What Is Science?
What Is Science?
What is science????
The word “science” probably brings to mind many different pictures: a fat textbook,
white lab coats and microscopes, an astronomer peering through a telescope, a
naturalist in the rainforest, Einstein’s equations scribbled on a chalkboard, the launch
of the space shuttle, bubbling beakers …. All of those images reflect some aspect of
science, but none of them provides a full picture because science has so many facets:
These images all show an aspect of science, but a complete view of science is more than any particular instance.
Diver photo provided by OAR/National Undersea Research Program (NURP); lab photo courtesy of Pacific Northwest National
Laboratory; photo of geologists on volcano by J.D. Griggs; photo of scientist in corn field by Scott Bauer; image of Mars
rover courtesy NASA/JPL-Caltech.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
2
Discovery: The spark for science…
“Eureka!” or “aha!” moments
may not happen frequently, but
they are often experiences that
drive science and scientists. For
a scientist, every day holds the
possibility of discovery—of
coming up with a brand new
idea or of observing something
that no one has ever seen before. Vast
bodies of knowledge have yet to be built and many of the most basic questions about
the universe have yet to be answered:
• What causes gravity?
• How do tectonic plates move around on Earth’s surface?
• How do our brains store memories?
• How do water molecules interact with each other?
We don’t know the complete answers to these and an overwhelming number of other
questions, but the prospect of answering them beckons science forward.
Discoveries, new questions, and new ideas are what keep scientists going and
awake at night, but they are only one part of the picture; the rest involves a lot
of hard (and sometimes tedious) work. In science, discoveries and ideas must be
verified by multiple lines of evidence and then integrated into the rest of science,
a process which can take many years. And often, discoveries are not bolts from
the blue. A discovery may itself be the result of many years of work on a
particular problem, as illustrated by Henrietta Leavitt’s stellar discovery …
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
3
Photo of Spiral Galaxy M81 provided by NASA, ESA, and The Hubble Heritage Team (STScI/AURA); photo of water
provided by Andrew Davidhazy.
STELLAR SURPRISES
Astronomers had long known about the existence of
variable stars—stars whose brightness changes over time,
slowly shifting between brilliant and dim—when, in 1912,
Henrietta Leavitt announced a remarkable (and totally
unanticipated) discovery about them. For these stars, the
length of time between their brightest and dimmest points
seemed to be related to their overall brightness: slower
cycling stars are more luminous. At the time, no one knew
why that was the case, but nevertheless, the discovery allowed astronomers
Henrietta Leavitt
to infer the distances to far-off stars, and hence, to figure out the
size of our own galaxy. Leavitt’s observation was a true surprise—a discovery in
the classic sense—but one that came only after she’d spent years carefully
comparing thousands of photos of these specks of light, looking for patterns in the
darkness.
The process of scientific discovery is not limited to professional scientists working in
labs. The everyday experience of deducing that your car won’t start because of a bad
fuel pump, or of figuring out that the centipedes in your backyard prefer shady rocks
shares fundamental similarities with classically scientific discoveries like working out
DNA’s double helix. These activities all involve making observations and analyzing
evidence—and they all provide the satisfaction of finding an answer that makes sense
of all the facts. In fact, some psychologists argue that the way individual humans
learn (especially as children) bears a lot of similarity to the progress of science: both
involve making observations, considering evidence, testing ideas, and holding on to
those that work.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
4
Photo of Henrietta Leavitt provided by the American Association of Variable Star Observers (AAVSO).
A science checklist
So what, exactly, is science? Well, science turns out to be difficult to define precisely.
(Philosophers have been arguing about it for decades!) The problem is that the term
“science” applies to a remarkably broad set of human endeavors, from developing
lasers, to analyzing the factors that affect human decision-making.
To get a grasp on what science is, we’ll look at a checklist that summarizes key
characteristics of science and compare it to a prototypical case of science in action:
Ernest Rutherford’s investigation into the structure of the atom. Then, we’ll look at
some other cases that are less “typical” examples of science to see how they measure
up and what characteristics they share.
This checklist provides a guide for what sorts of activities are encompassed by
science, but since the boundaries of science are not clearly defined, the list should not
be interpreted as all-or-nothing. Some of these characteristics are particularly
important to science (e.g., all of science must ultimately rely on evidence), but others
are less central. For example, some perfectly scientific investigations may run into a
dead end and not lead to ongoing research. Use this checklist as a reminder of the
usual features of science. If something doesn’t meet most of these characteristics, it
shouldn’t be treated as science.
