Demir 2007
Demir 2007
DOI 10.1007/s10902-007-9051-8
RESEARCH PAPER
Melikşah Demir
1 Introduction
M. Demir (&)
Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI 48201, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
123
258 M. Demir
Participants of the present study were college students who are described as
emerging adults. The construct of emerging adulthood came to light as an important
and valuable framework to study the lives of those individuals between the ages of
18 and 29, who are neither adolescents nor adults but who are in the process of
becoming an adult (Arnett, 2000, 2004). This age period is demographically, sub-
jectively and psychologically different than other life-span periods that involve a
deeper and more serious level of explorations in the areas of love, work and
worldviews (Arnett, 2000). Present studies were concerned with exploration in the
area of love.
Formation and maintenance of romantic relationships are central to the lives of
emerging adults (Kan & Cares, 2006; Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth, & Tellegen,
2004). College environment provides a context where the important developmental
task of this age could be explored. Supporting this notion are findings that romantic
relationships constitute an important part of the everyday lives of college students
(Gable, Reis, Impett, & Asher, 2004; Lokitz & Sprandel, 1976) and most college
students consider their romantic involvements as the closest relationships they have
(Berscheid, Snyder, & Omoto, 1989). One important issue to highlight is that the
romantic relationships experienced and explored during emerging adulthood are
different from those of the adolescent period. During adolescence, romantic rela-
tionships are mostly pursued with the intent of having fun and experiencing com-
panionship and do not last very long (Feiring, 1996; Roscoe, Dian, & Brooks, 1987).
In contrast, romantic relationships during emerging adulthood become serious, more
intimate and committed (Arnett, 2000; Montgomery, 2005; Prager, 1995). Emerging
adults are searching for a lifelong partner and this has the potential to shape the
intensity and commitment of love relationships experienced during this time period.
On the other hand, many middle-aged adults are married (Arnett, 2004; Noller,
Feeney, & Peterson, 2001). Research pertinent to the overall satisfaction and quality
of lasting marriages among middle-aged adults suggests a relatively stable pattern
(Weishaus & Field, 1988); even though some report different patterns (e.g., curvi-
linear; see Berscheid & Regan, 2005). In the present studies, the quality of romantic
relationships was investigated.
As for relationship quality, theoretical work in this area suggests that individuals
seek and/or experience certain provisions in their close relationships (Weiss, 1974).
These features include but are not limited to companionship, help, affection, inti-
macy and emotional security. Available instruments in the field assess features that
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 259
are theoretically identified (Fletcher, Simpson, & Thomas, 2000; Furman & Buhr-
mester, 1992). The common practice in the literature is to sum the means of different
features with the overall score being labeled relationship quality (Bagwell et al.,
2005; Fletcher et al., 2000). We relied on this overall score in the studies reported.
The outcome investigated across the studies was happiness. There were two reasons
for this. First, people rate personal happiness as very important in their lives
(Diener, Suh, Smith, & Shao, 1995). Second, happiness is associated with success in
different life domains (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005) and is related to positive
mental health (Taylor & Brown, 1988).
Research in the last three decades identified several variables related to happiness
(see Argyle, 2001; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Of the variables posited to be
associated with happiness, personality traits and temperamental characteristics, re-
ferred to as set-point variables (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005), have been shown to be
the major predictors of happiness, accounting for as high as 50% of the variance in
happiness. In the studies reported, our focus was on personality and assessed it with
the Big Five framework.
Prior research has shown that all dimensions of the Big Five framework are
associated with happiness to differing degrees (Compton, 1998; DeNeve & Cooper,
1998; Furnham & Cheng, 1997; McCrae & Costa, 1991). Among the big five, extro-
version and neuroticism were consistent predictors of happiness, extroversion being
positively correlated to happiness and neuroticism being negatively correlated to
happiness (Argyle, 2001; Cheng & Furnham, 2001). It was also found that consci-
entiousness, agreeableness and openness to experience were slightly positively cor-
related with happiness (for a review see Diener et al., 1999). Together, these
personality dimensions account for between 20 and 50% of the variance in happiness.
Close relationships are another source of happiness and received considerable
attention in the field as well (Argyle, 2001; Myers, 2000). Indeed, the role of close
relationships in happiness has been called as the ‘‘deep truth’’ (Myers, 1992). In the
present studies, we took the theoretical perspective that close relationships are a
source of happiness (Ryan & Deci, 2001) rather than a dimension of well-being
(Ryff, 1989).
