engagement
during storybook reading, it is necessary to consider potential confounds as they relate to the
average subjective rating of engagement. The descriptive data support a general trend for
children to demonstrate higher engagement during the first book reading interaction compared to
the second interaction. Table 4.20 provides the average subjective rating of engagement means
and standard deviations for the high and low engagement groups when the data are categorized
108
by the first and second book reading interaction regardless of prop presentation. As can be seen,
children in the low engagement group had a higher average rating of engagement during their
first reading interaction compared to their second book reading interaction with the children in
the high engagement group demonstrating relatively the same level of engagement during both
interactions. Paired sample t-tests were conducted to examine these differences further and
statistically significant differences were identified for the low engagement group only (t [11] =
2.781, p = .02). Specifically children in the low engagement group demonstrated an interaction
order trend in that they produced more engagement behaviors during their first reading
interaction regardless of prop presentation. However, prop presentation also presented an issue
for the low engagement group. Similar to the result reported for protesting behaviors, children
tended to have higher average ratings of engagement during their first read compared to their
second read, however the frequency of engagement behaviors was dramatically decreased if the
first reading interaction included props. Table 4.21 provides the descriptive data for the low
engagement group only with the groups subcategorized by whether or not props were presented
Statistically significant differences were not identified for the high engagement group
when the data were examined for an interaction order effect or a prop presentation effect. High
engagement children tended to receive higher ratings for engagement during the prop condition
regardless of which interaction it was (first vs. second). However, these differences did not
109
Table 4.20 Means and Standard Deviations for the Average Subjective Rating of Engagement of
the High and Low Engagement Groups Based on Their First and Second Reading Interaction
Table 4.21 Average Subjective Rating of Engagement Means and Standard Deviations for the
Low Engagement Group Only During the Prop and No Prop Book Conditions by Prop Order
Presentation.
110
CHAPTER V
DISCUSSION
Storybook reading has been considered an important means for fostering early literacy
skills and language development. When caregivers engage in storybook reading interactions
with their young children, they have the opportunity to interact positively and create pleasurable
experiences within the book reading context (Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002). The purpose
of this study was to examine the potential influence of the addition of toy props on the success of
storybook reading interactions with very young typically developing children. To the author’s
knowledge, there have not been any empirical studies that have examined the influence of toy
demonstrating difficulties with language and literacy as one method for facilitating storybook
reading interactions (King-DeBaun, 1990; Musselwhite, 1986). Given that storybook reading has
been shown to concurrently and predicatively relate to later language and literacy development
(Cipielewski & Stanovich, 1992; Moerk, 1985; Senechal et al., 1996; Snow & Goldfield, 1983)
and that the child’s engagement and interest is an important contributing factor to successful
storybook reading, the potential use of toy props to enhance engagement in storybook reading is
There were two main focal points for the current study: determining the influence of toy
props on caregiver and child behaviors during storybook reading and identifying any differential
effects that membership in the high or low engagement group may have had. It was
hypothesized that the addition of toy props would have a positive impact on shared book reading
interactions between caregivers and their young children. It was also hypothesized that the
addition of toy props would have a differential effect on children identified by their caregivers as
high engagement. The discussion that follows summarizes the current study findings, relates
them to previous research, and highlights implications for current practice. Study limitations
will also be addressed providing directions for future research in the area of storybook reading
The findings from the current study extend our knowledge base regarding different
strategies caregivers and professionals could use with typically developing children to initiate
and expand shared interactions around a storybook. This study also provides preliminary
evidence to support the use of toy props during shared book reading with very young children
and guides future research aimed at examining the potential benefits of toy props with children at
risk for experiencing difficulties in language and literacy development. Although current
literature is beginning to acknowledge the important role of the child’s active engagement and
examining specific ways to facilitate these interactions. The current study can be embedded
within the larger framework of efforts analyzing the factors that contribute to successful
storybook reading interactions and identifying those that may have the largest effect on child
engagement.
Total Interaction Time. The current study identified a potentially important influence of
toy props on caregiver-child interactions during storybook reading. A significant main effect
was identified for the total interaction time with dyads engaging for longer periods of time when
112
the props were present compared to when the props were not present. Given that there was a
medium to large effect size of the use of props, this finding is important in that it provides some
preliminary evidence of the benefits of toy prop use during storybook reading. For the
caregivers in this study, the addition of toy props appeared to provide a strategy for extending the
duration of their interactions with their children around a known context. For the children, the
benefits of extending the duration of the interactions may include increasing the amount of time
that they have to process the information and make important connections. The increased total
interaction time may also provide more opportunities for a mutual focus of attention thus
potentially facilitating episodes of joint attention which have been found to positively relate to
Extending the duration of storybook reading interactions may also prove beneficial by
facilitating the process of guided participation (Rogoff, 1990). One way that the addition of toy
props facilitated guided participation was to prompt episodes of pretend play. Many dyads were
observed to engage in simple pretend play with the toy props present in addition to the labeling
and questioning that were present in both the prop and no prop conditions. One mother in
particular took the opportunity to teach her child a new vocabulary word. When the child
pointed to some musical notes on the page and asked, “What’s this?” The mother replied,
“Notes…like lalalalala,” while pretending the toy pig was singing. The addition of toy props
may have provided a clearer connection for this child while also providing the mother with
additional resources to explain a particularly abstract concept for a child of this age. It is also
possible that the addition of toy props provided the dyads in the current study with an additional
topic for conversation and dialogue, which in turn, extended the length of time the dyads
113
As noted above, dyads often engaged in simple pretend play with the props, but the
nature of this play appeared to be influenced by the nature of the book and the props. Recall that
a trend in the data was identified for differences between the “Apple” and “Friends” books (p
=.079). Although not statistically significant at the .05 level, it was noted that dyads engaged for
longer periods of time when reading the “Apple” book compared to the “Friends” book. Perhaps
this is due to the fact that the storyline of the “Apple” book was better suited for interactions with
children of this age given the sequential nature of the plot (Pepo and Lolo find an apple, work
together to retrieve it, and then share the apple at the end). It may also be that this sequence was
more familiar and concrete and therefore better accommodated the addition of the props.
Conversely, the “Friends” storyline may have been considered more abstract by the caregivers
(the book begins by describing the things that make Pepo and Lolo friends, then Pepo and Lolo
get mad at each other when one loses their balloon but they make up in the end) and thus,
Additionally, it is possible that the Velcro apple included with the “Apple” book was
uniquely novel and engaging to most children. Many caregivers noted never before seeing this
type of toy. It is possible that the toy flowers and balloons used with the “Friends” book were
not perceived as novel or did not lend themselves as well to simple pretend play when compared
to the apple and knife included with the “Apple” book. The latter set of props lent themselves to
an “eating routine” familiar to all children and was possibly perceived by the caregivers as
Despite these differences, however, a main effect for prop condition was identified for
the total interaction time. This finding indicates that although dyads reading the “Apple” book
with props tended to engage in the interaction for longer periods of time compared to the dyads
114
reading the “Friends” book with props, children and caregivers spent more time overall engaged
in storybook reading interactions with the props present than when the props were not present
(regardless of which book was used). In other words, props always increased the total interaction
time, but props appear to extend the total time for some types of books more than others.
The addition of toy props might have also provided caregivers with an additional strategy
for scaffolding the book reading interaction that, in turn, extended their interaction time.
Observed scaffolding strategies included acting out a particular event in the story with the toy
props and simply talking about a particular page for a little longer when referencing the props.
caregivers’ abilities to scaffold interactions for their children within their current competency
level in order to facilitate success one level above where they are demonstrating independent
abilities. If an increase in the total time children remain engaged in a book reading interaction is
assumed to represent increased interests on the part of the child, then props might be seen as one
way to get children interested in storybook reading as well as a strategy to keep them engaged,
thus leading to longer interactions and more opportunities for scaffolding on the part of the
caregiver.
Baker et al (2001) suggested that child interest and motivation to engage in storybook
reading is fostered by pleasurable experiences during such interactions. The caregiver’s ability
to scaffold for the child and the child’s ability to benefit from guided interactions may contribute
to whether these experiences are interpreted by both communication partners as successful and
enjoyable. Sonnenschein and Munsterman (2002) added to this suggestion by noting that
allowing a child an active role in the interaction is also related to increases in later child
motivation and interest in engaging in book reading. The use of toy props during storybook
115
reading may provide children with another avenue to actively engage with their caregiver and
control the interaction thus extending the total time spent interacting around the storybook.
Child Protesting Behaviors. A main effect of prop condition for the rate of child
protesting behaviors was also identified, thus supporting the hypothesis of a positive impact of
toy props on storybook reading. Recall that there was not a main effect of order of prop
presentation on the total protesting behaviors when the children were combined from the two
engagement groups (i.e., all children receiving props first versus all children receiving props
second). Children demonstrated a general increase in protesting behaviors during the no prop
condition compared to the prop condition, regardless of the order in which the props were
presented. This finding was in the predicted direction in that children demonstrated fewer
protesting behaviors when the props were present compared to when the props were not present.
