tmp827F TMP
tmp827F TMP
XU, Yi*
University College London
This critical review is mainly concerned with methodological issues in prosody research, with the aim to highlight
progress toward developing predictive knowledge about prosody. The review shows that there has been a steady
progression in terms of methodological rigor as the field goes through major methodological trends that can be
described as analysis-by-introspection, analysis-by-transcription, analysis-by-hypothesis-testing and analysis-by-
modeling. All the major methodologies currently still co-exist and each still has it own merit. But all of them are
evaluated in terms of their effectiveness in establishing knowledge that is generalizable. Finally, an emphasis will be
made on the need to have much more linking and integration between different subareas of prosody research.
1 Introduction
Speech conveys information not only through segmental sounds like vowels and consonants, but
also through prosody, i.e., variations in fundamental frequency (also referred to as F0 and pitch),
duration, intensity and voice quality. Unlike in the case of segments, one of the greatest
difficulties about prosody is what I would call the lack of reference problem. By reference I
mean a pivot that serves as both a starting point of inquest and a point that one can comfortably
fall back on. In segmental research, for example, word identity serves as such a reference,
because it is consciously accessible whether the language under study has a writing system or
even whether the human informant is literate. Thus we can confidently investigate the phonetic
properties that distinguish one word from another. But words also give us a false sense of
certainty, because their ease of access may lead to the assumption that what underlies the lexical
contrast, namely, the phonemes, are also easily accessible to individual speakers. This has been
shown not to be the case by the discovery of phonological awareness (Mattingly, 1972;
Liberman et al. 1990). That is, awareness of the segments needs to be either introduced or
enhanced through literacy education (Bentin et al., 1992; Mann & Brady, 1988). So, the
existence of orthographic representations of a functionality does not guarantee its conscious
awareness and hence easy observation. In the case of prosody, very little of its functionality is
orthographically represented, except for the punctuations whose meanings are at best
ambiguous. Thus the starting point of inquest of prosody is inevitably vague and arbitrary, and it
is just as difficult to know for certain what to check against after an observation is made. The
difficulty is further compounded by the fact that our pitch awareness is not nearly as high as is
often assumed, especially when it comes to melodic events in prosody (Dankovicova et al.,
2007). As a result, the identification of the prosodic functionalities and their phonetic
realizations often has to be attempted at the same time, with limited help from our built-in
introspective ability. This is probably why approaches to prosody have been so disparate. There
is therefore an urgent need to examine prosody research from the perspective of methodology.
This paper offers a brief review of the strategies that have been attempted over the last century
or so, with the hope that this may help future research to achieve greater efficiency in finding
real solutions.
There have been a number of reviews of prosody research, the most recent only a year
ago (Wagner & Watson, 2010). The emphases of those reviews are different from what is
covered here. Shattuck-Hufnagel and Turk (1996) and Wagner and Watson (2010) were both
concerned mainly with prosodic structures as reflected by prominence and boundary strength
and their phonological representations. Cutler et al. (1997) focused on whether and how prosody
may help the perceptual processing of words, syntactic structures and discourse structures.
Botinis et al. (2001) was quite comprehensive and covered some of the issues discussed in the
current review. But it was meant to be a tutorial and hence was in general fairly noncritical.
Also much has happened in the past 10 years in terms of new empirical data and theoretical
development. The current review will focus on the progress over the years toward solving the
lack of reference problem by trying to build up predictive rather than descriptive knowledge
about prosody.
2 Analysis by transcription
Systematic examination of prosody can go back as early as Walker (1787), who proposed a tone
marking system for English intonation which is not very different from the IPA annotations for
lexical tones. The modern British intonation tradition is a continuation of this approach, with
representative work by Palmer (1922), Kingdon (1958), OConnor and Arnold (1961), Halliday
(1967), Crystal (1969), Cruttenden (1997) and Wells (2006). In this tradition, intonation is
portrayed by a transcription system consisting of representations for prominences (usually by
the size of successive dots corresponding to the stressed syllables) and contours (by curved
lines, sometimes with arrow heads to indicate the direction of pitch movements). Parallel to this
tradition, there have been transcription systems in America that put greater emphasis on tonal
levels rather than tonal contours. This tradition can go back to Rush (1827), and the subsequent
work includes Pike (1945), Trager and Smith (1951), Hockett (1958) and Pierrehumbert (1980).
One exception from this tradition is Bolinger (1986, 1989), who used a transcription system that
represents pitch contours by word-art like text arrangements. Despite the differences in the
proclaimed general goals (one mainly for teaching and the other for analysis), the two traditions
share one thing in common, namely, they both try to understand intonation through analysis-by-
transcription. This is despite the improvement over the years on the tools of transcript from
primarily auditory detection to instrumental observation. The latest major development of this
approach, which is still widely practiced, is the transcription system known as ToBI for TOnes
and Break Indices (Silverman et al., 1992). The system is developed based on the pitch accent
representations proposed by Pierrehumbert (1980) and the boundary representations proposed
by Price et al. (1991).
The methodological motivation behind the analysis-by-transcription approach is probably
well characterized by Shattuck-Hufnagel & Turk (1996:193): Because current theories do not
predict the precise prosodic shape that a particular utterance will take, it is important to
determine the prosodic choices that a speaker has made for utterances that are used in an
auditory sentence processing study. In other words, given the fact that our knowledge about
prosody has not yet developed to the level of being able to make precise predictions, there still
needs to be means of referring to whatever prosodic forms we observe. It is interesting to note,
however, the discrepancy between this characterization and the assumption widely held within
this approach that the transcribed categories such as pitch accents, boundary tones and phrase
tones are phonological rather than phonetic, i.e., the transcriptions represent phoneme-like
rather than allophone-like units. As explained in Pierrehumbert (1980:59), the establishment of
these categories was not based on whether or not they could clearly distinguish meanings, but
rather was the outcome of attempting to deduce a system of phonological representation for
intonation from observed features of F0 contours. In an effort to link the form-based intonation
categories proposed in Pierrehumbert (1980), Pierrehumbert & Hirschberg (1990) proposed a
compositional theory of tune interpretation, according to which the pitch accents H* and L* are
meaningful themselves, i.e., directly representing meanings like newness, salience, mutual
belief, incredulity, etc. Interestingly, this again differs from the classic assumption that
segmental phonemes like consonants and vowels are not directly meaningful, but serve only as
building blocks of morphemes that are the smallest units of meaning.
In general, therefore, as explained by Shattuck-Hufnagel & Turk (1996), analysis-by-
transcription is not meant to develop predictive knowledge per se, but only as an initial step in
that direction. But because the transcription systems themselves are not developed from
empirical data, the lack of reference problem mentioned above is not fully addressed. In more
recent research, analysis-by-transcription is often incorporated into empirical studies, in which
the transcriptions are used as measurements and subjected to statistical analysis (e.g., Caspers,
2003; Grice et al., 2009; Mady & Kleber, 2010; Metusalem & Ito, 2008; Yoon, 2010). This
seems to be a welcome step, but it may also be beneficial to combine the transcription analysis
with other measurements.
3 Analysis by introspection
Analysis-by-introspection refers to assignment of prosodic categories to text based mainly on
intuition, without experimental investigation or transcription of recorded utterances. This kind
of approach is adopted mainly in theoretical studies of syntax-prosody or pragmatics-prosody
interfaces, for example, Chaffe (1974, 1976), Brazil et al. (1980) and Bring (2006, 2007). An
apparent issue with this kind of approach is that, as mentioned in the introduction, human
introspection is not known to be highly reliable. Thus the assigned prosodic categories could
well have been affected by the imprecision of the introspection. Note that from the perspective
of hypothesis testing to be discussed next, there is nothing wrong with using introspection as a
means of hypothesis formation. What is crucial is not to end there, and treat the introspectively
derived categories as established ones.
function actually has a unique encoding scheme in a particular language. Table 1 (p. 110-111)
shows a list of plausible functions and their acoustic correlates as reported by various studies.
As can be seen in Table 1 (p. 110-111), focus and boundary marking have both attracted a
large amount of research effort, and clear acoustic cues have been identified. For focus,
increasingly clear evidence shows that its cue is not only in the focused items themselves, but
also in the post-focal components, which typically show reduced pitch range and intensity. But
such post-focus compression (PFC) is found only in some of the languages, while many other
languages show no evidence of PFC. The cause of such uneven distribution is currently under
active investigation (cf. Xu, 2011 for a brief overview). Also related to focus are prominence
and newness, two potential functions that have been widely recognized. Prominence has
sometimes been viewed as synonymous to focus, but often it is assumed to be free of any
specific linguistic functions. Such freedom, however, leaves room for confounding between
better defined functions such as lexical stress and focus. The problem resulting from such
confounding is highlighted by the recent controversial finding by Kochanski et al. (2005) that F0
contributes little to prominence. In the case of newness/givenness, conceptually the contrast can
be easily defined (Bock & Mazzella, 1983). However, when examined with other functions
systematically controlled, it does not seem to show distinct prosodic correlates independent of
focus and topic, other than a small durational effect, at least in Mandarin (Wang & Xu, in
press). In the studies on other languages, newness is not genuinely separated from focus and
therefore it is yet unknown whether it has unique cues of its own. Another focus-related issue is
the different types of focus that have been widely assumed (Gussenhoven, 2007). Of the four
studies shown in Table 1 (p. 110-111), that have looked into different types of focus, only one
has reported difference between narrow and contrastive focus (Avesani & Vayra, 2003, for
Florence Italian). More research is therefore needed to further investigate whether there is
distinct prosodic coding for different types of focus.