A considerable amount of research has shown that romantic relationships
(involvement in and/or quality of) are an important source of happiness (Argyle,
2001; Berry & Willingham, 1997; Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000; Khaleque
& Rohner, 2004; see Keyes and Waterman (2003) for a review). For instance, in a
recent study Dush and Amato (2005) showed that involvement in committed
romantic relationships and relationship quality were related to happiness across the
life-span. In another line of research, individuals were asked to list the benefits of
romantic relationships or things that make them happy (Crossley & Langdridge,
2005; Demir, Linley, Kumbul, & Grishkina, 2003; Sedikides, Oliver, & Campbell,
1994). Supporting the research cited earlier, it was found that individuals consider
romantic relationships as an essential source of happiness. Thus, the available lit-
erature suggests that romantic relationships play a vital role in happiness.
Personality is also related to the quality of romantic relationships. It has been
argued that personality influences the quality of and satisfaction with romantic
123
260 M. Demir
The question of whether romantic relationship quality predicts happiness above and
beyond the influence of personality is theoretically important and deserves attention.
Theory and empirical research suggest that quality of close relationships is an
important source of happiness (Argyle, 2001; Berscheid & Regan, 2005; Reis, 2001).
Likewise, personality is a major predictor of happiness (Diener et al., 1999). It is
proposed here that one major limitation of the available literature that documents an
association between the quality of romantic relationships and happiness is that they
do not control for personality. It could be that the relationship between social
experiences and happiness might disappear once personality is taken into account.
Support for this contention comes from studies investigating the relationship between
extroversion and happiness. Specifically, research suggests that extroversion and an
extrovert’s greater sensitivity to rewards might be the main factor predicting happi-
ness (Diener & Lucas, 1999; Lucas & Diener, 2001; Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh,
& Shao, 2000). This raises the possibility that extroverts’ closer romantic relationships
with their partners (e.g., White et al., 2004) might be attributed to their sensitivity to
rewards and close relationships would not emerge as a predictor of happiness.
Are there any theories that might offer a prediction to the question at hand? Other
than major meta-theories positing that romantic relationships are important for
happiness (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995), we do not know of any specific
hypothesis regarding the role of relationships in happiness above and beyond the
influence of personality. Nevertheless, there are studies that might give the reader an
idea. First of all, Lu (1999), in a longitudinal study, reported that social support from
close others (the source of support was not clear) predicted happiness above and
beyond the influence of baseline happiness and personality (extroversion and neu-
roticism). Second, Demir and Weitekamp (in press) found that best friendship quality
contributed to the happiness of emerging adults while controlling for personality
(dimensions of Big Five). Thus, there is evidence suggesting that close relationships
have the potential to contribute to happiness regardless of one’s personality.
In the present studies, the predictive ability of romantic relationships was sub-
jected to a strict test. If the quality of romantic relationships does not contribute to
one’s happiness while controlling for personality, then, relationship scholars would
need to revise their arguments with regard to the importance of close relationships
for the happiness of individuals. This would mean that an important developmental
task of emerging adulthood (formation of romantic relationships) does not have
implications for happiness. On the other hand, finding that quality of romantic
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 261
relationship still predicts happiness while controlling for personality would bolster
confidence to the theoretical arguments regarding the importance of close rela-
tionships in happiness.
One final note is on gender. Considering the research showing that gender is
weakly related to happiness (see Diener et al., 1999), it was not the focus of the
present studies.
2 Study 1
The aim of the first study was to investigate the role of romantic relationship quality
in predicting happiness above and beyond the influence of personality. In doing so,
the global perceived quality of romantic relationships and the global happiness of
emerging adults were assessed. In the light of the literature, the following hypotheses
were developed:
(a) Romantic relationship quality would be positively related to happiness.
(b) Extroversion (positively) and neuroticism (negatively) would be related to
happiness.
(c) Extroversion and agreeableness would be positively related to romantic
relationship quality, whereas neuroticism and openness to experience would
be negatively related.
(d) Romantic relationship quality would contribute to happiness above and
beyond the influence of personality.