Science asks questions about the
natural world
Science studies the natural world. This in-
cludes the components of the physical
universe around us like atoms, plants, eco-
systems, people, societies and galaxies, as
well as the natural forces at work on those
things. In contrast, science cannot study su-
pernatural forces and explanations. For ex-
ample, the idea that a supernatural afterlife
exists is not a part of science since this af-
terlife operates outside the rules that govern
the natural world.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
5
Cococino National Forest photo by Gerald and Buff Corsi © California Academy of Sciences; Jupiter photo by
NASA/JPL/ Space Science Institute; photo of smoggy skyline by EPA; fungus photo by Dr. Robert Thomas and
Dorothy B. Orr © California Academy of Sciences.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
6
Rutherford photo from the Library of Congress.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
7
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
8
Science works with testable ideas
Only testable ideas are within the purview of
science. For an idea to be testable, it must
logically generate specific expectations— in
other words, a set of observations that we
could expect to make if the idea were true
and a set of observations that would be
inconsistent with the idea and lead you to
believe that it is not true. For example,
consider the idea that a sparrow’s song is
genetically encoded and is unaffected by the
environment in which it is raised, in
comparison to the idea that a sparrow learns
the song it hears as a baby. Logical
reasoning about this example leads to a
specific set of expectations. If the sparrow’s
song were indeed genetically encoded, we
would expect that a sparrow raised in the
nest of a different species would grow up to sing a
sparrow song like any other member of its own species. But if, instead, the sparrow’s
song were learned as a chick, raising a sparrow in the nest of another species should
produce a sparrow that sings a non-sparrow song. Because they generate different
expected observations, these ideas are testable. A scientific idea may require a lot of
reasoning to work out an appropriate test, may be difficult to test, may require the
development of new technological tools to test, or may require one to make
independently testable assumptions to test—but to be scientific, an idea must be
testable, somehow, someway.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
9
idea? No. Because this supernatural being is all-powerful, anything we observe could
be chalked up to the whim of that being. Or not. The point is that we can’t use the
tools of science to gather any information about whether or not this being exists—so
such an idea is outside the realm of science.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
10
Science relies on evidence
Ultimately, scientific ideas must not only be
testable, but must actually be tested—
preferably with many different lines of
evidence by many different people. This
characteristic is at the heart of all science.
Scientists actively seek evidence to test their
ideas—even if the test is difficult and means,
for example, spending years working on a
single experiment, traveling to Antarctica to
measure carbon dioxide levels in an ice core,
or collecting DNA samples from thousands of
volunteers all over the world. Performing
such tests is so important to science because
in science, the acceptance or rejection of a
scientific idea depends upon the evidence
relevant to it—not upon dogma, popular
opinion, or tradition. In science, ideas that
are not supported by evidence are ultimately rejected. And
ideas that are protected from testing or are only allowed to be tested by one group
with a vested interest in the outcome are not a part of good science.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
11
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
12
Science is embedded in the scientific
community
The progress of science depends on
interactions within the scientific community—
that is, the community of people and
organizations that generate scientific ideas,
test those ideas, publish scientific journals,
organize conferences, train scientists,
distribute research funds, etc. This scientific
community provides the cumulative
knowledge base that allows science to build
on itself. It is also responsible for the further
testing and scrutiny of ideas and for
performing checks and balances on the work
of community members.
In addition, much scientific research is
collaborative, with different people bringing
their specialized knowledge to bear on
different aspects of the problem. For example, a
2006 journal article on regional variations in
the human genome was the result of a collaboration between 43 people from the U.K.,
Japan, the U.S., Canada, and Spain! Even Charles Darwin, who initially investigated
the idea of evolution through natural selection while living almost as a hermit at his
country estate, kept up a lively correspondence with his peers, sending and receiving
numerous letters dealing with his ideas and the evidence relevant to them.
In rare cases, scientists do actually work
in isolation. Gregor Mendel, for example,
figured out the basic principles of
genetic inheritance as a secluded monk
with very little scientific interaction.
However, even in such cases, research
must ultimately involve the scientific
community if that work is to have any
impact on the progress of science. In
Mendel’s case, the ultimate involvement
of the scientific community through his
published work was critical because it
allowed other scientists to evaluate
those ideas independently, investigate
new lines of evidence, and develop
extensions of his ideas. This community
process may be chaotic and slow, but it
is also crucial to the progress of science.
Scientists sometimes work alone and sometimes work
together, but communication within the scientific
community is always important.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
13
experiment was actually carried out by
Ernest Marsden, an undergraduate
student working in Rutherford’s lab.