There have been remarkably many studies looking into issues related to boundary
marking, and the list shown in Table 1 (p. 110-111) is far from complete. Overall, the most
consistent cue is in terms of duration, whether it is that of domain-final vowel, syllable or word,
pause or domain-initial consonant. The emerging picture is that such duration-based boundary
marking is applied across the board, from within a word to within a phrase to between phrases
to between sentences, as can be seen in Table 2. Also shown in Table 2 is an increase in the
number of cues as the units separated by the boundary become larger. Interestingly, as can be
seen in the bottom two rows, when the unit is the sentence or larger, boundary cues seem to be
joined by topic cues. This may suggest that the topic function could be a special case of the
boundary function. This possibility can be explored in future research.
Table 2: Prosodic boundary cues reported in a number of studies. An increase in the number of
cues can be seen as the boundary level becomes higher.
Boundary type Prosodic cues
Within phrase Duration (Gussenhoven & Rietveld 1992, Nakatani et al.
1981, White & Turk 2010, Xu & Wang 2009)
Between phrases Duration, F0 (Edwards et al. 1991, Gussenhoven & Rietveld
1992, Klatt 1975, Nakatani et al. 1981, Wagner 2005)
Between sentences Duration, pause, F0 (Berkovits 1993, 1994, Ladd 1988, Swerts
1997, Wang & Xu in press)
Between paragraphs (topic?) Duration, pause, F0 (Nakajima & Allen 1993, Lehiste 1975,
Swerts 1997)
Within the topic function, there is a subtype known as contrastive topic, which is widely
recognized among the syntacticians (Bring, 2003; Kiss, 2008; Krifka, 2008). It is assumed that
contrastive topic is associated with a rising pitch pattern known as B-accent (Bolinger, 1965).
However, judging from the pitch track provided in Bring (2003) which is cited from
Jackendoff (1972), there is no clear evidence of rising F0. Also in the course of conducting this
review, no systematic empirical data were found on contrastive topic. According to Bring
(2007), the contrastive component of contrastive topic is similar to focus. At least for Mandarin,
Wang & Xu (in press) found that contrastiveness in topic has no prosodic correlates
independent of focus when both topic and focus are controlled. This issue nevertheless needs
more investigation for other languages.
The prosody-syntax row in Table 1 (p. 110-111) shows studies on how prosody and
syntax influence each other and those on how prosody may aid comprehension in speech
perception. The prosodic cues are rather diverse and often not clearly stated. The relation
between prosody and syntax is nevertheless a very important issue, and there is a question that
has rarely been asked before: Why should the two be matched or linked in the first place? That
is, assuming that speech is about exchanging information, if a meaning is already syntactically
or prosodically conveyed, why should it be also encoded in the other domain? Unless, of course,
redundancy of coding is favored (Assmann & Summerfield, 2004). This possibility suggests
that exploring cross-domain relation in terms of redundant coding is a direction worth pursuing
in future research.
The last two rows of Table 1 (p. 110-111) show that there have been many studies
looking at emotions and attitudes. A multitude of cues have been reported for various emotions
and attitudes and they are too diverse to be discussed in detail in this review. Interested readers
may find a number of detailed literature reviews on vocal expression of emotions and attitudes,
including Mauss and Robinson (2009), Murray and Arnott (1993), Scherer (2003) and Scherer
and Bnziger (2004). In addition, a new line of research based on evolutionary mechanisms and
cross-species comparisons has shown some interesting results (Chuenwattanapranithi et al.
1996; Morton, 1977; Ohala 1984, 1996; Xu & Kelly, 2010; Xu et al., forthcoming).
al., 1996; Xu 1999). These contextual variations have many different sources, as summarized in
Xu (2006), and each need to be handled in terms of its own specific underlying mechanism.
Likewise, for any pattern found in production, there is also the question of perceptual
relevance and sensitivity. A case in point is the many acoustic patterns found to be associated
with focus in production studies. There has been some research on the perceptual relevance of
focus-related pitch patterns (Botinis, 2000; Prom-on et al. 2009; Rump & Collier, 1996; Xu et
al., 2004). Prom-on et al. (2009) also found that incorporating focus-appropriate duration
changes further improves focus identification rate based on F0 only changes. However, there has
been virtually no research examining the perceptual relevance of intensity (Kochanski et al.
2005) and spectral balance (Sluijter & van Heuven, 1996).
Another pattern found in recent research is a rather consistent temporal alignment of F 0
turning points relative to segmental events, especially to syllable edges (Arvaniti & Garding,
2007; Atterer & Ladd, 2004; Dilley et al., 2005; DImperio et al., 2007; Gili Fivela, 2002;
Prieto, 2009; Prieto & Torreira, 2007; Shue et al., 2010; Xu, 1998, 1999, 2001; Xu & Xu,
2005). The alignment has been found to be also sensitive to accent type, vowel length, syllable
structure and focus position, as well as being language- or even dialect-dependent (Arvaniti &
Garding, 2007; Atterer & Ladd, 2004; DImperio et al., 2007; Leyden & Heuven, 2006). The
perceptual significance of such alignment is only beginning to be investigated (Dilley, 2005;
Niebuhr, 2007; Tokuma & Xu, 2009, 2011). Niebuhr (2007) found that listeners are sensitive
not only to turning-point alignment, but also to changes in the shape of F0 trajectories due to the
alignment change. This means that information is not necessarily encoded in terms of F 0 peak
timing. Consistent with this possibility is the view that the actual encoding units are the
underlying pitch targets that are always synchronized with the syllable, and the alignment
pattern is the result of interaction between the underlying targets and other factors such as
surrounding targets, stress, position of the syllable, focus and duration, etc. (Xu, 2005; Xu &
Wang, 2001). Evidence for target-syllable synchronization and target-context interaction can be
seen in Dilley (2005), Gao & Xu (2010), Lu and Xu (2006, 2007), Niebuhr (2007) and Xu
(1998, 2001). Nevertheless, more research is needed to further resolve this issue by examining
the relation between F0 alignment, articulatory mechanisms and perceptual sensitivity.
1
Predictive knowledge can be also viewed as the kind that allows us to launch a rocket into the
air, knowing that it will reach the moon rather than falling into the ocean, or to give a medicine to a
patient, knowing that it will cure rather than kill him. In speech science, predictive knowledge would be
However, also because of the need for systematic controls, individual studies cannot examine all
factors at once, and so each is necessarily limited in scope. For example, in a typical lab
experiment, subjects are not asked to produce sentences with strong emotions, unless the study
is about emotional expressions. Are the findings, then, still valid in cases where emotion is
involved? This kind of question should also be empirically answered. There is some initial
evidence that functions like focus can be encoded in parallel with emotions (Xu et al.,
forthcoming). Bruce and Touati (1992) have demonstrated that prosodic patterns found in read
speech in Swedish are also found in spontaneous speech, in which emotions are certainly
involved (political debate, radio listener call-in conversation). More such research is needed to
further test the applicability of experimental findings to spontaneous speech.
Likewise, studies that intend to directly embrace the richness of natural, spontaneous
prosody should also go beyond only developing descriptive knowledge, and aim also at
establishing predictive knowledge that can be applied in the explanation, recognition, synthesis
and other applications of natural prosody.
the kind that will allow us to build a robot that sounds like a real human, saying all the right things with
all the right tone of voice, or a robot that will understand every word we are saying, and every subtlety in
our tone of voice, or at least to the extent we are able to do ourselves, or to apply a teaching method to a
language learner, knowing that it will work better than chance, and better than any potential "placebo"
effect. There is no doubt that we are still a long way away from such predictive knowledge, and most of
the research is not directly concerned with practical issues. But the lessons from other scientific
disciplines are that predictive knowledge developed in basic research is the key to real advances in
technology.