2.1 Method
2.1.1 Participants
The original sample consisted of 246 (61 men) college students attending a Mid-
western university in the USA who were involved in a romantic relationship. The
mean age of the sample was 23.79 (SD = 6.50), with a range from 19 to 40 years of
age. The ethnic distribution of the sample was as follows: 55% Caucasian (n = 133),
33% Black (n = 88), 7% Asian (n = 17) and 5% other (n = 10). In the sample, 90%
(n = 221) of the participants were currently involved in a dating relationship and
10% (n = 25) were either engaged or married. Considering the possibility that
inclusion of engaged/married adults might confound the findings and make com-
parisons across studies difficult, they were excluded from the analysis. Thus, the final
sample consisted of 221 (56 men) dating emerging adults (mean age = 22.49
(SD = 4.65) with a range from 19 to 28 years of age.
The average length of the dating relationships was 24.56 months (SD = 22.92)
and ranged from 1 to 79 months.
2.1.2 Procedure
2.1.3 Measures
The correlations between the study variables can be seen in Table 1. Supporting the
hypothesis (a), romantic relationship quality was positively related to happiness.
Moreover, in line with the hypothesis (b), extroversion and neuroticism were related
to happiness in the expected directions. Additionally, agreeableness and conscien-
tiousness were found to be significant (positive) correlates of happiness. Finally, the
relationship between personality and relationship quality were mostly in line with
the hypothesis (c), with the exception of openness to experience not showing any
relationship with romantic relationship quality. These findings are consistent with
the literature (Argyle, 2001; Diener et al., 1999; White et al., 2004).
Table 1 Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for the variables in study 1
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Step 1
Gender –.01
R2 = .00
Step 2
Extroversion .25**
Neuroticism –.47**
Agreeableness .22*
Openness to Experience .03
Conscientiousness .04
DR2 = .50**
Step 3
Romantic relationship quality .18**
DR2 = .03**
123
264 M. Demir
3 Study 2
Finding that romantic relationship quality was predictive of happiness above and
beyond the influence of personality is noteworthy, however, one should be cautious
about generalizing from one study. Thus, a second study was conducted to replicate
and extend the findings of the first study. In doing so, study 2 assessed happiness and
relationship quality with different instruments. Specifically, happiness was assessed
in terms of global life satisfaction, presence of positive affect and absence of negative
affect (Diener et al., 1999). In this point, it is essential to consider whether com-
ponents of subjective well-being (e.g., positive affect) might be different in different
age groups. As for life-satisfaction, considerable amount of research suggests that it
is stable across the life-span (Diener & Suh, 1998; Medley, 1980; see Diener et al.
(1999) for a review). The story is a bit different for positive and negative affect. First
of all, the affect scores of adolescents are comparable to those observed for adults
(Greene, 1990; Hussong, 2000). Second of all, research suggests that negative affect
declines across the life-span, at least until about age 60 (Carstensen, Pasupathi,
Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000; Diener & Suh, 1998). Longitudinal studies confirm these
findings (Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Stacey & Gatz, 1991; see Costa et al.
(1987) for an exception). That is, young adults report relatively higher levels of
negative affect compared to middle-aged and older adults. As for positive affect,
longitudinal studies suggest a stable pattern across the life-span (Charles et al., 2001;
Costa et al., 1987; Stacey & Gatz, 1991; see Diener and Suh (1998) and Lucas and
Gohm (2000) for cross-cultural comparisons). Overall, available literature could be
summarized as suggesting that young adults differ from older age groups in terms of
reporting higher levels of negative affect.
As for relationship quality, study 2 relied on McGill Friendship Questionnaire
(MFQ; Mendelson & Aboud, 1999) to assess theoretically identified features of
relationship: companionship, help, affection and intimacy, sense of reliable alliance,
emotional security and self-validation. One might question the practice of relying on
MFQ to assess romantic relationship quality. First of all, from a theoretical stand-
point, these features (e.g., intimacy) are also sought and satisfied in romantic rela-
tionships as well (Prager, 1995; Weiss, 1974). Second, other instruments in the field
are also used to assess multiple relationships including friendships and romantic
relationships (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). Considering these two points, the use of
MFQ to assess romantic relationship quality is justified. Overall, replicating the
findings of the first study with different instruments would suggest that the role of
relationship quality in happiness while controlling for personality is robust.