Furthermore, after his discovery of the
Ernest Rutherford (right) and Hans Geiger
layout of the atom, Rutherford published in the physics laboratory at Manchester a
description of the idea and the relevant University, England, circa 1912. Permission
evidence, releasing it to the scientific of the Alexander Turnbull Library, Wellington,
community for scrutiny and evaluation. New Zealand, must be obtained before any re-use
of this image. Reference number: And scrutinize they did. Niels Bohr no- PAColl-0091-1-
011. ticed a problem with Rutherford’s idea:
there was nothing keeping the orbiting electrons from spiraling into the nucleus of
the atom, causing the whole thing to collapse! Bohr modified Rutherford’s basic
model by proposing that electrons had set energy levels, which helped solve the
problem and earned Bohr a Nobel Prize. Since then, many other scientists have
built on and modified Bohr’s model.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
14
answer one question, it also generated new expectations (e.g., that DNA is copied
via base pairing), raised many new questions (e.g., how does DNA store
information?), and contributed to whole new fields of research (e.g., genetic
engineering). Like Watson and Crick’s work, most scientific research generates new
expectations, inspires new questions, and leads to new discoveries.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
15
Participants in science behave scientifically
But what exactly does one have to do to behave scientifically? Here is a scientist’s
code of conduct:
1) Pay attention to what other people have already done. Scientific knowledge
is built cumulatively. If you want to discover exciting new things, you need to
know what people have already discovered before you. This means that scientists
study their fields extensively to understand the current state of knowledge.
2) Expose your ideas to testing. Strive to describe and perform the tests that
might suggest you are wrong and/or allow others to do so. This may seem like
shooting yourself in the foot but is critical to the progress of science. Science
aims to accurately understand the world, and if ideas are protected from testing,
it’s impossible to figure out if they are accurate or inaccurate!
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
16
3) Assimilate the evidence. Evidence is the ultimate arbiter of scientific ideas.
Scientists are not free to ignore evidence. When faced with evidence
contradicting his or her idea, a scientist may suspend judgment on that idea
pending more tests, may revise or reject the idea, or may consider alternate
ways to explain the evidence, but ultimately, scientific ideas are sustained by
evidence and cannot be propped up if the evidence tears them down.
4) Openly communicate ideas and tests to others. Communication is important
for many reasons. If a scientist keeps knowledge to her- or himself, others
cannot build upon those ideas, double-check the work, or devise new ways to
test the ideas.
5) Play fair: Act with scientific integrity. Hiding evidence, selectively reporting
evidence, and faking data directly thwart science’s main goal—to construct
accurate knowledge about the natural world. Hence, maintaining high standards
of honesty, integrity, and objectivity is critical to science.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
17
Beyond physics, chemistry, and biology
We’ve seen that scientific research generally
meets a set of key characteristics: it focuses
on improving our understanding of the
natural world, works with testable ideas that
can be verified with evidence, relies on the
scientific community, inspires ongoing
research, and is performed by people who
behave scientifically. While not all scientific
investigations line up perfectly with the
Science Checklist, science, as an endeavor,
strives to embody these features. Ernest
Rutherford’s discovery of the atomic nucleus,
for example, satisfied those characteristics
quite neatly. But how would a less
stereotypically “scientific” investigation—one
that wouldn’t show up in a high school
science textbook— measure up against the
Science Checklist? To find out, we’ll look at an example from the field of psychology …
Beyond the prototype: Animal psychology
Most of us have probably wondered how other animals think and experience the world
(e.g, is Fido really happy to see me or does he just want a treat?)—but can that
curiosity be satisfied by science? After all, how could we ever test an idea about how
another animal thinks? In the 1940s, psychologist Edward Tolman investigated a
related question using the methods of science. He wanted to know how rats
successfully navigate their surroundings—for example, a maze containing a hidden
reward. Tolman suspected that rats would build mental maps of the maze as they
investigated it (forming a mental picture of the layout of the maze), but many of his
colleagues thought that rats would learn to navigate the maze through stimulus-
response, associating particular cues with particular outcomes (e.g., taking this tunnel
means I get a piece of cheese) without forming any big picture of the maze.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
18
twisted and turned but consistently led to the reward, and the rats quickly learned to
go down that tunnel. Then the experimenters blocked the entrance to the reward
tunnel. What would the rats do? Tolman reasoned that if the rats were navigating with
a mental map, they would pick another tunnel that, according to their mental map of
the maze, led in the direction of the food. But if the rats were navigating via
stimulusresponse, Tolman reasoned that they would choose the tunnel closest to the
original reward tunnel, regardless of where it led, since that was closest to the
stimulus with the pay-off.