5 Analysis by Modeling
Potentially the most rigorous test of our understanding of prosody, especially in terms of
predictive knowledge, is computational modeling, which, though also one type of experimental
approach, is discussed separately because its importance has in general not been duly
recognized. In modeling, attention to detail can be pushed to the limit, because our knowledge is
checked against all the minutiae of reality. To computationally model prosody, quantitative
algorithms need to be developed to generate continuous prosodic events whose every detail can
be compared to that of real speech. The development of each algorithm is based on a particular
understanding of or assumption about an underlying mechanism of prosody. Of course, the rigor
of computational modeling as a research tool is not automatically guaranteed, and it is in fact
more often not fully realized. This is because its effectiveness depends on a number of critical
aspects of the modeling process, including, in particular, (a) description vs. prediction, (b)
method of evaluation, and (c) degree of freedom and level of control.
utterance. This has been done in a number of studies for tone and intonation (Andruski &
Costello, 2004; Chen & Chang, 1992; de Ruiter, 2008; Gandour et al., 1999; Grab et al., 2007;
Hirst et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2006). With polynomial fitting, each section of a complex contour
can be represented by a set of coefficients, which may drastically reduce the amount of
representations from the original point-by-point data. A critical question about polynomial
representations, however, is whether they are linguistically meaningful and whether they can be
used in predictive modeling, i.e., serving as categorical parameters that can be generalized to
other instances of the same category. In the aforementioned studies, the polynomial coefficients
are used only in statistic comparisons or classifications, but not in predictive synthesis. So, the
predictiveness of models based on polynomial fitting is still unknown. Similarly, a number of
other studies that use non-polynomial models also only fitted the F0 contours of specific
utterances without predicting new F0 contours (Anderson et al., 1984; Bellegarda et al., 2001;
Fujisaki, 1983, 1988, 1992; Fujisaki et al., 1994, 2003; Mixdorff & Fujisaki, 1997, 2000;
Pierrehumbert, 1981; Taylor, 2000a; van Santen et al., 2005; Veronis et al., 1998). When the
modeling experiment is non-predictive, it is hard to know how much of the abstract
characteristics of each prosodic category have been captured.
Whether for the sake of building a practical synthesis system or increasing knowledge
about prosody, therefore, it is imperative to develop models that are predictive. Predictiveness,
however, can be achieved at different levels. The highest degree of predictiveness would be
found in a system with human-like performance, i.e., starting from idea formation and finishing
with production of fully natural and informative prosody. It will probably be a long time before
anything close to that is developed, of course. The next best would be something akin to a
concept-to-speech system (McKeown & Pan 2000; Taylor, 2000b; Young & Fallside, 1979),
which, though also very tantalizing, seems to be still far from materialization. Part of the
difficulty of concept-to-speech is the translation of concepts into proper functional prosodic
categories. But this is also the problem facing systems designed to achieve predictability at an
even lower level. That is, given a set of utterances that are functionally marked, regardless of
whether the category labels are derived from text or concepts, or determined by human labelers,
can the system then generate prosodic forms that fit closely to those of the original? From the
perspective of analysis by modeling, performing such prediction is actually a process of testing
both the validity of the predictors and the assumed encoding mechanisms.
Table 4 (p. 113-114) shows a summary of the predictors (also known as input features)
tested in various modeling studies. Note that some of the studies in the table have used a large
number of predictors, e.g., Bellegarda et al. (2001), Mixdorff and Jokisch (2001), Mohler and
Conkie (1998) and Sun (2002). In these cases, for the sake of developing predictive knowledge,
it would be desirable to not only assess the overall quality of data fitting, but also examine the
contribution of each predictor. Note also that the performance of a particular predictor does not
necessarily reflect its importance, because the performance should also be closely related to its
modeling implementation based on the assumed encoding strategy. As a tool for hypothesis
testing, prosodic modeling therefore should be a continuous process of not only trying to
identify all the relevant predictors, but also searching for the proper encoding mechanisms,
which are not necessarily shared across the predictors (cf. Xu, 2005 for some hypotheses).
objective evaluations is to measure goodness of fit between synthetic and original prosodic
forms. There have been three major methods: Root Mean Square Error (RMSE) (Fujisaki et al.,
2005; Kochanski et al., 2003; Ni et al. 2006; Prom-on et al., 2009; Raidt et al., 2004; Sakurai et
al., 2003; Sun, 2006) and Pearsons correlation or correlation for short (Mixdorff & Jokisch
2001; Prom-on et al., 2009; Raidt et al., 2004; Sun, 2006), and mean absolute frequency
deviation across each utterance (Bellegarda et al., 2001). Among the three, RMSE and
correlation are the most used. Hermes (1998) has shown that both measures are effective. For
RMSE, the unit of measurement varies from Hz (Mohler & Conkie, 1998; Morlect et al., 2001;
Sun, 2002) to semitone (Prom-on et al., 2009; Raidt et al., 2004) to ln(F0) (Fujisaki et al., 2005;
Gu et al., 2006; Ni et al., 2006; Sakurai et al., 2003), which makes cross-study comparison
difficult. Although ln(F0) and semitones can be mutually converted from one to the other
because both are logarithmic (semitoneRMSE = 12 ln(F0)RMSE / ln(2) ), RMSE values in Hz cannot
be properly converted to a logarithmic scale, because of lack of reference, see equation 1. The
advantage of logarithmic scale for intonation has been shown in terms of both production
(Fujisaki, 2003; Nolan, 2003; Xu & Sun, 2002) and perception (Traunmller & Eriksson, 1994).
Given such logarithmic nature, RMSE values in Hz need to be interpreted differently depending
on the average pitch of the speaker. For example, an RMSE value of 10 Hz ( 1.39 st) for a
male speaker with a mean F0 of F0reference = 120 Hz is roughly equivalent to that of 18 Hz ( 1.36
st) for a female voice with a mean F0 of F0reference = 220 Hz.
semitone = 12 log2(F0 / F0reference) (1)
In addition to the objective evaluations, the effectiveness of a model can also be
perceptually evaluated. The perceptual evaluations fall into two major types. In the first type,
the naturalness of the synthetic prosody is evaluated. Various methods have been used,
including direct judgment of naturalness (Sun, 2002), judgment of whether the prosody is
synthetic or natural (Prom-on et al., 2009), and similarity rating (Ni et al., 2006). Some studies
(e.g., Mohler & Conkie, 1998) have made use of mean opinion score (MOS) a measurement
designed for evaluating audio media quality, which employs a 5-level scale:
The MOS is then calculated as the arithmetic mean of all the individual scores. This method
seems to have the potential of standardizing formal naturalness evaluations. But its advantage
has yet to be fully demonstrated.
In the second type of evaluation, what is assessed is the perceptual accuracy of the
hypothetical categories being modeled, such as lexical tone, lexical stress, pitch accent, focus,
boundary strength, sentence modality, type of emotion and attitude, etc. Surprisingly, only two
of the studies reviewed here performed this kind of evaluations (Morlec et al., 2001; Prom-on et
al., 2009). All the other studies that performed perceptual evaluations only assessed perceptual
naturalness of synthetic prosody. For the sake of improving the predictiveness of the models and
using them as effective hypothesis testing tools, it is highly desirable in future research to
include perceptual identification of the input categories as one of the evaluation methods.
Table 5 (p. 115) shows a comparison of degrees of freedom in a number of models for
controlling local F0 contours. What is shown here, however, is only degrees of freedom for
generating local pitch events. Note that even at this level, degrees of freedom should not be used
as the sole criterion for judging a model, and it is not the case that smaller degrees of freedom is
always better. What is more crucial is whether each degree of freedom has sufficient
justifications. Ideally the degrees of freedom should reflect the assumed human control, at least
at the level that the model tries to simulate. For example, if transitions between basic events are
specified by one or more free parameters, it would be helpful to explicate the articulatory nature
of such controlled transitions. Furthermore, degrees of freedom also has to do with the level of
control the model tries to simulate, e.g., at the level of the muscle (Fujisaki et al., 2005), the
underlying target and its articulatory approximation (Kochanski & Shih, 2003; Prom-on et al.,
2009), the decomposed holistic contours (Baily et al., 2005) or surface contours (Pierrehumber,
1981; Taylor 2000; van Santen et al. 2005).
For more global events related to higher functions as shown in Table 1 (p. 110-111), more
degrees of freedom is certainly needed. For each contrastive prosodic component above the
basic units, at least a single degree of freedom is required to simulate its coding. Not
implementing this degree of freedom would result in a decrease in the functionality of the
model.
Acknowledgement
I would like to thank Plinio Barbosa, Paul Gettel and Santitham Prom-on for their comments on
an earlier version of the paper. All errors are mine, of course.
REFERENCES
Anderson M, Pierrehumbert J and Liberman M. Synthesis by rule of English intonation patterns In:
Proceedings of Proceedings of ICASSP, San Diego, CA, 1984. 77-80.
Andruski J and Costello J. Using polynomial equations to model pitch contour shape in lexical tones: An
example from Green Mong. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 2004;34:125-140.
Anttila A, Adams M and Speriosu M. The role of prosody in the English dative alternation. Language and
Cognitive Processes 2010;25:946-981.
Arvaniti A and Garding G. Dialectal variation in the rising accents of American English. In: J Cole and J
Hualde, editors. Laboratory Phonology 9, The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 2007. 547-576.
Atterer M and Ladd DR. On the phonetics and phonology of "segmental anchoring" of F0: Evidence from
German. Journal of Phonetics 2004;32:177-197.
Auberge V and Cathiard M. Can we hear the prosody of smile. Speech Communication 2003;40:87-97.
Avesani C and Vayra M. Broad, narrow and contrastive focus in Florentine Italian In: Proceedings of The
15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, 2003. 1803-1806.
Bagshaw PC. An investigation of acoustic events related to sentential stress and pitch accents, in English.
Speech Communication 1993;13:333-342.
Bailly G and Holm B. SFC: a trainable prosodic model. Speech Communication 2005;46:348-364.
Bnziger T and Scherer KR. The role of intonation in emotional expressions. Speech Communication
2005;46:252-267.
Barbosa PA. From syntax to acoustic duration: A dynamical model of speech rhythm production. Speech
Communication 2007;49:725-742.