The findings related to interaction order are potentially confounded when the engagement
groups were examined separately. Children in the high engagement group tended to follow the
same trend as the combined sample. These children produced more protesting behaviors during
the no prop condition regardless of the order in which the props were presented (i.e., props first
vs. props second). On the contrary, children in the low engagement group presented a different
trend. Specifically, for the low engagement group, children tended to produce fewer protesting
behaviors during their first interaction regardless of prop presentation. However, it was noted
that the protesting behaviors dramatically increased when the second interaction was the no prop
condition. Thus, the addition of props may increase willingness for children who have low
levels of engagement during book sharing, and, in the context of this study, particularly when
those props are introduced after a first reading. It is also possible that the influence of props on
protesting behaviors in the second reading may be increased even more when children have
116
props during both the first and second book. Further research examining this possibility may
provide caregivers and professionals a strategy for either extending a first book reading
interaction and/or increasing the likelihood that a child may be willing to participate in a second
It is also worth noting that the presence of protesting behaviors seemed to increase the
observable anxiety of all of the caregivers which in turn led to the interaction being rushed or
terminated. It is possible that the addition of toy props and subsequent decrease in protesting
behaviors made the interaction more enjoyable and engaging for both the children and the
caregivers. Thus, the dyads participated in more of a back and forth interaction extending the
overall time that the caregiver and child were sharing a mutual focus of attention and decreasing
This is an interesting finding given Lonigan’s (1994) conclusion that caregivers are likely
to read to children more often and/or for longer periods of time when the child appears receptive
and engaged in the interaction. In line with Sameroff’s (1993) transactional model of
development, it is possible that the lack of protesting was interpreted by the caregiver as an
indicator of the child’s interest in engaging in the storybook interaction and thus the caregivers
were more likely to spend more time engaging with the book. This conclusion is supported by
the descriptive data for the average subjective rating of engagement. Recall that a very similar
trend was identified for the low engagement group in that an interaction order effect was noted as
a potential influence. Children in the low engagement group were subjectively rated as
significantly less engaged during the second interaction, and particularly so, when this
interaction did not include props. Therefore, when children produced higher numbers of
protesting behaviors, they were also perceived by the researcher as less engaged in the book
117
reading interaction. It follows that the caregivers would also perceive their children as less
engaged.
If this is the case, then one may conclude that toy props are facilitating engagement and
interest on the part of the child in at least an indirect manner that may lead to enhanced
storybook interactions. This conclusion is supported by Scarborough and Dobrich (1994) who
noted that children who are read to more often show higher interest and motivation to engage in
these types of interactions. Similarly, the findings of Sonnenschein and Munsterman (2002)
suggest a positive relationship between early positive reading experiences and later motivation
Differential Impact of Toy Props. Another important finding of the current study was the
differential effect the addition of toy props had during storybook reading for children in the low
engagement group compared to children in the high engagement group. Although it was
hypothesized that the addition of toy props would have a differential impact on storybook
reading for the two engagement groups, the impact was hypothesized to be in the same direction
for both engagement groups favoring the prop book reading condition. The fact that the addition
of toys props was found to increase the amount of verbal/vocal communicative acts and gestures
used by the children in the low engagement group is a very positive finding. It is just this kind of
strategy that may help these less engaged children become more engaged in storybook
This finding is also important given the identified differences between the children in the
low engagement group compared to the children in the high engagement group. Recall that there
was a significant difference identified between the low engagement group and the high
engagement group in their expressive language abilities. Specifically, children in the low
118
engagement group tended to have lower scores on the expressive communication subtest of the
Communicative Developmental Inventory (Fenson et al., 1992), but still within the typical range.
Recall also that the children in the low engagement group were found to produce more
verbal/vocal acts and gestures during the prop condition compared to the no prop condition.
Thus, the props seemed to encourage the children in the low engagement group to talk more than
they did during the no prop condition despite their expressive language levels. In addition,
across several dependent measures, the low engagement group actually demonstrated higher
performance than the high engagement group during the prop condition, thus it would appear
This finding is particularly important for the children in the low engagement group who
are demonstrating language abilities at the lower end of the typical continuum. The caregivers of
these children perceive them to have limited engagement during routine storybook reading.
These perceptions may not be surprising given that the storybook context itself is inherently
demanding with respect to language when compared to other caregiver-child contexts (e.g.,
play). It is possible that this language demanding context coupled with the child’s demonstrated
limited expressive language skills and the caregiver’s perception of their child as less interested
and engaged may serve as an early indicator of a group of children who may never receive an
official label of language delay but who may struggle in the future with these kinds of language
related tasks.
language may decrease the likelihood that this group of children will seek out language rich
experiences such as storybook reading which will in turn decrease their motivation and interest
119
in engaging in these types of interactions. A cumulative effect may also be attributed to the
child’s difficulty in participating in the shared interaction verbally which is therefore perceived
by the caregiver as the child’s lack of interest to engage in the book reading interaction, thus
leading to the caregiver’s lack of motivation to attempt to facilitate these types of interactions. It
is possible that these early differences may represent larger differences later in life for this
development, the interrelated nature of these factors is important to consider. Therefore, the fact
that the addition of props supports children who are less engaged in book reading in their efforts
to interact and engage in shared book reading with their caregivers is important. Perhaps the
addition of props increases the likelihood that caregivers and their young children will participate
in shared book reading. In the current study, props did lead the children in the low engagement
group to talk more than they did during the no prop condition despite their expressive language
levels.
In direct contrast to the findings for the low engagement group, the addition of props
during storybook reading decreased the amount of verbal/vocal and gestural communicative acts
produced by the children in the high engagement group. In fact, children in the high engagement
group had fewer verbal/vocal and gestural communicative acts during both the prop and no prop
condition than did the children in the low engagement group when they had props. Not only does
it appear that the props depressed the output of the children in the high engagement group
compared to their no prop productions, on some variables their performance was even lower than
the children with low engagement. It is possible that the addition of toy props was distracting for
these children who typically demonstrate a high frequency of active engagement during
storybook reading. Observational notes support this idea by indicating a tendency for children in
120
the high engagement group to shift their attention back and forth between the toys and book, and
at times, to play with the toys to the exclusion of the storybook interaction. In addition, at least
one caregiver in the high engagement group remarked “how distracting” the toys were during
their interaction and several other caregivers in the high engagement group were observed to
seem confused and challenged by this additional element to an already established routine. For
example, one mother in the high engagement group noted, “The toys are too exciting” as she
attempted to situate herself and her daughter. This mother appeared to feel she needed to
compete with the toys as opposed to incorporating them into the book reading experience.
Another caregiver in the high engagement group, a highly educated early childhood teacher, also
demonstrated a feeling of competition for her child’s attention with the toys present. Despite this
mother’s exceptional reading style, she acknowledged the toys minimally and never made an
active attempt to incorporate the toys in the reading interaction (although the child was engaging
Perhaps for parents of children who are typically developing and who are already
perceived as highly engaged during storybook reading, the props added a new dynamic to the
interaction that was difficult for the parents to determine how to manage. As evidenced by their
ratings of their children’s interest and engagement in storybook reading, many of these parents
may be more likely to experience relatively successful interactions with their children on a
regular basis and therefore may not be accustomed to having to “work” to engage their children.
The addition of toy props for these dyads may have produced some competition for the child’s
attention that the caregivers in the high engagement group are not used to encountering. It
should be noted, however, that parental perceptions of the utility of the props were not collected
121
formally and observational notes were not consistently included with each data set. Therefore
There are important confounding factors that must be considered when drawing
conclusions based on the current study. First, despite efforts to do so, the order of prop
presentation was not counter-balanced fully across the engagement groups. As a result, within
engagement groups, there was an unequal distribution of dyads receiving the props during their
first and second book reading interaction. This unequal distribution seems to have had the
greatest overall impact on the results for the low engagement group. Specifically, there were
differences identified that may be better attributed to the child’s ability to sustain attention during
a second book interaction than to the effects of prop presentation. The possible confound
presented by the unequal representation of dyads receiving props first versus second in both
engagement groups is an important consideration. Given the potential for props to attenuate the
limited engagement of children identified by their caregivers as less interested and engaged in
book reading interactions highlighted in the current study, further research that systematically
In contrast to the initial hypotheses, no significant differences were found between the
high and low groups on measures such as the subjective rating of engagement and the overall
rate of communicative acts. Although on both variables the means paralleled many of the other
variables, the failure to identify significant findings in these measures may be due to several
reasons. The unequal representation within the two engagement groups in order of prop
presentation may certainly have an influence, but other factors may also be at play. For example,
the small sample size and the limited number of opportunities for the dyads to read books with
toy props may have influenced the results. With a larger sample, the dependent measures with
122
trends in the hypothesized direction may have reached significance. In addition, with added
opportunities for parents to perform with and without the toy props, other differences may
surface.