Relies on evidence?
Tolman and his colleagues tested the mental map idea with several experiments,
including the tunnel experiment described above. In that experiment, they found that
most of the rats picked a tunnel that led in the direction of the food, instead of one
close to the original reward tunnel. The evidence supported the idea that rats navigate
using something like a mental map.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
19
Scientific community?
Tolman published many papers on this topic in scientific journals in order to explain
his experiments and the evidence relevant to them to other psychologists.
Ongoing research?
This research is a small part of a much larger body of ongoing psychological research
about how organisms learn and make decisions based on their representations of the
world.
Scientific behavior?
Edward Tolman and his colleagues acted with scientific integrity and behaved in ways
that push science forward. They accurately reported their results and allowed others
to test their ideas.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
20
Science in disguise
Our Science Checklist fits well with a wide range of
investigations—from developing an Alzheimer’s drug, to
dissecting the structure of atoms, to probing the neurology of
human emotion. Even endeavors far from one’s typical picture
of science, like figuring out how best to teach English as a
second language or examining the impact of a government
deficit on the economy, can be addressed by science.
Disguised as science
However, other human endeavors, which might at first seem
Teaching is an example of a
challenge that can be
like science, are actually not very much like science at all. For
addressed by science.
example, the Intelligent Design movement promotes the idea
that many aspects of life are too complex to have evolved
without the intervention of an intelligent cause—assumed by most proponents to be a
supernatural being, like God. Promoters of this idea are interested in explaining what
we observe in the natural world (the features of living things), which does align well
with the aims of science. However, because Intelligent Design relies on the action of
an unspecified “intelligent cause,” it is not a testable idea. Furthermore, the
movement itself has several other characteristics that reveal it to be non-science.
Western astrology aims to explain and predict events on Earth in terms of the
positions of the sun, planets, and constellations; hence, like science, astrology focuses
on explaining the natural world. However, in many other ways, astrology is not much
like science at all.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
21
how the world is, but it cannot make any judgments about whether that state of
affairs is right, wrong, good, or bad.
Science doesn’t make aesthetic
judgments
Science can reveal the frequency of a
G-flat and how our eyes relay
information about color to our brains,
but science cannot tell us whether a
Beethoven symphony, a Kabuki
performance, or a Jackson Pollock
painting is beautiful or dreadful. Individuals make those decisions for themselves
based on their own aesthetic criteria.
Science doesn’t tell you how to use
scientific knowledge
Although scientists often care deeply
about how their discoveries are used,
science itself doesn’t indicate what should
be done with scientific knowledge.
Science, for example, can tell you how to
recombine DNA in new ways, but it
doesn’t specify whether you
should use that knowledge to correct a genetic disease, develop a bruise-resistant
apple, or construct a new bacterium. For almost any important scientific advance, one
can imagine both positive and negative ways that knowledge could be used. Again,
science helps us describe how the world is, and then we have to decide how to use
that knowledge.
Science doesn’t draw conclusions
about supernatural explanations
Do gods exist? Do supernatural entities
intervene in human affairs? These
questions may be important, but science
won’t help you answer them. Questions
that deal with supernatural explanations
are, by definition, beyond the realm of
nature—
and hence, also beyond the realm of what can be studied by science. For many, such
questions are matters of personal faith and spirituality.
Moral judgments, aesthetic judgments, decisions about applications of science, and
conclusions about the supernatural are outside the realm of science, but that doesn’t
mean that these realms are unimportant. In fact, domains such as ethics, aesthetics,
and religion fundamentally influence human societies and how those societies interact
with science. Neither are such domains unscholarly. In fact, topics like aesthetics,
morality, and theology are actively studied by philosophers, historians, and other
scholars. However, questions that arise within these domains generally cannot be
resolved by science.
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org
22
Science in sum
In this section, we’ve seen that, though
hard to define concisely, science has a
handful of key features that set it apart
from other areas of human knowledge.
However, the net cast by science is wide.
The Science Checklist matches up to a
diverse set of human endeavors—from
uncovering the fundamental particles of
the universe, to studying the mating
behavior of lobsters, to investigating the
effects of different economic policies.
We’ve also seen that science has limits:
some questions that are an important
part of the human experience are not
answerable within the context of science.
So science isn’t everything, but it is
important. Science helps us construct
knowledge about the natural world—knowledge that
can then be harnessed to improve our lives and solve problems. How does science do
it? To find out, read on …
© 2013 The University of California Museum of Paleontology, Berkeley, and the Regents of the University of California • www.understandingscience.org