Barbosa PA. Detecting changes in speech expressiveness in participants of a radio program In:
Proceedings of Interspeech 2009, Brighton, UK, 2009. 2155-2158.
Barbosa PA. and Bailly G. Characterisation of rhythmic patterns for text-to-speech synthesis. Speech
Communication 1994;15 (1-2):127-137.
Baum KM and Nowicki S. Perception of emotion: Measuring decoding accuracy of adult prosodic cues
varying in intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 1998;22:89-107.
Baumann S, Becker J, Grice M and Mcke D. Tonal and articulatory marking of focus in German In:
Proceedings of The 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrucken, 2007. 1029-1032.
Beckman ME and Edwards J. Lengthenings and shortenings and the nature of prosodic constituency. In: J
Kingston and ME Beckman, editors. Papers in Laboratory Phonology 1 Between the Grammar and
Physics of Speech, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990. 152-178.
Beckman ME. A typology of spontaneous speech. In: Y Sagisaka, N Campbell and N Higuchi, editors.
Computing Prosody: Computational Models for Processing Spontaneous Speech, New York: Springer
Verlag, 1997. 726.
Bellegarda J, Silverman K, Lenzo K and Anderson V. Statistical prosodic modeling: from corpus design
to parameter estimation. IEEE Trans actions on Speech Audio Process 2001;9:5266.
Beller G, Obin N and rodet X. Articulation Degree as a Prosodic Dimension of Expressive Speech In:
Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008, 2008.
Bentin S, Ram F and Leonard K. Chapter 11 Phonological Awareness, Reading, and Reading
Acquisition: A Survey and Appraisal of Current Knowledge. In: editors. Advances in Psychology.
Volume 94: North-Holland, 1992. 193-210.
Berkovits R. Utterance-final lengthening and the duration of final-stop closures. Journal of Phonetics
1993;21:479-489.
Berkovits R. Durational Effects in Final Lengthening, Gapping, and Contrastive Stress. language and
speech 1994;37:237-250.
Beyssade C, Hemforth B, Marandin J-M and Portes C. Prosodic markings of information focus in French
In: Proceedings of Interface Discours & Prosodie 2009, Paris, 2009. 109-122.
Birch S and Clifton C. Focus, accent, and argument structure: effects on language comprehension.
Language and Speech 1995;38:365-391.
Bock J and Mazzella J. Intonational marking of given and new information: Some consequences for
comprehension. Memory & Cognition 1983;11:64-76.
Bolinger DL. Forms of English: Accent, Morpheme, Order, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard
University Press; 1965.
Bolinger D. Intonation and its parts: melody in spoken English, Palo Alto: Stanford University Press;
1986.
Bolinger D. Intonation and Its Uses -- Melody in Grammar and Discourse, Stanford, California: Stanford
University Press; 1989.
Botinis A, Bannert R and Tatham M. Contrastive tonal analysis of focus perception in Greek and
Swedish. In: A Botinis, editors. Intonation : analysis, modelling and technology, Boston: Kluwer
Academic Publishers, 2000. 97-116.
Botinis A, Fourakis M and Gawronska B. Focus identification in English, Greek and Swedish In:
Proceedings of The 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, San Francisco, 1999. 1557-1560.
Botinis A, Granstrm B and Mbius B. Developments and paradigms in intonation research. Speech
Communication 2001;33:263-296.
Braun B and Tagliapietra L. The role of contrastive intonation contours in the retrieval of contextual
alternatives. Language and Cognitive Processes 2010;25:1024-1043.
Brazil DM, Coulthard M and Johns C. Discourse Intonation and Language Teaching, London: Longman;
1980.
Breitenstein C, Van Lancker D and Daum I. The contribution of speech rate and pitch variation to the
perception of vocal emotions in a German and an American sample. Cognition & Emotion 2001;15:57-
79.
Bulut M and Narayanan S. On the robustness of overall F0-only modifications to the perception of
emotions in speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 2008;123:4547-4558.
Bruce G and Touati P. On the analysis of prosody in spontaneous speech with exemplification from
Swedish and French. Speech Communication 1992;11:453-458.
Bruce G. Developing the Swedish intonation model. Lund University, Dept. of Linguistics Working
Papers 1982a;22:51-116.
Bruce G. Textual aspects of prosody in Swedish. Phonetica 1982b;39:274-287.
Bring D. On D-Trees, Beans, and B-Accents. Linguistics and Philosophy 2003;26:511-545.
Bring D. Focus Projection and Default Prominence. In: V Molnar and S Winkler, editors. The
Architecture of Focus, 2006.
Bring D. Semantics, Intonation and Information Structure. In: G Ramchad and C Reiss, editors. The
Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces: Oxford University Press, 2007.
Byrd D and Saltzman E. Intragestural dynamics of multiple prosodic boundaries. Journal of Phonetics
1998;26:173-199.
Byrd D and Saltzman E. The elastic phrase: Modeling the dynamics of boundary-adjacent lengthening.
Journal of Phonetics 2003;31:149180.
Calhoun S. How does informativeness affect prosodic prominence? Language and Cognitive
Processes25:1099-1140.
Campbell N. Automatic detection of prosodic boundaries in speech. Speech Communication
1993;13:343-354.
Campbell WN and Marumoto T. Automatic labelling of voice quality in speech databases for synthesis
In: Proceedings of ICSLP-2000, Beijing, 2000. 468-471.
Campbell N. Building a Corpus of Natural Speech - and Tools for the Processing of Expressive Speech -
the JST CREST ESP Project In: Proceedings of Eurospeech 2001, 2001. 1525-1528.
Campbell N. Databases of expressive speech. Journal of Chinese Language and Computing 2004;14:295-
304.
Carlson, K., Clifton, C. and Frazier, L. (2001). Prosodic Boundaries in Adjunct Attachment. Journal of
Memory and Language 45(1): 58-81.
Caspers J. Local speech melody as a limiting factor in the turn-taking system in Dutch. Journal of
Phonetics 2003;31:251276.
Chaffe W. Language and consciousness. Language 1974;50:111-133.
Chaffe W. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics and point of view. In: C Li, editors.
Subject and Topic, New York: Academic Press, 1976. 25-55.
Chahal D. Phonetic Cues to Prominence in Lebanese Arabic In: Proceedings of The 15th International
Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, 2003. 2067-2070.
Chen S-H and Chang S. A statistical model based fundamental frequency synthesizer for Mandarin
speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1992;92:114-120.
Chen A and Destruel E. Intonational encoding of focus in Toulousian French In: Proceedings of Speech
Prosody 2010, Chicago, 2010.
Chen A, Gussenhoven C and Rietveld T. Language-specificity in the perception of paralingistic
intonational meaning. Language and Speech 2004;47:311-349.
Chen S-w, Wang B and Xu Y. Closely related languages, different ways of realizing focus In:
Proceedings of Interspeech 2009, Brighton, UK, 2009. 1007-1010.
Chen Y. Durational Adjustment under Contrastive Focus in Standard Chinese. Journal of Phonetics
2006;34:176-201.
Cho T. Prosodically conditioned strengthening and vowel-to-vowel coarticulation in English. Journal of
Phonetics 2004;32:141176.
Cho T and McQueen JM. Prosodic influences on consonant production in Dutch: Effects of prosodic
boundaries, phrasal accent and lexical stress. Journal of Phonetics 2005;33:121-157.
Christophe A, Gout A, Peperkamp S and Morgan J. Discovering words in the continuous speech stream:
the role of prosody. Journal of Phonetics 2003;31:585598.
Chuenwattanapranithi S, Xu Y, Thipakorn B and Maneewongvatana S. Encoding emotions in speech with
the size code A perceptual investigation. Phonetica 2008;65:210-230.
Cieri C and Liberman M. More Data and Tools for More Languages and Research Areas: A Progress
Report on LDC Activities. LREC 2006: Fifth International Conference on Language Resources and
Evaluation, 2006.
Cole J, Mo Y and Baek S. The role of syntactic structure in guiding prosody perception with ordinary
listeners and everyday speech. Language and Cognitive Processes 2010;25:1141-1177.
Cooper WE, Eady SJ and Mueller PR. Acoustical aspects of contrastive stress in question-answer
contexts. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1985;77:2142-2156.
Cosmides L. Invariances in the acoustic expression of emotion during speech. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 1983;9:864-881.
Cruttenden A. Intonation (second edition): Cambridge University Press; 1997.
Crystal D. Prosodic Systems and Intonation in English, London: Cambridge University Press; 1969.
Cutler A, Dahan D and van Donselaar W. Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature
review. Language and Speech 1997;40:141-201.
Dankovicova J, House J, Crooks A and Jones K. The Relationship between Musical Skills, Music
Training, and Intonation Analysis Skills. Language & Speech 2007;50:177-225.
de Jong KJ. The supraglottal articulation of prominence in English: Linguistic stress as localized
hyperarticulation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1995;97:491-504.
de Jong K. Stress, lexical focus, and segmental focus in English: patterns of variation in vowel duration.