Further, it is also possible that the two groups did not differ enough in their initial level of
engagement. While there were significant group differences (p=<.01) between the high and low
engagement groups based on the caregiver’s report on the Individual Child Literacy
Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b), the groups were quite similar based on the Caregiver Literacy
Questionnaire (Roy, 2005a). Recall that the Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire provided
data on each child’s active participation during storybook reading, whereas the Caregiver
Literacy Questionnaire provided data regarding the home literacy environment and each child’s
literacy exposure. Roberts (2005) found that the home environment and daily literacy practices
accounted for some of the variance identified between groups of children later in life when it
comes to language and literacy abilities. Therefore it might be assumed that although the
caregivers in the current study reported differences in their child’s current level of active
participation during storybook reading, the similarity of their daily literacy experiences had a
strong enough influence to attenuate some of the expected differences between the two groups.
Given the trends in the current study and knowledge of the importance of the home literacy
environment and daily literacy practices, it is likely that additional group differences would have
been observed if the children had been from groups of children with more disparate home
Despite the similarities in home literacy exposure of the children, significant differences
were found in the total interaction time, frequency of child protesting behaviors, and total
communicative acts when props were present signaling early differences emerging between the
123
groups of high and low engaged children during shared book reading interactions. These
findings lend support to Kaderavek and Sulzby’s (1998) assertion that there is a group of
children who indeed have a specific disinterest in storybook reading and demonstrate limited
Further, when looking at the lack of significant differences on the subjective rating of
engagement, despite significant differences for other quantitative data, it is possible that the
rating scale used to make the subjective judgments was not sensitive enough to measure the
subtle differences between the groups. The five-point scale included a neutral score of “3.” This
appeared to reduce the variability in ratings. Perhaps a scale that did not include a default neutral
Another influence on the findings regarding the subjective rating, was the difficultly the
coders had reaching reliability on the subjective rating of engagement. Recall that reliability for
exact matches was only 72% with notable differences identified in the ability to reliably code the
prop book condition compared to the no prop condition. The resolution in the current study was
to conduct consensus coding on the entire data set for subjective rating of engagement.
Additional significant differences between the high and low engagement groups may be found in
the future with a rating scale that offers a clearer set of operational definitions and could be used
hypothesized at the onset of the study, the current study did not result in a main effect of prop
condition or group membership, or a group by prop condition interaction for the balance of
participation between the caregiver and the child during the book reading interactions. Recall
that the balance of participation examined the ratio of adult to child turns during the storybook
124
interactions. For the children in the low engagement group, the child and caregiver turns
remained consistent over the two prop conditions. Perhaps this was due to contingent caregivers
who were adequately judging their child’s current level of participation and affirming and
acknowledging each child’s communicative act similarly across prop conditions. The
consistency may also have been influenced by the fact that caregivers of children in the low
engagement group were accustomed to scaffolding for their children and did so whether the
props were present or not. Perhaps analysis of the quality of parental language input during the
Although statistically not significant, the ratio of caregiver to child turns for the high
engagement group was higher for the no prop condition. In other words, caregivers were taking
more turns during the no prop condition. As mentioned previously, it is possible that the
addition of toys props was more distracting for the children (and perplexing for the parents) in
the high engagement group, and therefore, the parents followed the children’s lead of fewer
overall turns. Indeed, the no prop condition may be viewed as the one more like what the dyads
in the high engagement group are more accustomed to and therefore, the higher ratio of caregiver
turns for every child turn may reflect their typical rate.
It is also possible that significant differences in balance of participation between the prop
and no prop book reading conditions were not identified due to the fact that data was only
collected at one single point. In fact, van Kleek and Beckley-McCall (2002) report that multiple
data points often provide a more accurate picture of caregiver-child interactions during book
reading and therefore may be an important research design consideration for future studies.
Perhaps if caregivers incorporate toy props on a consistent basis during storybook reading, a
main effect in the ratio of caregiver and child turns for the prop condition would be evident. As
125
found in McDonnell, Freil-Patti, and Rollins (2003), repeated readings of the same book often
lead to the child participating in more advanced ways. Thus it is possible that repeated readings
and/or repeated opportunities for the child to experience book reading with toy props present
Home Literacy Environment. It is interesting to note that given the similarity of the
participants’ literacy environments discussed previously, the children were already beginning to
demonstrate a specific interest (or lack of interest) in storybook reading at such an early age. In
contrast to the current study, Morrow (1983) identified significant differences in the literacy
environments of the high and low engagement groups in her study. The results of Morrow
(1983) indicated that the children in the two engagement groups differed in the frequency with
which they chose to look at books and the frequency with which they engaged in literacy
activities during free play. Both of these differences favored the children in the high engagement
group. There were also differences noted between Morrow’s caregivers, with the high
engagement mothers having completed a higher level of education and choosing to engage in
more literacy related types of leisure activities. The current study contrasted with these findings
in that the caregivers in both groups tended to have similar educational experiences (most
completing college or higher) and most caregivers reported similar available literacy materials at
home.
Some additional differences between the current study and the Morrow study should also
including observations during free play, teacher ratings of engagement, and a forced choice
interest survey to determine engagement group membership. The current study relied solely on
the caregivers’ report on the Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b) that requested
126
each caregiver to indicate the frequency with which their child demonstrated particular
observable engagement behaviors (e.g., turning pages, making book related comments).
Additionally, Morrow’s participants were between 3-4 years of age, making them significantly
older than the children in the current study (18-27 months). The differences between the findings
from the current study and those reported by Morrow may also be due to differences in how
families were recruited. The current study relied solely on volunteer participation whereas
Morrow recruited entire classrooms of children providing her with a wider range of participants.
Therefore, although dyads were not recruited to represent a specific population, the current study
Study Limitations
There are several limitations associated with this study that may affect the extent to
which the results can be generalized to other individuals. First, a potentially limiting factor of the
current study was the sequence of the prop condition procedures. Specifically, the researcher
administered the PLS-4 (Zimmerman et al., 2002) during the first home visit and dyads engaged
in a play activity for approximately 5 minutes. This allowed the researcher to establish rapport
with the child and for the child to become familiar with the video equipment. During the second
home visit each dyad engaged in the two storybook reading interactions with the presentation of
counterbalancing was not achieved given the nature of the study. Therefore, as described in the
results section, the final data set included 7 low engagement dyads and 4 high engagement dyads
who read the book with props first and 5 low engagement dyads and 8 high engagement dyads
who read the book with props second. Although an analysis was conducted to examine the main
effect of prop order and only two significant differences were identified, the small sample size
127
did not allow for separate analyses examining the main effect of prop order for each engagement
group. Instead, descriptive statistics were used to examine the relevant means and standard
deviations across groups and subgroups, and trends were noted that at times contradicted the
overall findings. Future studies can address this limitation by increasing the overall sample size
and insuring equal representation of dyads from each engagement group in each condition.
From a subjective standpoint, the sequence of prop conditions also posed a challenge for
some of the children who read the book with props first. For the children in the prop first group
(regardless of engagement group), it was sometimes difficult to transition the child away from
the toy props (e.g., putting them away) and move on to the second book. This was not the case
for children who read the book with props second, as they were typically allowed to play with
the toys while the researcher gathered and put away all of the equipment. Specifically, there
were at least two children (one from the high and one from the low engagement group) who
protested considerably during the reading of the second book that did not include props. This did
not happen for any child who received the same book initially without props. It may be assumed
that some children may be less willing to give up the toys after reading the first book and move
on to the second book without toys (regardless of engagement group membership), if not given
sufficient time to play with the toys from the first book. Therefore, problem solving about how
to separate the book reading sessions by a specific time period or by incorporating time for all
children to play with the props before moving on to the next book without props should be
considered in future studies addressing similar research questions. This may decrease the
likelihood that a particular child would be upset when the toys are taken away simply because
they want to play with the toys for a longer period, and separate this difference from children
who are genuinely not interested in reading the storybook unless the toys are present.
128
A second limitation regarding the design of the current study involved the criteria set to
determine engagement group membership and the rating criteria used to make the subjective
ratings of engagement during the caregiver-child interactions. Both criteria were created by the
researcher based on current literature; however, these measures have not been empirically tested
for validity and may not yet be considered reliable measures of engagement at this time. Due to
the difficulty finding child participants whose caregiver’s rated them at the extreme ends
(specifically in the low engagement group), the cut off criteria for group membership allowed
only a .25 break between the groups. This was considered a limitation in that there were several
children whose ratings on the Individual Child Literacy Questionnaire (Roy, 2005b) fell very
close to the cutoff point, decreasing the difference between the groups. A replication of the
current study should include at least a 1-point gap between the upper and lower criterion cutoff
scores for the high and low engagement group in order to examine specific differences related to
engagement group membership. It is unclear whether this will require a different measure or if
more disparate groups can be identified using the current tool. While the small difference
between the two groups is a potential limitation of the current study, nonetheless significant
differences were identified between the two engagement groups and the rating criteria did
Third, although specific instructions were provided to all caregivers including examples
as to how the props might be used, some caregivers demonstrated very limited toy prop use.
