Paulos Numeracy Humor
Paulos Numeracy Humor
2015
Part of the Mathematics Commons, and the Rhetoric and Composition Commons
Recommended Citation
Grawe, Paul H.. "Mathematics and Humor: John Allen Paulos and the Numeracy Crusade." Numeracy 8,
Iss. 2 (2015): Article 11. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11
Authors retain copyright of their material under a Creative Commons Non-Commercial Attribution 4.0 License.
Mathematics and Humor: John Allen Paulos and the Numeracy Crusade
Abstract
John Allen Paulos at minimum gave the Numeracy movement a name through his book Innumeracy:
Mathematical Illiteracy and Its Consequences. What may not be so obvious was Paulos’ strong interest in
the relationship between mathematics and mathematicians on the one hand and humor and stand-up-
comedian joke structures on the other. Innumeracy itself could be seen as a typically mathematical
Gotcha joke on American culture generally. In this perspective, a Minnesotan acculturated to Minnesota-
Nice Humor of Self-Immolation Proclivities (SImP) looks at the more raw-boned, take-no-prisoners humor
style Paulos outlined in Mathematics and Humor and implemented in Innumeracy.
Despite the difference in humor styles, there is much to applaud in Paulos’ analysis of the relationship
between certain types of humor and professional interests of mathematicians in Mathematics and
Humor. Much humor relies on the sense of incongruity which Paulos’ claims to be central to all humor and
key to mathematical reductio ad absurdum. Mathematics is rightfully famous for a sense of
combinatorial playfulness in its most elegant proofs, as humor often relies on clashing combinations of
word play. And a great range of mathematical lore is best understood within a concept of a sudden drop
from one sense of certainty to another (essentially a Gotcha on the audience). Innumeracy repeatedly
exemplifies Gotchas on the great unwashed and unmathematical majority.
Extensive empirical evidence over the last quarter century allows us to synthesize these Paulos
observations into the idea that inculcated mathematical humor has strong propensities to complex
Intellectual, Advocate, and Crusader humor forms. However, the Paulos humors do not include the
Sympathetic Pain humor form, the inclusion of which may increase teaching effectiveness.
Keywords
humor, jokes, comedian
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 2
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
a tow truck pulling a tow truck, pulling a tow truck. The infinite regress created
by facing mirrors has its own fascinating humor about it, and that humorous
reality is probably more explored in mathematics than in any other branch of
learning.
Jokes also rely strongly on axiomatic thought, on levels of diction, often on
self-reference, paradox, and grammar. All of these are shared interests with
mathematics.
In Chapter 5, Paulos becomes technically mathematical in the construction of
a “Catastrophe Theory Model of Jokes and Humor.” By this point, the equation
of jokes and humor is totally explicit. Moreover, what makes a joke has
essentially become that we as audience are led to expect one verbal result when,
in fact, the joke leads to an entirely different conclusion. Knock-knock jokes are,
of course, typically this sort of “brain-teaser,” trick problem, misdirectional joke.
Paulos’ model assumes a three-dimensional joke space, though presumably it
could in theory be extended to four-dimensional space and beyond. The x- and y-
axes represent intensity of separate emotions or other psychological inputs. The z
dimension represents resulting behavior.
For certain kinds of functions relating these three variables, mathematicians
rely on Thom’s Theorem and the conclusion that such functions when graphed
“give rise to the same general shape” (80). (As a non-mathematician, I had always
assumed that this was always true for all functions, that functions of the same
type, as graphed, always give rise to the same general shape.)
In any case, the general shape of Thom’s Catastrophe Theorem functions is
something like a split plane, one part of the plane ascending more rapidly than a
second part, so that at high enough degrees of intensity of x and y, it is almost like
there are two entirely different planes. But then, the higher plane has a projecting
node, nose, protuberance, or whatever, that hooks back over the lower plane. (To
form such a beak takes a curve in two-dimensional space that curves back rather
than proceeds forward.) For a small area out at the beak of this hooking
phenomenon, there is evidently no reason why one should prefer a solution on the
upper plane to a solution on the lower plane. And thus, it is possible to “fall from
the higher to the lower plane,” which seems a reasonable model of falling for a
joke, meaning moving from a misdirected interpretation to a second, often more
mundane conclusion.
“Knock, knock,” “Orange,” “—Orange who?” “Orange you glad I’m not
going to tell another one?”