Journal of Phonetics 2004;32:493-516.
de Jong K and Zawaydeh B. Comparing stress, lexical focus, and segmental focus: patterns of variation in
Arabic vowel duration. Journal of Phonetics 2002;30:53-75.
de Ruiter JP, Mitterer H and Enfield NJ. Projecting the end of a speaker's turn: a cognitive cornerstone of
conversation. Language 2006;82:515-535.
de Ruiter LE. How useful are polynomials for analyzing intonation? In: Proceedings of Interspeech 2008,
Brisbane, 2008. 785-789.
Delais-Roussarie E, Rialland A, Doetjes J and Marandin JM. The prosody of post-focus sequences in
French In: Proceedings of The 1st International Conference on Speech Prosody, Aix-en-Provence, France,
2002. 239-242.
Delattre P, Poenack E and Olsen C. Some characteristics of German intonation for the expression of
continuation and finality. Phonetica 1965;13:134-161.
Di Cristo A and Jankowski J. Prosodic organisation and phrasing after focus in French In: Proceedings of
The 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, San Francisco, 1999. 1565-1568.
Dilley LC, Ladd DR and Schepman A. Alignment of L and H in bitonal pitch accents: testing two
hypotheses. Journal of Phonetics 2005;33:115-119.
D'Imperio M. Focus and tonal structure in Neapolitan Italian. Speech Communication 2001;33:339-356.
DImperio M, Espesser R, Lvenbruck H, Menezes C, Nguyen N and Welby P. Are tones aligned with
articulatory events? Evidence from Italian and French. In: J Cole and J Hualde, editors. Papers in
Laboratory Phonology IX: Change in Phonology, The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, 2007. 577-608.
Dohen M and Lvenbruck H. Pre-focal rephrasing, focal enhancement and post-focal deaccentuation in
French In: Proceedings of The 8th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Jeju, Korea,
2004. 1313-1316.
Donati C and Nespor M. From focus to syntax. Lingua 2003;113:1119-1142.
Dorn A and N Chasaide A. Effects of focus on f0 and duration in Irish (Gaelic) declaratives. In:
Proceedings of Interspeech 2011, Florence, 2011.
Downing LJ. Focus and prominence in Chichewa, Chitumbuka and Durban Zulu. ZAS Papers in
Linguistics (ZASPiL) 2008;49:47-65.
Eady SJ and Cooper WE. Speech intonation and focus location in matched statements and questions.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1986;80:402-416.
Eady SJ, Cooper WE, Klouda GV, Mueller PR and Lotts DW. Acoustic characteristics of sentential
focus: Narrow vs. broad and single vs. dual focus environments. Language and Speech 1986;29:233-251.
Edlund J and Heldner M. Exploring Prosody in Interaction Control. Phonetica 2005;62:215-226.
Edwards JR, Beckman ME and Fletcher J. The articulatory kinematics of final lengthening. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 1991;89:369-382.
Face TL. Narrow Focus intonation in Castilian Spanish absolute interrogatives. Journal of Language and
Linguistics 2006;5:295-311.
Ferriera F. The creation of prosody during sentence production. Psychological Review 1993;100:233
253.
Fry C and Kgler F. Pitch accent scaling on given, new and focused constituents in German. Journal of
Phonetics 2008;36:680-703.
Fery C, Skopeteas S and Hornig R. Cross-linguistic comparison of prosody, syntax and information
structure in a production experiment on localising expressions. Transactions of the Philological Society
2010;108:329-351.
Fougeron C. Articulatory properties of initial segments in several prosodic constituents in French. Journal
of phonetics 2001;29:109-135.
Frick RW. Communicating emotion: The role of prosodic features. Psychological Bulletin 1985;97:412-
429.
Fujisaki H. Dynamic characteristics of voice fundamental frequency in speech and singing. In: PF
MacNeilage, editors. The Production of Speech, New York: Springer-Verlag, 1983. 39-55.
Fujisaki H. A note on the physiological and physical basis for the phrase and accent components in the
voice fundamental frequency contour. In: O Fujimura, editors. Vocal Physiology: Voice Production, New
York: Raven Press, Ltd., 1988. 347-355.
Fujisaki H. Modeling the process of fundamental frequency contour generation. In: Y Tohkura, E
Vatikiotis-Bateson and Y Sagisaka, editors. Speech Perception, Production and Linguistic Structure,
Amsterdam: IOS Press, 1992. 313-326.
Fujisaki H. Prosody, Information, and Modeling with Emphasis on Tonal Features of Speech In:
Proceedings of Workshop on Spoken Language Processing, 2003. 5-14.
Fujisaki H, Ohno S, Nakamura K, Fuirao M and Gurlekian J. Analysis of accent and intonation in
Spanish based on a quatitative model In: Proceedings of International Conference on Spoken Language
Processing, Yokohama, 1994. 355-358.
Fujisaki H, Ohno S, Osame M, Sakata M and Hirose K. Prosodic characteristics of a spoken dialogue for
information query In: Proceedings of International Conference on Spoken Language Processing,
Yokohama, 1994. 1103-1106.
Fujisaki H, Wang C, Ohno S and Gu W. Analysis and synthesis of fundamental frequency contours of
Standard Chinese using the commandresponse model. Speech communication 2005;47:59-70.
Gao H and Xu Y. Ambisyllabicity in English: How real is it? In: Proceedings of The 9th Phonetics
Conference of China (PCC2010), Tianjin, 2010.
Grding E. Sentence intonation in Swedish. Phonetica 1979;36:207-215.
Grding E. Speech act and tonal pattern in Standard Chinese. Phonetica 1987;44:13-29.
Gerard C and Dahan D. Durational variations in speech and didactic accent during reading. Speech
Communication 1995;16:293-311.
Gili Fivela B. Tonal alignment in two Pisa Italian peak accents In: Proceedings of The 1st International
Conference on Speech Prosody, Aix-en-Provence, France, 2002. 339-342.
Gobl C and Chasaide AN. The role of voice quality in communicating emotion, mood and attitude.
Speech Communication 2003;40:189-212.
Grabe E, Kochanski G and Coleman J. Connecting intonation labels to mathematical descriptions of
fundamental frequency. Language and Speech 2007;50:281-310.
Grice M, Baumann S and Jagdfeld N. Tonal association and derived nuclear accents--The case of
downstepping contours in German. Lingua 2009;119:881-905.
Grnnum N. A Danish phonetically annotated spontaneous speech corpus (DanPASS). Speech
Communication 2009;51:594-603.
Gu W, Hirose K and Fujisaki H. Modeling the effects of emphasis and question on fundamental
frequency contours of Cantonese utterances. Audio, Speech, and Language Processing, IEEE
Transactions on 2006;14:1155-1170.
Gu W and Lee T. Effects of tonal context and focus on Cantonese F0 In: Proceedings of The 16th
International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrucken, 2007. 1033-1036.
Gussenhoven C. Types of focus in English. In: C Lee, M Gordon and D Bring, editors. Topic and Focus:
Cross-linguistic Perspectives on Meaning and Intonation, New York: Springer, 2007. 83-100.
Gussenhoven C, Repp BH, Rietveld A, Rump HH and Terken J. The perceptual prominence of
fundamental frequency peaks. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1997;102:3009-3022.
Gussenhoven C and Rietveld ACM. Intonation contours, prosodic structure and preboundary lengthening.
Journal of Phonetics 1992;20:283-303.
Halliday MAK. Intonation and Grammar in British English, The Hague: Mouton; 1967.
Hansson P. Prosodic correlates of discourse markers in dialogue In: Proceedings of DIAPRO-1999, 1999.
99-104.
Hanssen J, Peters J and Gussenhoven C. Prosodic Effects of Focus in Dutch Declaratives In: Proceedings
of Speech Prosody 2008, Campinas, Brazil, 2008. 609-612.
Hartmann K. Focus and tone. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 2008;55:415-426.
Hartmann K and Zimmermann M. Focus Strategies in Chadic: The Case of Tangale Revisited. Studia
Linguistica 2007;61:95-129.
Heldner M and Strangert E. Temporal effects of focus in Swedish. Journal of Phonetics 2001;29:329-361.
Heldner M. On the reliability of overall intensity and spectral emphasis as acoustic correlates of focal
accents in Swedish. Journal of Phonetics 2003;31:3962.
Hellmuth S. Focus-related pitch range manipulation (and peak alignment effects) in Egyptian Arabic In:
Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2006, Dresden, Germany, 2006. PS4-12-164.
Hermes DJ. Measuring the Perceptual Similarity of Pitch Contours. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998;41:73-
82.
Hermes DJ and Rump HH. Perception of prominence in speech intonation induced by rising and falling
pitch movements. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1994;96:83-92.
Hirschberg J and Litman D. Now let's talk about now: Identifying cue phrases intonationally In:
Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 1987. 163-
171.
Hirst DJ, Di Cristo A and Espesser R. Levels of representation and levels of analysis for intonation. In: M
Horne, editors. Prosody: Theory and Experiment, Dordrecht: Kluwer, 2000. 51-87.
Hitzeman J, Black AW, Mellish C, Oberlander J, Poesio M and Taylor P. An Annotation Scheme for
Concept-to-Speech Synthesis In: Proceedings of European Workshop on Natural Language Generation,
Toulouse, France, 1999. 59-66.