Confounding this issue was the observation that some caregivers appeared very awkward in their
attempts to incorporate the props, with at least two caregivers remarking on how difficult it was
to manage the interaction when adding this additional element into the dynamics. Limited prop
use was typical of a total of 9 caregivers (6 high, 3 low) who demonstrated less than nine overall
129
toy prop behaviors (compared to an overall mean of 28.7 [SD = 12.9] behaviors across the other
caregivers), thus potentially influencing the results in favor of decreasing the potential
There are a few possible modifications to the study procedure that may ameliorate this
problem in future studies. Initially research could examine the influence of toy props when
pathologist, early education teacher). A study of this nature would control for the variability in
actual prop use and extend our knowledge base regarding specific strategies that are most
beneficial to managing such a dynamic interaction and particular techniques for implementing
them. Once strategies have been identified in a more controlled setting, caregiver training could
research (DeTemple, 2001; Justice et al., 2005; Martinez & Roser, 1985; McDonnell, Friel-Patti,
& Rollins, 1997; McDonnell et al., 2003), the frequency of storybook reading with young
children is an important component of this type of caregiver-child interaction and the benefits
associated with book reading. Therefore, the ultimate goal following a more structured
implementation of toy props with trained researchers would be to examine different strategies for
teaching caregivers and early care providers how to incorporate toy props into storybook reading
Fourth, there is also a potential “examiner presence” effect. The presence of the
researcher possibly influenced the child as well as the caregiver’s behavior. It has been accepted
in the field that the presence of an unknown party has the potential to influence the interaction
between the child and caregiver. Regardless, the study was designed to attempt to circumvent
130
“examiner presence” effects by administering the PLS-4 with each child on the first day,
allowing the examiner to establish rapport with the child prior to the storybook readings with the
caregiver. The examiner also videotaped children engaging in a favorite activity with their
caregiver in order to introduce the video equipment and allow the children an opportunity to
experience being videotaped prior to reading with their caregivers. Nevertheless, although the
majority of caregivers reported that both book reading interactions were relatively typical of
previous book reading interactions, several caregivers felt like the book reading interactions were
different than other similar interactions with their child. Additionally, several caregivers rated
their child’s perceived interest and engagement during the book reading interactions as less than
usual and indicated they felt like their child was not as actively engaged as typical during the no
prop book reading condition. Therefore, the degree to which the study’s storybook interactions
were similar to typical interactions between the caregivers and their young children may have
Fifth, the relatively homogenous group of caregivers and children who participated in the
study may be viewed as a limitation. Specifically, most of the dyads were from white families of
middle-income socioeconomic status and the majority of caregivers had received high levels of
education. Although a range was represented in the children’s receptive and expressive language
abilities, none of the children in the current study were considered to be delayed in any area of
development. Another factor contributing to the make up of the sample may have been the
nature of the study and home visitation requirement which may have limited the families who
were willing to allow the researcher to visit them at home or those who had the time to do so. It
is also very possible that families with greater variability in the availability of resources,
educational level, and home environment might have responded differently to the addition of toy
131
props during storybook reading. Families who choose to volunteer their time may be more likely
to value literacy and provide quantitatively and qualitatively different experiences for their
children during storybook reading compared to families who do not volunteer for research
studies. Therefore there is a question as to whether the addition of toy props may have a
differential effect on the study participants’ interactions compared to other groups who may have
chosen not to participate in the current study. For these reasons, the results of this study should
The demographic makeup of the current sample should be considered, however, in light
of the fact that the parental education of the participating caregivers was very similar to that of
caregivers in a number of other storybook reading studies (DeBaryshe, 1995a; Haynes &
Saunders, 1998; McDonnell et al., 2003). It may be that caregivers with more formal education
are more likely to seek or have access to study participation, or they may be more likely to
respond to or be selected to participate in research studies. Indeed, Dillman (2000) reported that
fewer minority respondents and those with low levels of formal education tend to volunteer for
studies. Additionally, the homogenous nature of the sample may be considered a strength of the
study in that the two groups were very similar in their demographic makeup and home literacy
practices and yet significant differences based on engagement were observed. Thus the
conclusions based on the findings are more likely related to differences in levels of storybook
reading engagement.
Finally, an important limitation that should be considered when examining the results of
the current study is the multiple ANOVAs run to answer the proposed research questions. It has
been debated in the literature as to when and how to account for the potential inflated risk of
132
Type I errors when multiple comparisons or multiple tests (in the case of the current study) are
conducted to test hypotheses on the same data (Cook & Farewell, 1996; Curran-Everett, 2000;
Rothman, 1990). The risk of identifying a significant finding, when there is indeed no
underlying effect or difference, is increased when many tests are conducted. Therefore, given
the fact that multiple tests were conducted and the possibility that some of the findings in the
current study may not actually be an indicator of true differences, it is important to identify this
as a potential limitation to the current study, thus warranting continued investigation aimed at
testing specific hypothesis based on the preliminary findings of this study and efforts utilized to
The results of this investigation suggest several important directions for future research
and outline some potentially beneficial areas of consideration for clinical practice. First, this
study extends our current knowledge base regarding child engagement and interest as a
contributing component of the success of storybook reading. Prior research has supported the
important benefits associated with the frequency of shared book reading interactions between
caregivers and their young children (Bus et al., 1995; Lonigan, 1994). The current study adds to
this understanding by identifying one potential strategy for increasing a child’s interest and
active engagement, particularly when the child is perceived as having low engagement during
shared book reading. Child engagement has recently been discussed in the literature as an
important contributing factor associated with storybook reading (Baker & Scher, 2002; Lonigan,
1994; Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002) and thus identifying strategies to promote and
facilitate child engagement are important. If caregivers are able to extend the time that they are
engaged with their children during storybook reading by adding toy props to facilitate these
133
interactions, they increase the child’s opportunities to experience rich language input and to
extending interaction times also increased the amount of time caregivers and their young
children spent in interactions centered around a mutual focus of attention. This may be a
potentially important finding given the known relationship between episodes of joint attention
and later language development (Baldwin, 1995). Future research examining different strategies
for incorporating toy props into storybook reading interactions will further increase the clinical
The current study only involved one opportunity for caregivers and children to engage
with the toy props during storybook reading. Future research including data collected over
multiple data points with dyads having several opportunities to incorporate the toy props into
their book reading interactions is warranted. Additionally, future studies examining the
influence of toy props on storybook reading should include a measure of the caregiver’s
perceptions of the ease with which they were able to incorporate the props and how often they
typically incorporate props into their routine book reading interactions at home.
Future studies are needed examining a variety of child related factors and their influence
on the success of storybook reading. Factors including the affective quality of the interaction
(Sonnenschein & Munsterman, 2002), the child’s interest and motivation to engage in shared
book reading (Baker & Wigfield, 1999), and different strategies to promote and facilitate active
engagement during caregiver-child interactions (Danko, 2004) will all contribute to the
storybook reading literature and serve to influence current practice when working with young
children.
134
Finally, future research should aim to expand the methodology and findings of this study
particular interest to the researcher are the implications and potential impact of the addition of
toy props during storybook reading with children with a diagnosis of autism. It has been
reported in the literature that children with disabilities are less likely then their same aged peers
to engage in storybook reading with a caregiver and these interactions tend to be less rich than
interactions with typically developing children (Light & Kelford Smith, 1993). One potential
factor contributing to this finding is the fact that children with disabilities typically have a co-
occurring language disorder that can make processing and understanding the language of
storybooks difficult, thus decreasing a child’s motivation to engage in such language rich
interactions. The addition of toy props may provide a bridge for some children between the
abstract language of the book and the concrete nature of the toy props. This may be especially
true for very young children who are demonstrating receptive and expressive language skills at
the lower end of the range. Recall that the current study identified two groups of children (low
and high engagement) and the children in the low engagement group differed significantly from
the children in the high engagement group in terms of their expressive language skills. Although
future research is warranted, low interest and engagement in storybook reading coupled with
language abilities that border the lower end of the typical range may be a potential indicator of
later language and literacy difficulties. Thus, the current study lends some preliminary support
for extending this study to include children with identified language and literacy difficulties as
well as children who may present as “at risk” for difficulties in the future.