Different from the first study, study 2 also investigated the most important feature
of romantic relationship that predicted happiness. Overall, relationship quality might
be important for happiness but it could be that certain features of the relationship
make more important contributions than others in predicting happiness. For instance,
the studies reported by Demir and his colleagues (Demir & Weitekamp, in press;
Demir, Özdemir, & Weitekamp, in press) showed that even though overall quality
was related to happiness, companionship and self-validation features of friendship
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 265
were the best predictors of happiness. Considering this, study 2 attempted to identify
the strongest feature of romantic relationship quality that predicted happiness.
Study 2 aimed to extend the findings of the first study such that identity formation
was investigated as a potential moderator of the relationship between relationship
quality and happiness. Theory suggests that successful identity formation is related to
psychological well-being and promotes the formation of close relationships with
others (Erikson, 1982). Supporting theory, research has shown positive relationships
between successful identity formation and well-being (Meeus, Iedema, Hersen,
& Vollebergh, 1999; Vleioras & Bosma, 2005). Research also established a positive
association between identity formation and close relationships among adolescents
and emerging adults (Craig-Bray, Adams, & Dobson, 1988; Kacerquis & Adams,
1980; Orlofsky, Marcia, & Lesser, 1973). Research in this field could be summarized
as suggesting that identity formation has implications for the well-being and close
relationships of emerging adults. Study 2 investigated whether the degree of identity
formation moderated the relationship between romantic relationship quality and
happiness. Even though it might be possible to classify individuals with regards to
identity formation (e.g., moratorium; Marcia, 1966), theory suggests that identity
formation is a continuous process (Arnett, 2004; Erikson, 1982). This argument could
be taken to suggest that, especially during emerging adulthood, some might have a
clearer idea of who they are and what they want with their lives. Considering the
available research and theory, then, it is plausible to argue that the degree to which
one establishes his/her identity might influence the relationship between romantic
relationship quality and happiness. That is, those who are high in identity formation
might benefit more from the quality of their romantic relationships when compared to
those low in identity formation. Finding support for this argument would mean that
certain individuals might benefit more from the quality of their close relationships.
This has the potential to broaden certain theoretical arguments regarding the role of
identity in close relationships and happiness (e.g., Vleioras & Bosma, 2005).
3.1 Hypotheses
The predictions of the first study are also proposed here (e.g., romantic relationship
quality predicting happiness while controlling for personality). In an attempt to
prevent repetition, they are not specified again. Predictions pertinent to Study 2 are
reported below:
(a) Companionship and self-validation features of romantic relationship quality
would emerge as the strongest predictors of happiness.
(b) Identity formation would be positively related to happiness and romantic
relationship quality.
(c) Identity formation would moderate the relationship between romantic
relationship quality and happiness.
3.2 Method
3.2.1 Participants
The original sample consisted of 350 (222 women, 128 men) college students
attending a Midwestern university in the USA where the sample for Study 1 was
123
266 M. Demir
recruited. Of the original sample, 62% stated that there were in a romantic rela-
tionship (n = 217, 159 women, 58 men). The mean age of the sample was 22.58
(SD = 3.20), with a range from 18 to 29. The ethnic distribution of the sample was as
follows: 54% Caucasian (n = 117), 21% Black (n = 46), 5% Asian (n = 11) and 20%
other (n = 27). In the sample, 86% (n = 187) of the sample was involved in a dating
relationship and 14% (n = 30) were either engaged or married. The latter group
(e.g., engaged) was excluded from the sample in order to make possible comparisons
across studies. Thus, the final sample for the analyses reported below consisted of
187 (49 men) dating emerging adults with a mean age of 22.02 (SD = 3.02) and a
range from 18 to 29 years of age.
The average length of the dating relationships was 24.76 months (SD = 29.61)
with a range from 1 to 82 months.