A great deal of mathematical insight is probably related to this catastrophic
sense. For example, an instructor may ask, “What is the probability that the
second card in a standard deck is the Ace of Spades?” The joke, which is
supposed to educate, is that it is the same probability for the 47th card or for the
first card.
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 4
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
A famous example Paulos himself discusses is of the little and naughty boy
asked to stay after school for his sins and then asked to add up all the integers
between 1 and 1000. Unfortunately, the boy turned out to be one of the geniuses
of mathematics and came back with the answer in two minutes. The higher plane
here was to sit and add 1, 2, 3, … 1000. The naughty little boy took the short-cut
of inventing the formula for such addition of integers.
Paulus also cites the joke of the bird flying between two trains initially
starting 300 miles apart and both traveling at 50 miles an hour until they collide,
crushing the bird, in the middle. How far did the bird fly before being crushed?
You can work at it all day adding bird distances, but the short-cut, the catastrophe,
is to realize that the trains will collide in exactly three hours, so the bird will have
traveled its speed times three hours.
we as audience are led to follow the lead, to tell ourselves that we, of course,
recognize and sympathize with the pain and suffering of virtually anyone. (In
terms of Thom’s Theorem functions, we are being led up the primrose path to the
higher plane of our and journalistic superiority over GM and other Neanderthals.)
And then, a second perspective, the unexpected perspective of industrial
scientists carefully measuring probabilities and calculating for all our good, is
worked into the text.
As we say in the humor trade, Gotcha!
This curving back (remember the curve of a Thom’s Theorem function)
throws us for an emotional loop, and we end up falling from our previous
sympathy with journalists and their portrayed victims to sympathizing with GM
and its perhaps righteous intent. (The joke has been sprung; the misdirection is
thoroughly resolved to a second plane of everyone professional having the benefit
of the doubt of conscientiously doing her or his job.)
By appropriate word choice, grammar, timing, level, and the like, Paulos can
make any such chapter more or less of an outright joke, more or less of a virulent
humorous attack. In Innumeracy, the joke form is often quite apparent because it
is we, the innumerate majority who are, in fact, the butts of the grand joke, the
great unwashed in need of conversion to thinking in mathematics and getting Got
when we don’t. Throughout, Paulos is a master “spin doctor,” very carefully
calculating and manipulating the seriousness or the humorousness of texts which
are essentially analyzable under Thom’s Theorem.
So if you want to understand the writing talent of Innumeracy and of A
Mathematician Reads the Newspaper, Mathematics and Humor is an insightful
guide written years in advance.
And Paulos’ writing talent, we can hope, will be remembered as inciting—or
shaming—Americans into mathematical literacy amidst the computational
complexities of modern popular culture.
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 6
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
A joke that leaves Republicans laughing in the aisles will typically only hurt
the Democrat in the back row, and the same is true for a Democratic joke with
either an impotent or non-present Republican butt.
Since we are talking a primarily academic area of humor, mathematician
humor, it should be explicitly clear that a college classroom is a wonderfully
secure place for the tenured professor to tell or do jokes on defenseless students.
The art of telling a joke over the supine bodies of those who cannot afford to
object is a neglected branch of humor study generally, but it is a habit-forming
reality, as, for example, in the motivational speaker I once heard who first asked
his Winona, Minnesota audience (proud of its long Polish heritage) if there were
any Poles in the room and then went on to announce he was telling a Polish joke
anyway. (I left before finding out exactly how anti-Polish the speaker was willing
to be in this amusing situation.)
In recent centuries, we have liked to think of ourselves as civilized. In
discussions of humor, it is important, however, to remember basic human nature
and inconvenient facts like the Roman Coliseum often being filled with laughter
as gladiators were skewered. Comedian Sid Caesar based his entire career on pure
pain humor. Cartoonist Al Capp wrote at length about pain humor underlying his
own considerable achievements in Li’l Abner (Capp 1950).
Paulos seems quite at ease with pain humor, though distinctions between
various types of pain humor are not central to his discussion. He himself
emphasizes Freud’s contributions to humor theory as I must as well.
Within a hard-headed recognition of pain as related to humor, Sigmund Freud
is given the credit for articulating pain and Gotcha humor as tri-furcated rather
than bi-furcated joke forms (Freud 1960). The aggressive joke has 1) a teller or
author, 2) an authenticating audience, and 3) a butt. The joke only succeeds if the
audience is willing to authenticate it.