135
The current study supports the addition of toy props as one strategy for actively engaging
children in book reading interactions and possibly extending these interactions for longer periods
of time. Increasing the time a child spends in a potentially beneficial learning context, such as
the increased amount of time observed in the current study, may allow the child more
opportunities to make new discoveries in a meaningful environment as well as allow extra time
for the child to process and assimilate this information into their current understandings. Future
research is warranted examining the potential benefits and applicability of toy props during
Final Conclusions
professionals working with young children. The findings from the current study contribute to
our knowledge base highlighting child engagement and interest as an important consideration for
future research. Furthermore, the identified group differences between the prop and no prop
storybook conditions may provide evidence to support toy prop use as a potentially beneficial
strategy for facilitating storybook interactions with very young children. Of particular interest is
the finding that the addition of toy props extends the total interaction time that caregivers and
their young children spend in a shared context. This finding identifies an important rational for
future research in this area. Given the fact that storybook reading has been found to facilitate
areas of development such as vocabulary, expressive and receptive language development, and
emergent literacy, it can be assumed that longer interactions within this positive context may
provide opportunities for caregivers to expand and extend their child’s development in these
areas. Additionally, the differences in the mean protesting behaviors during the prop and no
prop conditions favoring the prop condition may contribute to the current knowledge base
136
regarding the hypothesis that toy props facilitate engagement and therefore may contribute to
independent reading later in life. Future studies are needed to further examine the influence of
toy props on storybook reading interactions especially as one strategy for extending second book
readings. Future research should aim to control for the limitations of the current study including
increasing the sample size, extending the total number of observations of caregivers and their
young children reading storybooks with toy props, controlling for prop order effects and
identifying valid measures of engagement that can be utilized with very young children.
137
APPENDIX A
Dear Caregivers,
I am a speech-language pathologist and a doctoral student in the Division of Speech and Hearing
Sciences at the University of North Carolina in Chapel Hill. I am conducting a research study
entitled “Caregivers and Young Children Reading Storybooks with Toy Props.” The purpose of
this study is to examine the influence of toy props during storybook reading with young children
and their caregivers. I am looking for children to participate in my study who are between the
ages of 18-24 months of age and who are developing language without difficulty. I am
interested in both children who are highly engaged in storybook reading with a caregiver as well
as children who are not very engaged by this type of activity.
Caregivers and children who participate in this study will be asked to do the following:
• Caregivers will consent to allow the child’s classroom daycare provider to fill out a
questionnaire regarding the child’s literacy attitudes and behaviors observed in the
classroom.
• Caregivers will fill out 5 brief questionnaires which should take a total time of
approximately 10-20 minutes to complete.
• Children will have their receptive language (what the child understands) and expressive
language (what the child says) assessed by the researcher. This assessment is estimated
138
to require approximately 15-20 minutes to complete with children between the age of 18-
24 months of age.
• On two different occasions convenient for you, the research will come to your house and
videotape you and your child engaging in two different activities together. During the
first home visit, you will be asked to identify a “favorite” activity that you and your child
typically engage in together. The researcher will videotape you and your child engaging
in this activity for approximately 3-5 minutes. During the second home visit, the
researcher will videotape you and your child reading two different children’s storybooks.
Each of these home visits will take approximately 30-45 minutes depending on how long
it takes your child to become comfortable with the camera and how long you are involved
in the videotaped interactions. At the end of each session, you will be asked to complete
a brief questionnaire about the interactions and whether or not these interactions were
typical of similar interactions with your child. This questionnaire is expected to take less
than 5 minutes to complete.
• As a part of your participation in the study, you will receive a copy of your videotaped
interactions, two free children’s storybooks, and a one page report summarizing your
child’s scores on the general developmental questionnaire, the vocabulary questionnaire,
and the language assessment.
If you are interested in participating, please read and sign the attached consent form and return
them in the envelope provided. I will be placing a box in your child’s daycare classroom to
collect your envelope. By returning the attached forms you are indicating your interest in the
study and willingness to participate. However, you are not required to participate and may
choose to withdraw at any time. It is possible that some consenting families will not be selected
to participate in the study. In this case, you will be contacted in writing or by telephone. If you
have any questions, please do not hesitate to call me at (919) 824-1263 or email me at
[email protected]. You can also contact my dissertation advisor Dr. Elizabeth Crais at
(919) 966-9458 or by email at [email protected]. Thank you very much for your interest in
this research.
Sincerely,
All research on human volunteers is reviewed by a committee that works to protect your rights
and welfare. If you have questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant you
may contact, anonymously if you wish, the University of North Carolina’s Biomedical
Institutional Review Board at 919-966-1344 or [email protected].
139
APPENDIX B
Title of Study: Caregivers and Children Reading Storybooks With Toy Props
What are some general things you should know about research studies?
You are being asked to take part in a research study. To join the study is voluntary.
You may refuse to join, or you may withdraw your consent to be in the study, for any reason.
Research studies are designed to obtain new knowledge that may help other people in the future.
You may not receive any direct benefit from being in the research study. There also may be risks
to being in research studies.
Deciding not to be in the study or leaving the study before it is done will not affect your
relationship with the researcher or the University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill.
Details about this study are discussed below. It is important that you understand this information
so that you can make an informed choice about being in this research study. You will be given a
copy of this consent form. You should ask the researchers named above, or staff members who
may assist them, any questions you have about this study at any time.
140
You are being asked to be in the study because you have a child between the ages of 18 and 24
months of age who attends daycare at least 10 hours a week.
1. You will consent to allow your child’s classroom daycare provider to fill out a
questionnaire regarding your child’s literacy attitudes and behaviors at daycare.
2. You will complete five brief questionnaires. One questionnaire will provide information
to the researcher in order to contact you to schedule your home visits and to send you
information related to your participation in this study. A second questionnaire will
request some general information about you and your child including age, gender,
ethnicity, education level, and estimated family income. The third and fourth
questionnaires will gather information regarding the literacy practices in your home and
your perceptions of your child’s literacy attitudes and behaviors. The fifth questionnaire
will ask you to rate your child’s familiarity with a short list of children’s storybook.
3. You will complete three additional questionnaires in order to document the
developmental status of your child. One questionnaire assesses your child’s overall
development, the second questionnaire assesses your child’s expressive vocabulary (the
words your child can say), and the third questionnaire requests information regarding
your child’s hearing status.
4. The researcher will assess your child’s receptive (what your child understands) and
expressive (what your child can say) language skills by administering the Preschool
Language Scale - 4th Edition (PLS-4), a standardized tool used for children X to Y age.
5. On two different occasions convenient to you, the researcher will come to your home and
video and audio-tape you and your child engaging in a “favorite” activity and sharing two
different storybooks together. Each of these home visits will take approximately 30-45
minutes. On both days, you will complete a short questionnaire about how typical the
video-recorded interactions were for you and your child.
141
6. As a token of our appreciation, you will receive two children’s storybooks, a copy of
your videotaped interactions, and a brief summary of your child’s scores on the
developmental measures administered as a part of the study.
What are the possible risks or discomforts involved with being in this study?
There are no expected risks involved in participating in this study. However, if any should
occur, you should report any problems to the Principal Investigator.
No participant will be identified in any report or publication about this study. Although every
effort will be made to keep research records private, there may be times when federal or state law
requires the disclosure of such records, including personal information. This is very unlikely,
but if disclosure is ever required, UNC-Chapel Hill will take steps allowable by law to protect
the privacy of personal information. In some cases, your information in this research study could
be reviewed by representatives of the University, research sponsors, or government agencies for
purposes such as quality control or safety.
What if you want to stop before your part in the study is complete?
You may choose not to be in the study or to stop being in the study at any time before it is over.
This will not affect your child’s enrollment at your local daycare provider. You will also not be
offered or receive any special consideration if you take part in this research. The investigators
also have the right to stop your participation at any time. This could be because you were unable
to complete some part of the study or information gained from the study indicates you are no
longer eligible to participate.
142
Will you receive anything for being in this study?
You will receive a copy of your video-recorded interactions, two children’s storybooks and a
brief summary of your child’s scores on the developmental measures collected as a part of the
study.
I have read the information provided above. I have asked all the questions I have at this time. I
voluntarily agree to participate in this research study.
_________________________________________ _________________
Signature of Research Subject Date
_________________________________________
Printed Name of Research Subject
_________________________________________ _________________
Signature of Person Obtaining Consent Date
_________________________________________
Printed Name of Person Obtaining Consent
143
APPENDIX C
Child’s Gender: M F
Email:__________________________________________________________________
144
APPENDIX D
The following section will ask you to describe how often you note your child engaging in a variety of reading related behaviors. Please
rate your child based on what he/she is currently doing at home using the following scale.