3.2.2 Procedure
3.2.3 Measures
Table 3 Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix for the variables in study 2
M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Gender – – –
2. Extroversion 3.66 .64 .04 –
3. Agreeableness 3.87 .65 .14 .26** –
4. Neuroticism 2.64 .70 .17* –.31** –.20** –
5. Relationship quality 7.01 1.05 .08 .16* .15* –.03 –
6. Identity formation 3.91 .59 .29** .22** .26** –.09 .18* –
7. Positive affect 3.65 .58 –.08 .28** .28** –.23** .11 .16 –
8. Negative affect 2.09 .69 .11 –.38** .07 .46** –.15* –.30** –.07 –
9. Life satisfaction 4.69 1.33 .05 .16** .08 –.19** .29** .27** .27** –.13 –
10. Happiness –.12 1.09 .03 .40** .17* –.42** .30** .37** .68** –.59** .72** –
Furthermore, the same scale was also related to depression (negatively) in another
study (M. Demir, 2006, unpublished). Second of all, prior studies assessing multiple
relationships with the same scale report a positive association across relationships
(e.g., Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). In a separate study conducted at our lab (Demir,
2006), romantic relationship quality, as assessed with the MFQ (modified version as
used herein), was positively related to friendship quality. Even though these two
points might not provide the best examples of construct validity, the two points
raised above and the theoretical background of the scale justify the use of the
instrument to assess romantic relationship quality.
Personality. As in Study 1, personality was measured with the Big Five Inventory
(BFI) (John & Srivastava, 1999). Different from study 1, only extroversion (7 items),
neuroticism (8 items) and agreeableness (9 items) were assessed in study 2. This
practice stemmed from the findings of the first study such that only these three
personality dimensions emerged as predictors of happiness. The reliabilities of the
scales in study 2 were as follows: a = .87 for extroversion; a = .82 for agreeableness;
a = .85 for neuroticism.
Happiness. Happiness was assessed with the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS)
(Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) and the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Prior studies also relied on
these instruments to assess happiness (Sheldon, Kasser, Houser-Marko, Jones, &
Turban, 2005).
The SWLS assesses the global cognitive evaluations of one’s life. The scale
consists of five items and respondents were asked to rate their agreement with the
items on a 7-point scale (ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree.)
The mean scores for the five items was used to obtain a satisfaction with life com-
posite score. The reliability of the scale for the present study was high (a = .86).
The PANAS was used to assess general positive and negative affect. The PANAS
consists of 10 mood states for positive affect (PA) (e.g., attentive) and 10 for neg-
ative affect (NA) (e.g., hostile). Respondents were asked to rate the extent to which
they feel each mood in general on a 1 (very slightly or not all) to 5 (extremely) scale.
The mean scores for the ten positive and negative affect scores were used to create a
composite positive and negative affect scores. Reliabilities for the scales in the
present study were satisfactory (a = .82 for PA; a = .81 for NA). The means and
123
268 M. Demir
standard deviations reported for the measures (see Table 3) were similar to those
found in other research (Demir & Weitekamp, in press; Kasser & Sheldon, 2002).
In order to simplify the presentation of happiness, an aggregate happiness score
was created by standardizing the satisfaction with life, positive and negative affect
scores and subtracting the standardized negative affect scores from the sum of
standardized satisfaction with life and positive affect scores. Others also used the
same method in creating an aggregate happiness score (Kasser & Sheldon, 2002;
Sheldon & Kasser, 2001; Sheldon et al., 2005). This procedure resulted in scores that
ranged from –5.84 to 5.12. The range obtained for the happiness score was compa-
rable to that obtained in other studies (Demir et al., in press).
Identity Formation. The identity subscale of the Erikson Psycho-Social Index
(EPSI; Rosenthal, Gurney, & Moore, 1981) was used to assess identity formation.
Twelve items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from almost always
true (5) to almost never true (1), with half of the items representing successful res-
olution of the identity (versus confusion) crises and half representing unsuccessful
resolutions. Sample items include ‘‘I know what kind of person I am’’ and ‘‘The
important things in life are clearer to me.’’ Twelve items, after recoding the nega-
tively worded ones, were summed to create the Identity Formation composite score,
with high scores indicating higher identity formation. The reliability of the scale in
the current study was satisfactory (a = .88).
3.3 Results
The correlations between the study variables are reported in Table 3. As seen in the
table, relationships between the variables mostly supported the hypotheses. Of
particular importance, identity formation was positively related to happiness and
romantic relationship quality (hypothesis b of Study 2). In general, these findings are
in line with research (Argyle, 2001; Craig-Bray et al., 1988) and replicate the findings
obtained from Study 1.