In this sense, most pain humor, including Gotcha humor, has something of a
bullying character. The joke author gangs up with an authenticating audience and
pummels the butt. At least in theory, there is no limit on the amount of bullying
and pummeling inflicted. In years since Paulos wrote, fatal attacks on
schoolchildren by other students as well as student suicides have often focused on
pummeling, bullying pain humor as possible contributory causes for such
calamities. There is nothing in humor theory that makes such claims outlandish.
If Paulos at all dissents from a Freudian analysis here, it is in silence.
However, from his comments on humor in Beyond Numeracy, it often seems that
he is describing mathematicians making jokes at the expense of their classes
where the primary authenticating audience is the mathematician joke author him
or herself.
For example, Paulos cites the mathematician who seemed always to teach to
a single exemplary and cooperative student. One day, when the instructor entered
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 8
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
the class, it was evident that the exemplary student had somehow been detained
from attendance that day. The mathematician turned around and left the class
without any instruction, muttering to himself that there was no one present to
listen. Perhaps it was not originally meant as a joke, but if it was, the joke author
and authenticating audience were one.
Coming from Minnesota where Minnesota Nice is something of a cultural
imperative or prerequisite, I am aware that we Minnesotans are leery of pain
humor and don’t appreciate tri-furcation any more than bi-furcation of jokes and
instead try to uni-furcate our humor, telling jokes only on ourselves, something of
a Self-Immolation Proclivity (SImP) of the Upper Midwest.
These strenuous Minnesotan SImP attempts do not refute Freud. Uni-furcating
Minnesotans are only joining the teller, the authenticating audience, and the butt
into one and the same person in an attempt to soften the joke. For those interested
in uni-furcation, I suggest a summer or fall visit, and decidedly not a dead-of-
winter visit to Minnesota when even uni-furcation often goes into hibernation due
to cabin fever.
Combinational Play and Humor Personality
If we’ve taken pain in humor seriously, we may be queasy about humor
“personality.” As Robin Jaeckle Grawe and I have defined it technically, humor
personality is a combination of humor forms. For example, if someone we know
is forever telling raunchy sex jokes and graphic excremental jokes, we could
analyze that humor. But I think we are all aware that we wouldn’t leave it there.
We’d press the analysis to an analysis of the person. And we’d almost inevitably
come up with some statement like, “He has a smutty personality.”
Now Paulos and many others have pointed out that mathematics often finds
combinational play central to mathematical genius. It is also central to literary
genius.
But even before we get further into combinational play, let us note that
certainly since Freud and probably forever before him, pain humor taken
separately from all other humor gets associated with bullying and pummeling. So
again, it pays to pay very special attention to pain features in humor.
But now note that the last section of the present discussion seemed to wander.
It had been talking about Gotcha and other pain humors. But it ended up talking
about some mythical beast call uni-furcation.
“Uni-furcation” is not an example of pain or Gotcha humor; it is an example
of Word Play humor. We were right to notice a shift because human beings are
sensitive not just to humor in general but to discrepancies between forms of
humor.
What happens when Word Play and Gotcha are used in combination? We’ve
written at length about these combinations of mental humor forms in great
American motion pictures. For Gotcha and Word Play, we wrote a full chapter on
Disney’s Aladdin (Grawe, P. et. al. 2008). Word Play is particularly prominent in
Robin Williams’ unforgettable Genie. Gotcha is the entire direction of the plot
landing Jaffer back in the cave Genie emerged from and back in a bottle as well—
Gotcha!
We’ve called this combination of Gotcha and Word Play Advocate humor
personality. Advocate has typically a no-holds-barred, my-way-or-the-highway
feel. Aladdin, after all, is a boy of the streets. He’s out to marry Jasmine, the
sultan’s daughter. There’s nothing coy or shy about his and Genie’s approach, nor
is there anything coy about Genie’s desire to be free of his bottle. The Advocate
personality also routinely suggests an a priori superiority. Why should Aladdin
cow tow to rules of a civil society? Why shouldn’t he make a good college try for
a sultan’s daughter? The superiority isn’t stated, it is enacted. And Advocate
personality moves strongly toward the done deal, signed, sealed, and delivered,
including a certified superiority of the Advocate.
Multiple uses of the same humor form are important in establishing humor
personality. “Uni-furcation” is Word Play. “SImP” is another. Probably neither
one is very hurtful to anyone in particular. Some would argue that failure to inflict
pain makes these Word Plays only half-jokes or half-funny. One way or another,
the repetition of joke form works to develop humor personality.