Ho AT. Intonation variation in a Mandarin sentence for three expressions: interrogative, exclamatory and
declarative. Phonetica 1977;34:446-457.
Ho AT. Mandarin tones in relation to sentence intonation and grammatical structure. Journal of Chinese
Linguistics 1976;4:1-13.
Hockett CF. A course in modern linguistics, New York: MacMillan; 1958.
House D. Phrase-final rises as a prosodic feature in wh-questions in Swedish humanmachine dialogue.
Speech Communication 2005;46:268-283.
Ipek C. Phonetic Realization of Focus with no On-Focus Pitch Range Expansion in Turkish In:
Proceedings of The 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Hong Kong, 2011.
Ishihara S. Intonation and interface conditions. Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2003.
Jackendoff R. Semantics in Generative Grammar, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1972.
Jannedy S. Prosodic Focus in Vietnamese. In: S Ishihara, S Jannedy and A Schwarz, editors.
Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure (ISIS): Potsdam University Press, 2007. 209-230.
Jin S. An Acoustic Study of Sentence Stress in Mandarin Chinese [Ph.D. dissertation]. The Ohio State
University; 1996.
Kabagema-Bilan E, Lpez-Jimnez B, Truckenbrodt H, University Tb and Berlin ZAS. Multiple focus in
Mandarin Chinese. Lingua 2011;In Press, Corrected Proof
Kingdon R. The groundwork of English intonation, London: Longman; 1958.
Klatt DH. Vowel lengthening is syntactically determined in a connected discourse. Journal of Phonetics
1975;3:129-140.
Kjelgaard MM and Speer SR. Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary
syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language 1999;40:153-194.
Kochanski G, Grabe E, Coleman J and Rosner B. Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental frequency
lends little. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 2005;118:1038-1054.
Kochanski G and Shih C. Prosody modeling with soft templates. Speech Communication 2003;39:311-
352.
Kochanski G, Shih C and Jing H. Quantitative measurement of prosodic strength in Mandarin. Speech
Communication 2003;41:625-645.
Kohler KJ. Modelling prosody in spontaneous speech. In: Y Sagisaka, N Campbell and N Higuchi,
editors. Computing Prosody, New York: Springer, 1997. 187-210.
Kiss K. Topic and focus: Two structural positions associated with logical functions in the left periphery
of the Hungarian sentence. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 2008;55:287-296.
Krifka M. Basic notions of information structure. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 2008;55:243-276.
Krivokapic J. Prosodic planning: Effects of phrasal length and complexity on pause duration. Journal of
Phonetics 2007;35:162-179.
Kgler F and Skopeteas S. On the universality of prosodic reflexes of contrast: The case of Yucatec Maya
In: Proceedings of The 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrcken, Germany, 2007.
Ladd DR. Declination reset' and the hierarchical organization of utterances. Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 1988;84:530-544.
Ladd DR, Silverman KEA, Tolkmitt F, Bergmann G and Scherer KR. Evidence for the independent
function of intonation contour type, voice quality, and F0 range in signaling speaker affect. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 1985;78:435-444.
Ladd DR, Verhoeven J and Jacobs K. Influence of adjacent pitch accents on each other's perceived
prominence: two contradictory effects. Journal of Phonetics 1994;22:87-99.
Lecumberri MLG. Perceptibility of nuclear focus in english. Barcelona English language and literature
studies 1997:175-186.
Lee Y-c and Xu Y. Phonetic Realization of Contrastive Focus in Korean In: Proceedings of Speech
Prosody 2010, Chicago, 2010. 100033:1-4.
Lehiste I. The phonetic structure of paragraphs. In: A Cohen and SEG Nooteboom, editors. Structure and
process in speech perception, New York: Springer-Verlag, 1975. 195-206.
Lehiste I, Olive JP and Streeter LA. Role of duration in disambiguating syntactically ambiguous
sentences. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1976;60:1199-1202.
Leyden K and van Heuven VJ. On the Prosody of Orkney and Shetland Dialects. Phonetica 2006;63:149-
174.
Liberman IY, Shankweiler D and Liberman AM. The Alphabetic Principle and Learning to Read. In: D
Shankweiler and IY Liberman, editors. Phonology and Reading Disability: Solving the Reading Puzzle,
Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1990.
Lin M. On production and perception of boundary tone in Chinese intonation In: Proceedings of
International Symposium on Tonal Aspects of Languages: With Emphasis on Tone Languages, Beijing,
2004. 125-129.
Liu F, Surendran D and Xu Y. Classification of statement and question intonations in Mandarin In:
Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2006, Dresden, Germany, 2006. PS5-25_0232.
Liu F and Xu Y. Parallel encoding of focus and interrogative meaning in Mandarin intonation. Phonetica
2005;62:70-87.
Liu F and Xu Y. Question intonation as affected by word stress and focus in English In: Proceedings of
The 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrcken, 2007. 1189-1192.
Liu F and Xu Y. The Neutral Tone in Question Intonation in Mandarin In: Proceedings of Interspeech
2007, Antwerp, 2007. 630-633.
Mann VA and Brady S. Reading Disability: The Role of Language Deficiencies. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology 1988;56:811-816.
Makarova V. Perceptual correlates of sentence-type intonation in Russian and Japanese. Journal of
Phonetics 2001;29:137-154.
Mattingly I. Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness. In: J Kavanagh and I Mattingly,
editors. Language by ear and by eye: The relationships between speech and reading, Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press, 1972. 133147.
Mady K and Kleber F. Variation of pitch accent patterns in Hungarian In: Proceedings of Speech Prosody
2010, Chicago, 2010.
Mauss IB and Robinson MD. Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion 2009;23:209-237.
McKeown KR and Pan S. Prosody modelling in concept-to-speech generation: methodological issues.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and
Engineering Sciences 2000;358:1419-1431.
Metusalem R and Ito K. The role of L+H* pitch accent in discourse construction In: Proceedings of
Speech Prosody 2008, Campinas, Brazil, 2008.
McRoberts GW, Studdert-Kennedy M and Shankweiler DP. The role of fundamental frequency in
signalling linguistic stress and affect: Evidence for a dissociation. Perception & Psychophysics
1995;57:159-174.
Mixdorff H. A Novel Approach to the Fully Automatic Extraction of Fujisaki Model Parameters In:
Proceedings of ICASSP 2000, Istanbul, Turkey, 2000. 1281-1284.
Mixdorff H and Fujisaki H. Automated quantitative analysis of F0 contours of utterances from a German
ToBI-labeled speech database In: Proceedings of The '97 Eurospeech, Rhodes, Greece, 1997. 187-190.
Mixdorff H and Jokisch O. Building An Integrated Prosodic Model of German In: Proceedings of
Eurospeech 2001, 2001.
Mohler G and Conkie A. Parametric modeling of intonation using vector quantization In: Proceedings of
The Third ESCA/COCOSDA Workshop on Speech Synthesis, Blue Mountains, NSW, Australia, 1998.
Morlec Y, Bailly G and Auberg V. Generating prosodic attitudes in French: Data, model and evaluation.
Speech Communication 2001;33:357-371.
Morton EW. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal
sounds. American Naturalist 1977;111:855-869.
Most RB and Saltz E. Information structure in sentences: New information. Language & Speech
1979;22:89-95.
Mozziconacci SJL. Modeling emotion and attitude in speech by means of perceptually based parameter
values. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction 2001;11:297-326.
Murray IR and Arnott JL. Toward the simulation of emotion in synthetic speech: A review of the
literature on human vocal emotion. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1993;93:1097-1108.
Nakajima S and Allen JF. A study on prosody and discourse structure in cooperative dialogues. Phonetica
1993;50:197-210.
Nakatani LH, O'Connor KD and Aston CH. Prosodic aspects of American English speech rhythm.
Phonetica 1981;38:84-106.
Ni J, Kawai H and Hirose K. Constrained tone transformation technique for separation and combination
of Mandarin tone and intonation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 2006;119:1764-1782.
Niebuhr O. The Signalling of German Rising-Falling Intonation Categories The Interplay of
Synchronization, Shape, and Height. Phonetica 2007;64:174-193.
Nolan F. Intonational equivalence: an experimental evaluation of pitch scales In: Proceedings of The 15th
International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, 2003. 771-774.
Nooteboom SG and Kruyt JG. Accents, focus distribution, and the perceived distribution of given and
new information: An experiment. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1987;82:1512-1524.
O'Connor JD and Arnold GF. Intonation of Colloquial English, London: Longmans; 1961.
Ofuka E, McKeown JD, Waterman MG and Roach PJ. Prosodic cues for rated politeness in Japanese
speech. Speech Communication 2000;32:199-217.
Ogden R and Routarinne S. The Communicative Functions of Final Rises in Finnish Intonation.
Phonetica 2005;62:160-175.
Ohala JJ. An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica
1984;41:1-16.
Ohala JJ. Ethological theory and the expression of emotion in the voice In: Proceedings of ICSLP96,
1996. 1812-1815.
Oliveira M and Freitas T. Intonation as a cue to turn management in telephone and face-to-face
interactions In: Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008, Campias, 2008. 485-488.
O'Shaughnessy D and Allen J. Linguistic modality effects on fundamental frequency in speech. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America 1983;74:1155-1171.