Rarely or Less
Frequently or
A few times a
A few times a
than2 times a
Sometimes or
Almost Never
Almost daily
Often or
month
month
week
145
5. Who does your child live with? (Check all that apply)
__ Mother __ Step Mother __ Foster Mother
__ Father __ Step Father __ Foster Father
__ Grandmother __ Brother(s) ___ Other (please describe )
__ Grandfather __ Sister(s) ______________________
6. How many hours does your child spend in daycare each week?
__ less than 10 hours per week __ between 20-30 hours per week
__ between 10-20 hours per week __ more than 30 hours per week
10. What is the highest level of education the child’s mother has completed?
__ Less than 12th grade __ College Graduate
__ High School Graduate or GED __ Graduate or Professional School
__ Some college
146
APPENDIX E
12. What is the highest level of education the child’s father has completed?
__ Less than 12th grade __ College Graduate
__ High School Graduate or GED __ Graduate or Professional School
__ Some college
(**Modified from Anderson-Yockel & Haynes, 1994; Frijters et al., 2000; Watson, Andrews, and Orovitz, In prep)
147
APPENDIX F
2. Approximately how many times per week do you have the opportunity to read with your child?
__ Rarely __ 3-5 times per week __ more than 10 times per week
__ 1-2 times per week __ 6-10 times per week
3. Approximately how many different books do you read with your child in a week?
__ None __ 3-5 different books __ more than 10 different books per week
__ 1-2 different books __ 6-10 different
4. How many times per week does another caregiver have the opportunity to read to your child?
__ Rarely __ 3-5 times per week __ more than 10 times per week
__ 1-2 times per week __ 6-10 times per week
5. How often are other children taking part when you are reading storybooks with your child?
__ Never __ Sometimes __ Always
__ Rarely __ Frequently
6. Approximately how many children’s books does your child have at home?
__ Less than 3 __ 16-30 __ More than 50 children’s books
__ 5-15 __ 30-50
7. When you do have the opportunity to read to your child, approximately how long does a “typical “
reading session last?
__ Less than 1 minute __ 6-10 minutes __ more than 15 minutes
__ 3-5 minutes __ 11-15 minutes
8. Which of the following are found in your home? (Check all that apply)
__ Comic books __ Magazines for younger children (age 2-5)
__ Alphabet books __ Instructional videos for children
__ Picture books without words __ Books for adults
__ Books with single words on each page __ Cookbooks
__ Number books (books about numbers/counting) __ Dictionary or Encyclopedias
__ Interactive Books __ Telephone books
(books with flip-flaps, things to push/pull, etc) __ Daily or weekly newspapers
__ Books with several sentences per page
9. How often does your child have the opportunity to visit the public library?
__ Rarely __ 2-4 times per month __ more than 10 times per month
__ 1 time per month __ 5-10 times per month
10. How often does your child have the opportunity to visit a local bookstore?
__ Rarely __ 2-4 times per month __ more than 10 times per month
__ 1 time per month __ 5-10 times per month
148
APPENDIX G
6. Pepo and Lolo and the Red Apple by Ana Martin Larranaga 1 2 3 4 5
149
APPENDIX H
1. Do you have any concerns regarding your child’s ability to hear? Yes __ No __
3. If yes, approximately how many ear infections would you estimate in the last year?
__ less than 5
__ between 5-10
__ more than 10
4. Has your child experienced any ear infection(s) in the past three months?
Yes __ No __
6. How did you typically become aware of the ear infection (e.g., routine doctor visit, child exhibited
symptoms/symptomatic behavior)?
8. How was the ear infection resolved (e.g., recovery with full/partial course of medication,
reoccurrence, spontaneous recovery)?
9. Have you noticed any changes in your child’s hearing as a result of the ear infections?
150
APPENDIX I
If different from your answer above, please list 3 activities or toys that you and your child currently enjoy
playing together.
1.
2.
3.
Please list 3 toys or activities that you feel are difficult to get your child to engage with at this time.
1.
2.
3.
Approximately how long will your child engage in the following activities independently or with a
caregiver without redirection or prompting.
a) Block Play (e.g., stacking, sorting, building)
__ less than 1 min __ 1-2 min __2-5 min __ more than 5 min __ does not engage in this activity
c) Physical play (e.g., catch, ball rolling, rough and tumble play)
__ less than 1 min __ 1-2 min __2-5 min __ more than 5 min __ does not engage in this activity
e) Pretend play (e.g., dolls, action figures, cooking set, play food)
__ less than 1 min __ 1-2 min __2-5 min __ more than 5 min __ does not engage in this activity
f) Storybook reading
__ less than 1 min __ 1-2 min __2-5 min __ more than 5 min __ does not engage in this activity
151
APPENDIX J
1. How would you rate the “favorite” activity interaction between you and your child?
Very typical of our regular play interactions
Somewhat typical of our regular play interactions
Very different from our regular play interactions.
If different, please indicate how the interaction differed. ____________________________________
_________________________________________________________________________________
2. How would you rate your child’s interest and engagement during the “favorite” activity?
very interested and engaged (greater than usual)
typical interest and engagement
less interested and engaged than usual
not interested and engaged at all
initially uninterested and/or engaged but engaged with adult prompting
3. How would you rate your child’s active involvement during the “favorite” activity?
more active than usual (e.g., talked/vocalized more, participated in the interaction more)
typical of our regular play interactions
less active than usual (e.g., did not talk/vocalize as much, did not participate in the activity as much)
If less active, please describe. _________________________________________________________
4. How would you rate your child’s overall level of alertness today?
very alert (greater than usual)
typical alertness
less than usual/sleepy
If less alert than usual, please describe. __________________________________________________
152
APPENDIX K
To receive a Score = 2
Child demonstrates more disengagement behaviors than engagement behaviors. Child is reluctant to participate but does so
at least briefly. Child demonstrates overt disengagement but at least one behavioral indicator that the child was engaged for
a brief period of time. Disengagement should represent the majority of the segment.
To receive a Score = 3
Equal number of engagement and disengagement behaviors. With the toys, child is not necessarily engaging with the
caregiver but is self-directed following own agenda with the toys. (Passive engagement plus disengagement, mostly
passive engagement plus one overt sign of disengagement, no active engagement at all and almost ½ disengaged)
To receive a Score = 4
Child is engaged for the most part (demonstrates more engagement behaviors listed below than disengagement behaviors)
but demonstrates a few behaviors that indicate fleeting disengagement. Child is engaged in the interaction but is not
actively participating. The child is sitting, listening, and generally paying attention. The child will respond to the
caregiver’s questions and/or comments but is not initiating any comments or questions. If the child is passively observing
but does not demonstrate any ACTIVE participation, score segment as a 4. If child is engaged for the majority of the
interaction (only 1 instance of subtle disengagement) then must score as 4 or 5. If child demonstrates any OVERT
disengagement, can NOT score as 4-5. With the toys, the child is mostly engaging in a connected interaction with the
caregiver but may demonstrate some brief disconnection or own agenda behaviors. However, these are brief and do not
represent the majority of the segment.
To receive a Score = 5
Child is completely engaged in the storybook interaction. Child is ACTIVELY participating with the book and/or toys AS
WELL AS the caregiver. The child demonstrates appropriate eye contact (either with the book, toy or caregiver), makes
comments about the book or toys, demonstrates positive affect, and seems to be overall engaged with the interaction. Child
can ONLY receive a rating of 5 if NO disengagement behaviors are noted at all. When reading the book with props - the
child can still receive a score of 5 if they are focusing on the toys and shifting back to the book as long as there is at least 1
ACTIVE engagement behavior and no other disengagement behaviors. Attention to the toys is not coded as disengagement
unless it is CLEARLY to the exclusion of the caregiver.
Possible Engagement Behaviors: directing eye gaze toward book, pointing to pages, making book/toy related comments
(e.g., labeling toy or picture), holding book appropriately, turning pages, sitting within close proximity of caregiver, facing
direction of caregiver, appropriately responding to caregiver’s comments/requests, demonstrates positive affect, physically
reorienting towards the caregiver.
Possible Disengagement Behaviors: Refusing to participate in the interaction, turning away from the caregiver, making
off topic or irrelevant comments, not responding to the caregiver’s request for information or action, crying, fidgeting with
clothing or other items, gazing away from the book/toys/activity, physically orienting away from the caregiver.
*Note if child shifts gaze briefly to notice something the researcher is doing, this is not considered a disengagement
behavior. However, if the child persists in this action for longer than 3 seconds, count as disengagement.
Modified from Kaderavek and Sulzby (1998a)
154
APPENDIX M
Book Transcripts
Pepo and Lolo and the Red Apple. By Ana Martin Larranaga
155
REFERENCES
Allen, L., Cipielewski, J., & Stanovich, K. (1992). Multiple Indicators of Children's Reading Habits
and Attitudes: Construct Validity and Cognitive Correlates. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 84(4), 489-503.
Almy, M. C. (1949). Children's experiences prior to first grade and success in beginning reading. New
York, NY: Teachers College.