Step 1
Gender .03
R2 = .00
Step 2
Extroversion .28*
Agreeableness .03
Neuroticism –.29*
DR2 = .25*
Step 3
Romantic relationship quality .24*
DR2 = .06*
Zero-order ba Semipartial2
two variables (identity · relationship quality) were entered in the second step. The
first step of the regression was significant and accounted for 19% of the variance
(F (2, 185) = 21.196, p < .05). Second step involving the interaction was also sig-
nificant (F (1, 184) = 2.989, p < .05) and accounted for an additional 1% of the
variance in happiness, supporting the hypothesis (c). The beta value for the inter-
action was .12. To aid in the interpretation of significant interaction effect, effects
were plotted and simple slopes were tested to evaluate if they differed significantly
from zero (Aiken &West, 1991). The interaction can be seen in Fig. 1. As seen from
the figure, high levels of romantic relationship quality was related to higher levels of
happiness at high levels of identity formation (t (183) = 3.74, p < .05) but not at low
levels of identity formation (t (183) = 1.82, ns). The interaction suggest that
emerging adults who are successful in forming their identity benefit more from the
quality of their romantic relationships.
3.4 Discussion
Study 2 replicated the findings obtained in the first study in that romantic rela-
tionship quality contributed to happiness above and beyond the influence of per-
sonality among emerging adults. Study 2 also assessed relationship quality with a
different instrument and examined the strongest feature predicting happiness.
Results revealed that emotional security and companionship were the strongest
0.50
Happiness
0.00
-0.50
-1.00
-1.50
low med high
Romantic Relationship Quality
Identity Formation high low
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 271
123
272 M. Demir
4 General discussion
One major conclusion that could be drawn from the studies reported is that the
quality of romantic relationships contributes to happiness above and beyond the
influence of personality among emerging adults. It is noteworthy that this finding was
obtained with different instruments to assess happiness and relationship quality. This
robust finding across the two studies suggests that the association between romantic
relationships and happiness cannot be accounted by personality. For instance, we
suggested earlier that extroverts’ close relationships with their romantic partners
might be attributed to their sensitivity to rewards and close relationships would not
appear a predictor of happiness. Findings of the present study and other studies done
with a focus on different relationships (Lu, 1999) suggest that this is not the case.
Moreover, theory and research pertaining to the importance of romantic
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 273
relationships in happiness were subjected to a strict test in two studies and findings
suggests that achievement and/or involvement in this normative developmental task
has implications for happiness even when controlling for personality. Overall, then,
even though one might have certain dispositions to be happy, experiencing high
quality close romantic relationships has the potential to make a difference and
contributes to one’s happiness.
In two studies, 3–6% of the variance was accounted for by the quality of romantic
relationships. We consider these findings important and suggest that one should not
equate the amount of variance explained as unimportant or disappointing. The
findings were obtained while controlling for one of the major predictor of happiness,
personality. The task of future research, we propose, is to investigate whether the
findings reported could be obtained in different age groups and while taking into
account different ways of assessing personality (e.g., self-monitoring).
The present studies were not free of limitations. First of all, even though we treated
romantic relationship quality as a predictor of happiness one cannot establish cause–
effect relationship with cross-sectional studies. It could be that being happy influences
the quality of romantic relationships. Nevertheless, the way we interpreted our find-
ings were consistent with the theoretical position we entertained (Ryan & Deci, 2001)
and with empirical research (Argyle, 2001). Second limitation involves not providing a
definition of romantic relationship to the participants. It is possible that some of the
romantic relationships assessed could be purely sexual in nature. Nevertheless, the
duration of the romantic relationships in both studies suggest that participants were
involved in romantic relationships for a considerable period of time. Third limitation is
that conflict experienced in romantic relationships was not assessed. As relationship
scholars argue (Hinde, 1997), conflict (e.g., frequency) is an inevitable part of any
relationship. Future research should assess conflict in romantic relationships and test
whether this dimension also influences the happiness of emerging adults while con-
trolling for personality. Another limitation of the present study was the ratio of wo-
men–men in the sample. There were more women than men in the analyses reported.
Studies relying on college students as their participants might inevitably face such a
problem. We believe the findings would not be different had equal number of men and
women participated in the study. This argument stems from the findings that romantic
relationships are important for both and men (Berscheid et al., 1989). Another limi-
tation was that participants across the two studies were involved in dating relation-
ships. It is possible that the findings obtained in the present studies might not be
generalizable to engaged/married adults. As such it is the task of future research to
conduct similar studies with samples of married adults. Finally, results reflect the
responses of a college-aged sample in a large Midwestern university and future re-
search might benefit greatly by benefit by conducting similar studies in different age
groups to establish confidence in the findings reported.