Ouden Hd, Noordman L and Terken J. Prosodic realizations of global and local structure and rhetorical
relations in read aloud news reports. Speech Communication 2009;51:116-129.
Palmer HE. English Intonation, with Systematic Exercises, Cambridge: Heffer; 1922.
Pan H. Focus and Taiwanese unchecked tones. In: C Lee, M Gordon and D Bring, editors. Topic and
Focus: Cross-linguistic Perspectives on Meaning and Intonation: Springer, 2007. 195-213.
Patil U, Kentner G, Gollrad A, Kgler F, Fry C and Vasishth S. Focus, word order and intonation in
Hindi. Journal of South Asian Linguistics 2008;1:55-72.
Pell MD. Influence of emotion and focus on prosody in matched statements and questions. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 2001;109:1668-1680.
Pierrehumbert J and Hirschberg J. The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse.
In: PR Cohen, J Morgan and ME Pollack, editors. Intentions in Communication, Cambridge,
Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1990. 271-311.
Pierrehumbert J. Synthesizing intonation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1981;70:985-995.
Pierrehumbert J. The Phonology and Phonetics of English Intonation [Ph.D. dissertation]. MIT,
Cambridge, MA. [Published in 1987 by Indiana University Linguistics Club, Bloomington]; 1980.
Pike KL. The Intonation of American English, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press; 1945.
Popper K. The Logic of Scientific Discovery (translation of Logik der Forschung), London: Hutchinson;
1959.
Potisuk S, Gandour J and Harper MP. Contextual variations in trisyllabic sequences of Thai tones.
Phonetica 1996;53:200-220.
Price PI, Ostendorf M, Shattuck-Hufnagel S and Fong C. The use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1991;90:2956-2970.
Prieto P and Torreira F. The segmental anchoring hypothesis revisited: Syllable structure and speech rate
effects on peak timing in Spanish. Journal of Phonetics 2007;35:473-500.
Prieto P. Tonal alignment patterns in Catalan nuclear falls. Lingua 2009;119:865-880.
Prom-on S, Xu Y and Thipakorn B. Modeling tone and intonation in Mandarin and English as a process
of target approximation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 2009;125:405-424.
Protopapas A and Lieberman P. Fundamental frequency of phonation and perceived emotional stress.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1997;101:2267-2277.
Raidt S, Bailly G, Holm B and Mixdorff H. Automatic generation of prosody: Comparing two
superpositional systems In: Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2004, Nara, Japan, 2004. 417-420.
Redi L and Shattuck-Hufnagel S. Variation in the realization of glottalization in normal speakers. Journal
of Phonetics 2001;29:407-429.
Rialland A and Robert S. The intonational system of Wolof. Linguistics 2001;39:893939.
Rialland A. African "lax" question prosody: its realisations and its geographical distribution. Lingua
2009;119:928-949.
Rietveld ACM and Gussenhoven C. On the relation between pitch excursion size and prominence.
Journal of Phonetics 1985;13:299-308.
Rump HH and Collier R. Focus conditions and the prominence of pitch-accented syllables. Language and
Speech 1996;39:1-17.
Rump HH and Hermes DJ. Prominence lent by rising and falling pitch movements: Testing two models.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1996;100:1122-1131.
Rush, J. (1827). The philosophy of the human voice. 7th ed. J. B. Ltppincott & Co., Philadelphia, 1879.
Sadat-Tehrani N. The alignment of L+H* pitch accents in Persian intonation. Journal of the International
Phonetic Association 2009;39:205-230.
Sakurai A, Hirose K and Minematsu N. Data-driven generation of F0 contours using a superpositional
model. Speech Communication 2003;40
Sanderman AA and Collier R. Prosodic rules for the implementation of phrase boundaries in synthetic
speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1996;100:3390-3397.
Schafer AJ, Speer SR, Warren P and White D. Intonational disambiguation in sentence production and
comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2000;29:169-182.
Schafer D. The role of intonation as a cue in turn taking in conversation. Journal of Phonetics
1983;11:243-344.
Scherer KR. Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms. Speech Communication
2003;40:227-256.
Scherer KR and Bnziger T. Emotional expression in prosody: a review and an agenda for future research
In: Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2004, 2004. 359-366.
Shami M and Verhelst W. An evaluation of the robustness of existing supervised machine learning
approaches to the classification of emotions in speech. Speech Communication 2007;49:201-212.
Shattuck-Hufnagel S and Turk AE. A Prosody Tutorial for Investigators of Auditory Sentence Processing.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1996;25:193-247.
Shen XS. The use of prosody in disambiguation in Mandarin. Phonetica 1993;50:261-271.
Shriberg EE and Lickley RJ. Intonation of clause-internal filled pauses. Phonetica 1993;50:172-179.
Shue Y-L, Shattuck-Hufnagel S, Iseli M, Jun S-A, Veilleux N and Alwan A. On the acoustic correlates of
high and low nuclear pitch accents in American English. Speech Communication 2010;52:106-122.
Silverman K, Beckman M, Pitrelli J, Ostendorf M, Wightman C, Price P, Pierrehumbert J and Hirschberg
J. ToBI: A standard for labeling English prosody In: Proceedings of The 1992 International Conference
on Spoken Language Processing, Banff, 1992. 867-870.
Sityaev D and House J. Phonetic and Phonological Correlates of Broad, Narrow and Contrastive Focus in
English In: Proceedings of The 1st International Conference on Speech Prosody, Aix-en-Provence,
France, 2002. 1819-1822.
Sluijter AMC and Terken JMB. Beyond sentence prosody: Paragraph intonation in Dutch. Phonetica
1993;50:180-188.
Sluijter AMC and van Heuven VJ. Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America 1996;100:2471-2485.
Smith CL. Topic transitions and durational prosody in reading aloud: production and modeling. speech
communication 2004;42:247-270.
Snedeker J and Casserly E. Is it all relative? Effects of prosodic boundaries on the comprehension and
production of attachment ambiguities. Language and Cognitive Processes 2010;25:1234-1264.
Speer SR, Kjelgaard MM and Dobroth KM. The influence of prosodic structure on the resolution of
temporary syntactic closure ambiguities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1996;25:249-271.
Studdert-Kennedy M and Hadding K. Auditory and linguistic processes in the perception of intonation
contours. Language and Speech 1973;16:293-313.
Sugahara M. Conditions on post-focus dephrasing in Tokyo Japanese. In: Proceedings of The 1st
International Conference on Speech Prosody, Aix-en-Provence, France, 2002.
Sugahara M. Post-focus prosodic phrase boundaries in Tokyo Japanese: asymmetric behavior of an f0 cue
and domain-final lengthening*. Studia Linguistica 2005;59:144-173.
Sun X. The determination, analysis, and synthesis of fundamental frequency [Ph.D. dissertation].
Northwestern University, 2002; 2002.
Swerts M. Prosodic features at discourse boundaries of different length. Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America 1997;101:514-521.
Swerts M and Ostendorf M. Prosodic and lexical indications of discourse structure in human-machine
interactions. Speech Communication 1997;22:25-41.
Swerts M. Prosodic features at discourse boundaries of different length. Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America 1997;101:514-521.
Taylor P. Analysis and synthesis of intonation using the Tilt model. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 2000a;107:1697-1714.
Taylor PA. Concept-to-Speech Synthesis by Phonological Structure Matching. Philosophical
Transactions: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences 2000b;358:1403-1417.
't Hart J, Collier R and Cohen A. A perceptual Study of Intonation An experimental-phonetic approach
to speech melody, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
Terken J. Fundamental frequency and perceived prominence of accented syllables. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 1991;89:1768-1776.
Terken JMB and Hermes DJ. The perception of prosodic prominence. In: M Horne, editors. Prosody:
Theory and experiment. Studies presented to Gsta Bruce, Dordrecht: Kluwer, 2000. 89-127.
Thorsen N. An acoustical investigation of Danish intonation. Journal of Phonetics 1978;6:151-175.
Thorsen NG. A study of the perception of sentence intonation Evidence from Danish. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 1980;67:1014-1030.
Tokuma S and Xu Y. The effect of F0 peak-delay on the L1 / L2 perception of English lexical stress In:
Proceedings of Interspeech 2009, Brighton, UK, 2009. 1687-1690.
Trager, G.L.; Smith, H.L.: An outline of English structure (Battenberg Press, Norman 1951).
Trainor LJ, Austin CM and Desjardins ReN. Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal
Expression of Emotion? Psychological Science 2000;11:188-195.
Traunmller H and Eriksson A. The size of F0 excursions in speech production and perception. Working
Papers. Lund, Sweden: Dept of Linguistics and Phonetics. 43, 1994.
Tseng C-y. Corpus Phonetic Investigations of Discourse Prosody and Higher Level Information.
Language and linguistics 2008:659-719.
Tseng C-y, Su Z-y and Lee L-s. Prosodic Patterns of Information Structure in Spoken Discoursea
Preliminary Study of Mandarin Spontaneous Lecture vs. Read Speech In: Proceedings of Speech Prosody
2010, Chicago, 2010. 100446:1-4.