Altwerger, B., Deiehl-Faxon, J., & Dockstader-Anderson, K. (1985). Read-aloud events as meaning
construction. Language Arts, 62, 476-484.
Anderson, R., Hiebert, E. F., Wilkinson, I., & Scott, J. (1985). Becoming a nation of readers: The
report of the Commission on Reading. Washington, DC: National Institute of Education.
Anderson, R., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend
their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 285-303.
Anderson-Yockel, J., & Haynes, W. (1994). Joint book-reading strategies in working-class African
American and white mother-toddler dyads. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(3),
583-593.
Arnold, D., Lonigan, C., Whitehurst, G., & Epstein, J. (1994). Accelerating language development
through picture book reading: replication and extension to a videotape training format.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 235-243.
Baker, L. (2003). The role of parents in motivating struggling readers. Reading and Writing
Quarterly, 19, 87-106.
Baker, L., Dreher, M., & Guthrie, J. (2000). Why teachers should promote reading engagement. In
L. Baker, M. Dreher & J. Guthrie (Eds.), Engaging young readers: Promoting achievement
and motivation (pp. 1-16). New York, NY: Guilford.
Baker, L., Mackler, K., Sonnenschein, S., & Serpell, R. (2001). Parents' interactions with their first-
grade children during storybook reading and relations with subsequent home reading
activity and reading achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 39(5), 415-438.
Baker, L., & Scher, D. (2002). Beginning readers' motivation for reading in relation to parental
beliefs and home reading experiences. Reading Psychology, 23, 239-269.
Baker, L., Scher, D., & Mackler, K. (1997). Home and family influences on motivation for reading.
Educational Psychologist, 32(2), 69-82.
Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children's motivation for reading and their
relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(4),
452-477.
Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P.
Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131-158). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
156
Bates, E. (1979). The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York:
Academic Press.
Bellon, M., Ogletree, B., & Harn, W. (2000). Repeated storybook reading as a language intervention
for children with autism. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 15(1), 52-58.
Beukelman, D., & Mirenda, P. (1998). Augmentative and Alternative Communication (2nd ed.).
Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks.
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in
learning to read: a meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of
Educational Research, 65(1), 1-21.
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment manual.
Little Rock: University of Arkansas.
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and
communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 63(4), Serial No. 255.
Cipielewski, J., & Stanovich, K. (1992). Predicting growth in reading ability from children's
exposure to print. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54(1), 74-89.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Cook, R. J., & Farewell, V. T. (1996). Multiplicity considerations in the design and analysis of
clinical trials. Statistical Society, Series A, 159, 93-110.
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity,
preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 421-429.
Crowe, L. (2000). Reading behaviors of mothers and their children with language impairment
during repeated storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33, 503-524.
Crowe, L., Norris, J., & Hoffman, P. (2000). Facilitating storybook interactions between mothers
and their preschoolers with language impairment. Communication disorders quarterly, 21(3),
131-146.
Dale, P., Bates, E., Reznick, S., & Morisett, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of
child language at 20 months. Journal of Child Language, 16, 239-250.
Dale, P., & Crain-Thoreson, C. (1999). Language and literacy in a developmental perspective.
Journal of Behavioral Education, 9(1), 23-33.
157
Danko, C. (2004). Improving the engagement levels of preschool children with autism through the use
of visual supports package during circle time. Unpublished Dissertation, University of North
Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC.
DeBaryshe, B. (1995a). Joint picture-book reading correlates of early oral language skills. Journal
of Child Language, 20, 455-461.
DeBaryshe, B. (1995b). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in the home reading process. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 1-20.
Deloache, J., & DeMendoza, O. (1987). Joint picturebook interactions of mothers and 1-year-old
children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 111-123.
DeTemple, J. (2001). Parents and children reading books together. In D. Dickinson & P. Tabors
(Eds.), Beginning Literacy with Language: Young children learning at home and school (pp.
31-52). Baltimore, MA: Paul Brookes.
Dexter, M. E. (1998). The effects of aided language stimulation upon verbal output and augmentative
communication during storybook reading for children with pervasive developmental disabilities.
Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, John Hopkins University, Baltimore, MA.
Dickinson, D., & McCabe, A. (1991). The acquisition and development of language. A social
interactionist account of language and literacy development. In J. Kavanagh (Ed.), The
language continuum from infancy to literacy. Parkton, MA: York Press.
Dickinson, D., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: the multiple origins, skills, and
environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(4),
186-202.
Dodici, B., Draper, D., & Perterson, C. (2003). Early parent--child interactions and early literacy
development. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 23(3), 124-136.
Dreher, M., & Baker, L. (2003). Motivating struggling readers to succeed: Introduction to the
theme. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 1-4.
Dunn, L. W., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised. Circle Pines:
American Guidance Service.
Ezell, H. K., & Justice, L. M. (2000). Increasing the print focus of shared reading through
observational learning. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 36-47.
Ezell, H. K., Justice, L. M., & Parsons, D. (2000). Enhancing the emergent literacy skills of pre-
schoolers with communication disorders: a pilot investigation. Child Language Teaching &
Therapy, 16(2), 121-140.
Fenson, L. (1986). The developmental progression of play. In A. W. Gottfried & C. Brown (Eds.),
Play interactions: The Contribution of Play Materials and Parental Involvement to Children's
Development (pp. 53-65). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early
communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
59(Serial No. 242).
158
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartnung, J., et al. (1992). The MacArtur
Communicative Developmental Inventory. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
Frijters, J. C., Barron, R., & Brunello, M. (2000). Direct and mediated influences of home literacy
and literacy interest on prereaders' oral vocabulary and early written language skill. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 466-477.
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S. (1999). The relationship between
maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expressive
vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 364-374.
Goodwyn, S., Acredolo, L., & Brown, C. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language
development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81-103.
Guthrie, J., & Knowles, K. (2001). Promoting reading motivation. In L. Verhoeven & C. Snow
(Eds.), Literacy and motivation: Reading engagement in individuals and groups (pp. 159-176).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishing.
Hammill, D. D., & Newcomer, P. L. (1988). Test of Language Development - 2. Austin: PRO-ED.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American
children. Baltimore: Brooks.
Hayden, C., Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers' extratextual comments during storybook
reading: Stylistic differences over time and across contexts. Discourse Processes, 21, 135-169.
Haynes, W., & Saunders, D. (1998). Joint book-reading strategies in middle-class African American
and white mother-toddler dyads: Research note. Journal of Children's Communication
Development, 20, 9-17.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child's
development of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 22, 155-163.
Howell, D. (2002). Statistical Methods for Psychology (5th ed.). Pacific Grove: Duxbury Thomas
Learning.
Jones, C., & Adamson, L. (1987). Language use in mother-child and mother-child-sibling
interactions. Child Development, 58, 356-366.
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2000). Enhancing children's print and word awareness through
home-based parent interaction. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 257-269.
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2002). Use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk
children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 17-29.
159
Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. (2002). Using shared storybook reading to promote emergent
literacy. Teaching Exceptional Children, 34(4), 8-13.
Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. (2003). Topic control during shared storybook reading: mothers
and their children with language impairments. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education,
23(3), 137-150.
Justice, L. M., Meier, J., & Walpole, S. (2005). Learning new words from storybooks: An efficacy
study with at-risk kindergartners. Language , Speech, & Hearing Services in the School, 36,
17-32.
Kaderavek, J., & Sulzby, E. (1998a). Low versus high orientation toward literacy in children. Paper
presented at the Annual Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association, San Antonio, TX.
Kaderavek, J., & Sulzby, E. (1998b). Parent-child joint book reading: An observational protocol for
young children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7(1), 33-43.
King-DeBaun, P. (1990). Storytime: stories, symbols, and emergent literacy activities for young, special
needs children. Park City, UT.: Creative Communicating.
Kirchner, D. (1991). Using verbal scaffolding to facilitate conversational participation and language
acquisition in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Childhood
Communication Disorders, 14(1), 81-98.
Kruif, R., & McWilliam, R. A. (1999). Multivariate relationships among developmental age, global
engagement, and observed child engagement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14(4),
515-536.
Larranaga, A. (2004a). Pepo and Lolo and the red apple. Cambridge: Candlewick Press.
Larranaga, A. (2004b). Pepo and Lolo are friends. Cambridge: Candlewick Press.
Light, J., Binger, C., & Kelford Smith, A. (1994). Story reading interactions between preschoolers
who use AAC and their mothers. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 10, 255-268.
Light, J., & Kelford Smith, A. (1993). Home literacy experiences of preschoolers who use
augmentative communication systems and of their nondisabled peers. Augmentative and
Alternative Communication, 9, 10-25.
Lonigan, C. (1994). Reading to preschoolers exposed: Is the emperor really naked? Developmental
Review, 14, 303-323.