5 Conclusion
Two studies showed that the quality of romantic relationships contribute to the
happiness of emerging adults while controlling for personality. The findings suggest
123
274 M. Demir
Acknowledgement The author would like to thank to anonymous reviewers for their valuable
comments.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness. London: Routledge.
Argyle, M., & Henderson, M. (1985). The anatomy of relationships. London: Heinemann.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the
twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469–480.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from late teens through twenties. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Bagwell, C. L., Bender, S. E., Andreassi, C. L., Kinoshita, S. A., Montarello, S. A., & Muller, J. G.
(2005). Friendship quality and perceived relationship changes predict psychosocial adjustment in
early adulthood. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 22, 235–254.
Baldassare, M., Rosenfield, S., & Rook, K. S. (1984). The types of social relations predicting elderly
well-being. Research on Aging, 6, 549–559.
Baltes, B. B., Parker, C. P., Young, L. M., Huff, J., & Altmann, R. (2004). The practical utility of
importance measures in assessing the relative importance of work related perceptions and
organizational characteristics on work related outcomes. Organizational Research Methods, 7,
326–340.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachment
as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Berry, D. S., & Willingham, J. K. (1997). Affective traits, responses to conflict, and satisfaction in
romantic relationships. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 564–576.
Berscheid, E., & Regan, P. (2005). The psychology of interpersonal relationships. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Berscheid, E., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (1989). Issues in studying relationships: Conceptualizing
and measuring closeness. In C. Hendrick (Ed.), Close relationships: Vol. 10. Review of person-
ality and social psychology (pp. 63–91). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1988). Individual difference variables in close relationships: A
contextual model of marriage as an integrative framework. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 713–721.
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. (2000). Emotion experience in
everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644–
655.
Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive
and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 136–151.
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2001). Attributional style and personality as predictors of happiness and
mental health. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2, 307–327.
Compton, W. C. (1998). Measures of mental health and a five factor theory of personality. Psy-
chological Reports, 83, 371–381.
Corcoran, K., & Fischer, J. (2000). Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook, Vol. 2. New York,
NY: The Free Press.
Costa, P. T. Jr., Zonderman, A. B., McCrae, R. R., Cornoni-Huntley, J., Locke, B. Z., & Barbano, H.
E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of psychological well-being in a national sample: Stability of
mean levels. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 50–55.
Craig-Bray, L., Adams, G. R., & Dobson, W. R. (1988). Identity formation and social relations
during late adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 173–187.
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 275
Crossley, A., & Langridge, D. (2005). Perceived sources of happiness: A network analysis. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 6, 107–135.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: The uses of experience
sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 185–199.
Demir, M., Linley, A. P., Kumbul, B., & Grishkina, T. (2003). What makes people happy? A cross-
cultural comparison of British and Turkish young adults. Poster presented at the 2nd Interna-
tional Positive Psychology Summit, October 5–7, Washington, D.C., USA.
Demir, M., Özdemir, M., & Weitekamp, L. A. (2006). Looking to happy tomorrows with friends:
Best and close friendships as they predict happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, DOI 10.1007/
s10902-006-9025-x.
Demir, M., & Weitekamp, L. A. (2006). I am so happy ‘cause today I found my friend: Friendship
and personality as predictors of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, DOI 10.1007/s10902-
006-9012-7.
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality
traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197–229.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Gohm, C., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the relations between marital status
and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 419–436.
Diener, E., & Lucas, R. (1999). Personality, and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener,
& N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New
York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). Subjective well-being and age. An international perspective. Annual
Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 17, 304–324.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of
progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 76–302.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Smith, H. L., & Shao, L. (1995). National differences in reported well-being:
Why do they occur? Social Indicators Research, 34, 7–32.
Dush, C. M. K., & Amato, P. R. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for sub-
jective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 607–627.
Erikson, E. H. (1982). The life cycle completed: A review. New York: Norton.
Feiring, C. (1996). Concepts of romance in 15-year-olds. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 181–
200.
Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., & Thomas, G. (2000). The measurement of perceived relationship
quality components: A confirmatory factor analytic approach. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 26, 340–354.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of networks of
personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 105–115.
Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (1997). Personality and happiness. Psychological Reports, 80, 761–762.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go right?
The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing of positive events. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 87, 228–245.
Greene, A. L. (1990). Patterns of affectivity in the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 50, 340–356.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close
relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1–22.
Hinde, R. A. (1997). Relationships: A dialectical perspective. Psychology Press: Taylor & Francis Ltd.
Hussong, A. M. (2000). Perceived peer context and adolescent adjustment. Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 10, 391–415.
John, O., Donahue, E., & Kentle, R. (1991). The ‘‘Big Five’’ Inventory – Versions 4a and 54.
Technical Report. Institute of Personality Assessment and Research, Berkeley, CA: University
of California, Berkeley.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
and research (pp. 102–138). NewYork: Guilford.
Kacerquis, M. S., & Adams, G. R. (1980). Erikson stage resolution: The relationship between
identity and intimacy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, 117–126.
Kan, M. L., & Cares, A. C. (2006). From ‘‘friends with benefits’’ to ‘‘going steady’’: New directions in
understanding romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In A. C. Crouter &
123
276 M. Demir
A. E. Booth (Eds.), Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Risks and
opportunities (pp. 241–258). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A
review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34.
Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2002). What makes for a merry Christmas? Journal of Happiness
Studies, 3, 313–329.
Kelly, E. L., & Conley, J. J. (1987). Personality and compatibility: A prospective analysis of marital
stability and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 27–40.
Keyes, C. L. M., & Waterman, M. B. (2003). Dimensions of well-being and mental health in
adulthood. In M. H. Bornstein, L. Davidson, C. L. M. Keyes, & K. A. Moore (Eds.), Positive
development across the life course (pp. 477–497). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates, Publishers.
Khaleque, A. R., & Rohner, N. (2004). Intimate adult relationships, quality of life and psychological
adjustment. Social Indicators Research, 69, 351–360.
Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., & Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction:
Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
1038–1051.
Lokitz, B. D., & Sprandel, H. Z. (1976). The first year: A look at the freshman experience. Journal of
College Student Personnel, 17, 274–279.
Lu, L. (1999). Personal or environmental causes of happiness: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Social Psychology, 139, 79–90.
Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2001). Understanding extraverts’ enjoyment of social situations: The
importance of pleasantness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 343–356.
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the
fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 452–468.
Lucas, R. E., & Gohm, C. (2000). Age and sex differences in subjective well-being across cultures. In
E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Subjective well-being across nations and cultures (pp. 291–317).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefit of frequent positive affect: Does hap-
piness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803–855.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability
and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137–155.
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of
sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 3, 551–558.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1991). Adding liebe and arbeit: The full five-factor model and well-
being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227–232.
Medley, M. L. (1980). Life satisfaction across four stages of adult life. International Journal of Aging
and Human Development, 11, 193–209.
Meeus, W., Iedema, J., Hersen, M., & Vollebergh, W. (1999). Patterns of adolescent identity
development: Review of literature and longitudinal analysis. Developmental Review, 19, 419–
461.
Menard, S. (1995). Applied logistic regression analysis. Sage university paper series on quantitative
applications in the social sciences, 07-106. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mendelson, M. J., & Aboud, F. E. (1999). Measuring friendship quality in late adolescents and young
adults: McGill Friendship Questionnaires. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 31, 130–
132.
Miller, R. S., & Lefcourt, H. M. (1982). The assessment of social intimacy. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 46(5), 514–518.
Montgomery, M. J. (2005). Psychosocial intimacy and identity: From early adolescence to emerging
adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 346–374.
Myers, R. (1990). Classical and modern regression with applications (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Dux-
bury.
Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness: Who is happy and why. New York: William Morrow.
Myers, D. (2000). The funds, friends and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55, 56–67.
Noller, P., Feeney, J. A., Peterson, C. (2001). Personal relationships across the lifespan. Hove, UK:
Psychology Press.
Orlofsky, J. L., Marcia, J. E., & Lesser, I. M. (1973). Ego identity status and the intimacy versus
isolation crisis of young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 211–219.
123
Romantic relationship quality and happiness 277
123