Now when someone chooses to emphasize Word Play and Incongruity over
other mental humors, as Paulos suggests mathematicians do, that is what our
empirical research shows to be Intellectual humor, a concern with words and
ideas and not with people. So from a humor-of-the-mind perspective, Paulos
seems to suggest that mathematicians are very Intellectual in their humor, a
suggestion which seems clearly supported in all of Paulos’ work.
But Paulos’ catastrophe theory is essentially Gotcha in orientation, typically
dependent on a Word Play like “Orange” for “Aren’t ya” to prick the balloon and
send us in free-fall down to the lower plane of pedestrian meaning. And Paulos’
later works show strong Gotcha tendencies as well.
As already indicated, Gotcha and Word Play preferences create an Advocate
humor personality, and Paulos often seems to suggest an inherent advocacy of
personal superiority among mathematicians. (See Beyond Numeracy, 78-81 inter
alia.)
Gotcha and Incongruity, as a third possible mathematical combination
(remember Paulos’ emphasis on combination for good mathematical proofs),
create a Crusader humor personality. Innumeracy can rightfully claim to have
launched a crusade–or at least a “campaign”—for mathematics literacy, and in
general, quantification has been a long-term and iffy crusade since the early
Greeks.
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 10
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
Another Sympathetic Pain joke was a Reader’s Digest joke decades ago
about a harried young mother toting three toddlers through a museum cafeteria
line. Getting to the desserts, she says to the server, “Three gelatins—and please,
make them all the same color.”
For further insight on Sympathetic Pain, we’d suggest the comedies of Gentle
Will Shakespeare. He didn’t get the appellation for nothing.
Currently, the most impressive comedy of Shakespeare among American
audiences seems to be Midsummer Night’s Dream. In Cheshire Smile (Grawe, P.
et al. 2014), we devote a chapter to Midsummer and find it predominantly a
combination of Gotcha and Sympathetic Pain—a tough combination. But it does
marvelous things, which we call Bridgebuilder. Think about Bottom, that leading-
man actor who thinks he can play any part, who then gets an ass’s head, literally
instead of just figuratively. That’s Gotcha for you!
But—and that’s always near the center of Shakespearean genius—BUT,
we’ve also come to like Bottom. And thus, however incongruously, “We know
exactly how he feels (!)” when he yearns for a good peck of oats. Midsummer
Night’s Dream bridges so very much, the workaday world and the supernatural,
royalty and mechanic players, lovers as idiots and lovers as ideals.
Mathematician Lewis Carroll’s Alice in Wonderland as children’s story
(rather than as symbolic or allegorical adult satire) is also internationally beloved
for its Sympathetic Pain humor ambiance: “Off with their heads!!” “Yikes!!”
Alice, I know exactly how you feel!
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 12
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
It is easy to see that this is a trick question, and that mathematicians in their
profession often have to find the trick in order to get a problem solved.
Unfortunately, as discussed earlier, it comes off as a pain joke on the student who
doesn’t immediately get the point.
Is there an alternative humor approach—which turns out to also be an
alternate teaching approach? And particularly a Sympathetic Pain approach?
This question should be answered, ‘sure,” assuredly. Unfortunately, the long-
term neglect of Sympathetic Pain as a legitimate humor form makes the doing
something of a trick. A little practice, like practicing the trick of the bird-train
problem, should make it much easier to extemporize in a Sympathetic Pain humor
vein.
The teacher sets up the back-and-forth flight situation. She or he then suggests
what would be involved in calculating the first step, when will Bird meet Train B.
Doing that mathematics can be an interesting review of formulating and solving
simultaneous equations.
But then the instructor stops, and says something like, “Oh, golly, this is
painful. Maybe more for you than for me, because I get a certain kick out of
solving simultaneous equations. But look how much time this is taking, and we
haven’t even gotten to the part about what Train A’s been doing while we worry
about Bird and Train B. And you know what? This could go on forever, because
Bird always has to fly back to the other train and we have to calculate just how
long that takes even if at the end he is flying back and forth in nano-seconds. Oh,
my heart bleeds tears for us undertaking such a gruesome assignment! Isn’t there
any help available to us?”
From there, students should already be smiling, knowing exactly how the
instructor feels, probably even about sin and Purgatory.
And when it turns out that mathematicians are lazy and would just like quick
answers, they know how that feels as well.