Lonigan, C., & Whitehurst, G. (1998). Getting ready to read: Emergent literacy and family literacy.
Washington, DC.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
Administration for Children and Families.
160
Mahoney, G., & Wheedon, C. A. (1997). Parent-child interaction - the foundation for family-
centered early intervention practive: A response to Baird and Peterson. Topics in early
childhood special education, 17, 165-184.
Markus, J., Mundy, P., Morales, M., Delgado, C., & Yale, M. (2000). Individual differences in
infant skills as predictors of child-caregiver joint attention and language. Social
Development, 9(3), 302-314.
Martinez, M., & Roser, N. (1985). Read it again: the value of repeated readings during storytime.
The Reading Teacher, 38, 782-786.
Marvin, C., & Mirenda, P. (1993). Home literacy experiences of preschoolers enrolled in Head Start
and special education programs. Journal of Early Intervention, 17, 351-367.
Marvin, C., & Wright, D. (1997). Literacy socialization in the homes of preschool children.
Language, Speech, & Hearing Services in the School, 28, 154-163.
McDonnell, S., Friel-Patti, S., & Rollins, P. (1997). Changes in maternal/child discourse across
repeated storybook readings. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for
Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.
McDonnell, S., Friel-Patti, S., & Rosenthal-Rollins, P. (2003). Patterns of change in maternal-child
discourse behaviors across repeated storybook readings. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(3),
323-341.
McGarity, J. R., & Butts, D. P. (1984). The relationship among teacher classroom management
behavior, student engagement, and student achievement of middle and high school science
students of varying aptitude. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2(1), 55-61.
McNeill, J., & Fowler, S. (1999). Let's talk: Encouraging mother-child conversations during story
reading. Journal of Early Intervention, 22(1), 51-69.
McWilliam, R. A., & Bailey, D. (1992). Promoting engagement and mastery. In D. Bailey & M.
Wolery (Eds.), Teaching infants and preschoolers with disabilities. (2nd ed., pp. 230-255). New
York, NY: MacMillan Publishing Company.
Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (2004). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts. Language
Laboratory Waisman Research Center: University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI.
Moerk, E. (1985). Picture-book reading by mothers and young children and its impact upon
language development. Journal of Pragmatics, 9, 547-566.
Morales, M., Mundy, M., & Rojas, J. (1998). Following the direction of gaze and language
development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 373-377.
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, Y., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R., et al. (2000). Responding to
joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition.
Journal-of-Applied-Developmental-Psychology, 21(3), 283-298.
Morrow, L. M. (1983). Home and school correlated of early interest in literature. Journal of
Educational Research, 76(4), 221-230.
161
Mundy, P., & Gomes, A. (1998). Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the
second year. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(3), 469-482.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language
development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115-
128.
Musselwhite, C. (1986). Adaptive Play for Special Needs Children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill.
Musselwhite, C., & King-DeBaun, P. (1997). Emergent Literacy Success: Merging Technology and
Whole Language for Students with Disabilities. Park City, UT: Creative Communicating.
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 38(Serial no. 149).
Neuman, S. B. (1996). Children engaging in storybook reading: The influence of access to print
resources, opportunity, and parental interaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11(4),
495-513.
Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child
Language, 5, 1-15.
Olofsson, A., & Niedersoe, J. (1999). Early language development and kindergarten phonological
awareness problems: From 3-11 years of age. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 464-472.
Paratore, J. (2002). Home and school together: Helping beginning readers succeed. In A. Farstrup
& S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What Research Has to Say About Reading Instruction. Newark DE:
International Reading Association.
Payne, A., Whitehurst, G., & Angell, A. (1994). The role of home literacy environment in the
development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 427-440.
Pellegrini, A. D., Brody, G., & Sigel, I. (1985). Parents' book reading habits with their children.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(3), 332-340.
Pellegrini, A. D., McGillicuddy-Delisi, A., Sigel, I., & Brody, G. (1986). The effects of children's
communicative status and task on parents' teaching strategies. Contemporary Education
Psychology, 11, 240-252.
Phillips, G., & McNaughton, S. (1990). The practice of storybook reading to preschool children in
mainstream New Zealand families. Reading Research Quarterly, 15(3), 196-212.
Pierce, P. L., & McWilliams, P. J. (1993). Emerging literacy and children with severe speech and
physical impairments (SSPI): Issues and possible intervention strategies. Topics in Language
Disorders, 13(2), 47-57.
162
Reese, E., Cox, A., Harte, D., & McAnally, H. (2003). Diversity in adults' styles of reading books to
children. In A. van Kleeck, S. Stahl & E. Bauer (Eds.), On Reading Books to Children:
Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishing.
Roberts, J., Burchinal, M. R., & Durham, M. (1999). Parents' report of vocabulary and
grammatical development of African American preschoolers: Child and environmental
associations. Child Development, 70(1), 92-106.
Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. R. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool
children's language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing
Research, 48(2), 345-359.
Rothman, K. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology, 1, 43-46.
Roy, V. P. (2006). Post Storybook Rating Form. Unpublished rating form. Chapel Hill, NC:
Division of Speech and Hearing Science, Universtiy of North Carolina.
Sameroff, A., & Fiese, B. (1990). Transactional regulation and early intervention. In S. Meisels & J.
Shonkoff (Eds.), Early Intervention: A handbook of theory, practice, and analysis. New York:
CAmbridge University Press.
Scarborough, H., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental
Review, 14(3), 245-302.
Scarborough, H., Dobrich, W., & Hager, M. (1991). Preschool literacy experience and later reading
achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(8), 508-511.
Schneider, P., & Hecht, B. (1995). Interaction between children with developmental delays and
their mothers during a book-sharing activity. International Journal of Disability,
Development & Education, 42(1), 41-56.
Schuele, M., & van Kleeck, A. (1987). Precursors to literacy: Assessment and intervention. Topics in
Language Disorders, 7(2), 32-44.
163
Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks as a
predictor of young children's vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 520-536.
Senechal, M., Thomas, E., & Monker, J.-A. (1995). Individual differences in 4-year-old children's
acquisition of vocabulary during storybook reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8(2),
218-229.
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with
autism, down syndrome and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 64(1), Serial No. 256.
Snow, C., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Snow, C., & Goldfield, B. (1983). Turn the page please: situation-specific language acquisition.
Journal of Child Language, 10, 551-569.
Snow, C., Scarborough, H., & Burns, M. S. (1999). What speech-language pathologists need to
know about early reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(1), 48-58.
Sonnenschein, S., & Munsterman, K. (2002). The influence of home-based reading interactions on
5-year-olds’ reading motivations and early literacy development. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 17(3), 318-337.
Squires, J., Potter, L., & Bricker, D. (1999). Manual for the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (2nd
ed.). Baltimore, MA: Paul Brookes Publishing.
Tannock, R. (1988). Mothers' directiveness in their interactions with their children with and
without down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 93(2), 154-165.
Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Thal, D., & Tobias, S. (1992). Communicative gestures in children with delayed onset of oral
expressive vocabulary. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1281-1289.
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6),
1454-1463.
Trelease, J. (2001). The Read Aloud Handbook (5th ed.). New York, NY: Penguin Books.
van Kleeck, A., & Beckley-McCall, A. (2002). A comparison of mothers' individual and
simultaneous book sharing with preschool siblings: An exploratory study of five families.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 175-189.
164
Vernon-Feagans, L., Hurley, M., & Yont, K. (2002). The effect of otitis media and daycare quality
on mother/child bookreading and language use at 48 months of age. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 113-133.
Wallace, I., Roberts, J., & Lodder, D. (1998). Interactions of African American infants and their
mothers: Relations with development at 1 year of age. Journal of Speech, Language &
Hearing Research, 41, 900-912.
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol Formation. New York: Wiley.
Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (1993). Profiling communication and symbolic abilities in young
children. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 15(1), 23-32.
Whitehurst, G., Arnold, D., Epstein, J., Angell, A., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. (1994). A picture book
reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families.
Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679-689.
Whitehurst, G., Fischel, J., Lonigan, C., Valdez-Menchaca, M., Arnold, D., & Smith, M. (1991).
Treatment of early expressive language delay: If, when, and how. Topics in Language
Disorders, 11, 55-68.
Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child
Development, 69(3), 848-872.
Whitehurst, G., Zevenbergen, A., Crone, D., Schultz, M., Velting, O., & Fischel, J. (1999).
Outcomes of an Emergent Literacy Intervention From Head Start Through Second Grade.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 261-272.
Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. (1997). Relations of children's motivation for reading to the amount and
breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 420-432.
Yarosz, D. J., & Barnett, W. S. (2001). Who reads to young children?: Identifying predictors of
family reading activities. Reading Psychology, 22(1), 67-81.
Zimmerman, I., Steiner, V., & Pond, R. (2002). Preschool Language Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio,
TX: Psychological Corporation.
165