Moreover, when it turns out that the answer is as simple as dividing the
distance by the combined train speeds and multiplying that answer in hours by the
speed of Bird in miles per hour, the hallelujahs are a joint triumph of a smiling,
uni-furcating teacher/student work group.
Or how about chances of the second card in the deck being the Ace of
Spades? Again, this is a perfectly legitimate example of problems coming in
words, words coming by themselves and not helping answer the question. There’s
a lot of Word Play here, but when the student falls for it, the student is Got or
otherwise humiliated. Probably for the young Einstein, such humiliation was a
useful stage in growing up. But for mediocre mathematicians who will eventually
major elsewhere, Quantitative Reasoning quite probably isn’t being inculcated by
the pain.
What alternative then?
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 14
Grawe: Paulos' Numeracy Humor
For some, Sympathetic Pain humor, lacking the zing and sting of stand-up
comedy, may be somewhat sugary. Adding a little vinegar here and there,
however, can be assumed, and sweet-and-sour may be a better flavor for helping
normally intelligent students across the curriculum to use math effectively.
Sympathetic Pain humor is real and just as powerful as Paulos’ humors. Used
well, it includes rather than excludes. It can also energize, enable, and enrich.
Acknowledgments
Deep gratitude is due to the reviewers and their comments on a first draft of this
paper. The paper is substantially longer and no doubt much better for their
constructive help. Special thanks go to Len Vacher, who suggested the wonderful
idea of turning back to John Allen Paulos’ work and achievement. And repeated
thanks go to Robin Jaeckle Grawe and to the thousands of responding associates
of the Institute for Travesty Comedy, and Humor Studies (ITCHS 1) whose
combined preferences in humor are the base for any confidence I may have in
suggesting alternative humor approaches and strategies. The unremitting
encouragement and support of the entire Winona State University faculty and its
students over at least five student generations is again acknowledged with greatest
appreciation.
References
Capp, A.1965. The comedy of Charlie Chaplain. In Corrigan 1965, 219‒229.
Corrigan, R. 1965. Comedy: Meaning and form. San Francisco: Chandler
Publishing Company.
Freud, S. 1960. Jokes and their relation to the unconscious. ed. J.Strachey. New
York: W. W. Norton.
Grawe, P. 1994. Humor Quotient Test: Validation and results. Paper presentation
to the Twelfth International Society for Humor Studies Conference. Ithaca,
New York. 2008. December comedy. Institute for Travesty, Comedy and
Humor Studies.
http://www.itchs.org/December%20Comedy/DC%20ch%206%20.htm
(accessed March 6, 2015).
———, and R. Grawe. 2008. Aladdin: Do you trust me? Comedic tenor, comic
vehicle: Humor in American film comedy. Institute for Travesty, Comedy,
and Humor Studies. http://www.itchs.org/CTCV/ctcv%20ch%209.htm
(accessed May 1, 2015).
1
www.itchs.org (last accessed 6/1/2015).
———. 2009. Laughter is the best medicine: The appropriate and therapeutic use
of humor in aging services settings. Paper presentation to Changing Lives:
Aging Services of Minnesota Institute. Minneapolis, MN.
———. 2014. A Cheshire Smile: Humor texture and personality in Shakespeare’s
comedies. Institute for Travesty, Comedy, and Humor Studies.
http://www.itchs.org/Cheshire%20Smile/CS%20F%20contents%20A.htm
(accessed May 1, 2015).
Grawe, R. 1994. Humor Quotient Test: Theoretical design. Paper presentation to
the Twelfth International Society for Humor Studies Conference. Ithaca, New
York. 2008. December comedy. Institute for Travesty, Comedy, and Humor
Studies.
http://www.itchs.org/December%20Comedy/DC%20ch%205%20.htm
(accessed March 6, 2015).
Humor Quotient Newsletter. Institute for Travesty, Comedy, and Humor Studies.
http://www.itchs.org/HQN%20contents.htm (accessed March 6, 2015).
Meredith, G. 1965. From An essay on comedy. In Corrigan 1965, 466‒470.
Paulos, J. 1980. Mathematics and humor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
———. 1988. Innumeracy: Mathematical illiteracy and its consequences. New
York: Hill and Wang.
———. 1991. Beyond numeracy: Ruminations of a numbers man. New York:
Alfred A. Knopf.
———. 1995. A mathematician reads the newspaper. New York: BasicBooks.
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol8/iss2/art11
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.8.2.11 16