Ueyama M and Jun S-A. Focus realization in Japanese English and Korean English intonation. Japanese
and Korean Linguistics 1998;7:629-645.
Uldall E. Attitudinal meanings conveyed by intonation contours. Language and Speech 1960;3:223-234.
Umeda N. "F0 declination" is situation dependent. Journal of Phonetics 1982;10:279-290.
van Santen J, Kain A, Klabbers E and Mishra T. Synthesis of prosody using multi-level unit sequences.
Speech Communication 2005;46:365-375.
van Wijk C and Kempen G. A dual system for producing self-repairs in spontaneous speech: Evidence
from experimentally elicited corrections. Cognitive Psychology 1987;19:403-440.
Veronis J, Cristo PD, Courtoise F and Chaumette C. A stochastic model of intonation for text-to-speech
synthesis. Speech Communication 1998;26:233-244.
Wagner M. Prosody and Recursion [Ph.D. Dissertation]. Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2005.
Wagner M and Watson DG. Experimental and theoretical advances in prosody: A review. Language and
Cognitive Processes25:905-945.
Walker J. The melody of speaking delineated: (printed for the author, London 1787; reprinted by The
Scolar Press, Menston. English Linguistics 15001800: No. 218, 1970). 1787.
Wang B and Xu Y. Differential prosodic encoding of topic and focus at sentence initial position in
Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Phonetics in press;
Wang B, Wang L and Kadir T. Prosodic encoding of focus in six languages in China In: Proceedings of
The 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Hong Kong, 2011.
Wells JC. English intonation: an introduction, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006.
Wennerstrom A. Intonation and evaluation in oral narratives. Journal of Pragmatics 2001;33:1183-1206.
White L and Turk AE. English words on the Procrustean bed: Polysyllabic shortening reconsidered.
Journal of Phonetics 2010;38:459-471.
Wichmann A. Attitudinal intonation and the inferential process In: Proceedings of The 1st International
Conference on Speech Prosody, Aix-en-Provence, France, 2002.
Wightman CW, Shattuck-Hufnagel S, Ostendorf M and Price PJ. Segmental durations in the vicinity of
prosodic phrase boundaries. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1992;91:1707-1717.
Williams CE and Stevens KN. Emotion and speech: Some acoustical correlates. Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 1972;52:1238-1250.
Table 1: Hypothetical prosodic functions and their reported encoding schemes. [back]
Functions Languages Main acoustic correlates Studies
Bagshaw 1993, Beyssade et al., 2009, Bolinger 1961, Botinis et al. 1999,
Botinis et al. 2000, Bruce 1982a, 1982b, Chahal 2003, Chen 2006, Chen &
Destruel, 2010, Chen et al., 2009, Cooper et al., 1985, de Jong 1995, 2004,
Egyptian Arabic, English, French, On-focus increase of pitch range, duration de Jong & Zawaydeh 2002, Delais-Roussarie et al. 2002, Di Cristo A and
Hindi, Irish, Japanese, Italian, and intensity; post-focus compression and Jankowski 1999, D'Imperio 2001, Dohen & Lvenbruck 2004, Dorn & N
Korean, Lebanese Arabic, Mandarin, lowering of pitch range and intensity; Chasaide, Eady et al., 1986, Gerard & Dahan 1995, Face 2006, Fry &
Persian, Swedish, Tibetan, Turkish, change of pitch targets of on-focus Kgler 2008, Heldner 2003, Heldner & Strangert 2001, Hellmuth 2006,
Uygur stressed syllable (English) Ipek 2011, Jin 1996, Kabagema-Bilan 2011, Lecumberri 1997, Lee 1956,
Lee & Xu 2010, Nooteboom & Kruyt 1987, Patil et al. 2008, Rump &
Focus
Collier 1996, Sadat-Tehrani 2009, Sugahara 2002, 2005, Ueyama & Jun
1998, Wang et al. 2011, Xu 1999, Xu & Xu 2005, Xu et al. 2004,
Cantonese, Buli, Chichewa,
Chichewa, Chitumbuka, Dagbani, Chen et al. 2009; Downing 2008, Gu & Lee 2007; Hartmann 2008,
Deang, Durban Zulu, Ewe, Hausa, Hartmann & Zimmermann 2007, Jannedy 2007, Kgler & Skopeteas 2007,
Lack of post-focus compression of pitch
Konni, Northern Sotho, Qiang, Nguy et al. 2008, Pan 2007, Rialland & Robert 2001, Schwarz 2009, Wang
range and intensity
Taiwanese, Tangale, Vietnamese, et al. 2011, Wu & Chung 2011, Wu & Xu 2010, Xu 2011, Zerbian et al.
Wa, Wolof, Yi, Yucatec Maya 2010
2001, Sanderman & Collier 1996, Swerts 1997, Wagner 2005, Wightman et
al. 1992, Xu & Wang 2009
Lehiste 1975, Sluijter & Terken 1993, Smith 2004, Tseng 2008, Umeda
Topic Dutch, English, Mandarin Increased sentence-initial F0
1982, Wang & Xu in press
Contrastive topic No experimental investigations
Turn taking Dutch, English Map Task dialogues Caspers 2003, de Ruiter 2006, Oliveira & Freitas 2008, Schafer 1983
Delattre et al. 1965, Eady & Cooper 1986, Grding 1979, 1987, Ho 1976,
Danish, English, German, Japanese, Descriptive monologues, map task 1977, Lin 2004, Liu, 2010, Liu & Xu 2005, 2007a, 2007b, Makarova 2001,
Russian, Swedish dialogues McRoberts et al. 1995, Studdert-Kennedy & Hadding 1973, Thorsen 1978,
Modality, question vs. 1980
statement Multiple languages in the Niger- Lack of final rising;
Congo, Nilo-Saharan, Afro-Asiatic, Falling pitch contour, sentence-final low
Rialland 2009
Nilo-Saharan and Afro-Asiatic vowel, vowel lengthening, and a breathy
superfamilies utterance termination
Anttila et al. 2010, Barbosa 2007, Braun & Tagliapietra 2010, Birch &
Prosody-Syntactic
Clifton 1995, Bock & Mazzella 1983, Christophe et al. 2003, Cole et al.
interaction, Syntactic English, Finnish, French, Georgian,
Multiple cues 2010, Donati & Nespor 2003, Fery et al. 2010, Grassmann & Tomasello
disambiguation, impact German, Italian, Japanese, Mandarin
2010, Ishihara 2003, Kjelgaard & Speer 1999, Lehiste 1976, Schafer et al.
on comprehension
2000, Shen 1993, Snedeker & Casserly 2010, Speer et al. 1996
Auberge & Cathiard 2003, Barbosa 2009, Bnziger & Scherer 2005, Baum
& Nowicki 1998, Beller 2008, Breitenstein 2001, Bulut & Narayanan 2008,
Chuenwattanapranithi et al. 2008, Frick 1985, Gobl & Chasaide 2003, Ladd
French, English, German, Thai,
Emotion Multiple cues et al. 1985, Mauss & Robinson 2009, Ohala 1996, Pell 2001, Protopapas &
Brazilian Portuguese
Lieberman 1997, Scherer & Bnziger 2004, Shami & Verhelst 2007,
Trainor et al. 2000, Wennerstrom 2001, Wildgruber et al. 2005, Williams &
Stevens 1972, Xu & Kelly 2010
Ambrazaitis 2005, Chen et al. 2004, House 2005, Morlec et al. 2001,
Dutch, English, French, German,
Attitude Multiple cues Mozziconacci 2001, Ofuka et al. 2000, O'Shaughnessy & Allen 1983,
Japanese, Swedish
Uldall 1960, Wichmann 2002
Table 3: Types of spontaneous speech and labeling schemes used in a number of studies. [back]
Study Language Type of spontaneous speech Prosodic transcription/annotation
Swedish, French Restricted samples from conversations, interviews, Accentual prominence; phrasing; pitch range; boundary tones; pausing
Bruce & Touati, 1992 political debates and radio programs
Campbell, 2004 Japanese Phone dialogue, daily conversation Speaker state, speaking style, voice type
Caspers, 2003 Dutch Map task dialogues ToDI (http://todi.let.kun.nl/), transition type
Edlund & Heldner, 2005 Swedish Map task dialogues Turn types
Danish Descriptive monologues, map task dialogues Pitch relation between stressed and immediate post-tonic syllable;
Grnnum, 2009 phrasal intonation contour
House, 2005 Swedish Human-machine dialogues Presence of final rise and final focal accent
Kohler, 1997 German Unspecified Kiel intonation model (KIM) with markers of 10 prosodic domains
Nakajima & Allen, 1993 English Map task dialogues Discourse structure markers in terms of topic boundary classes
Ogden & Routarinne, 2005 Finnish Phone calls and face-to-face conversations Overlapping talk, pause location & duration
American & Human-machine dialogues Filled pauses and surrounding f0 values
Shriberg & Lickley, 1993 British English
Swerts & Ostendorf, 1997 English Human-machine dialogues Discourse segmentation, utterance purpose
Swerts, 1997 Dutch Monologues describing paintings Boundary tones, boundary strength
Terken, 1984 Dutch Instruction monologues Pitch accents based on perceptual relevant F0 movements
Tseng et al., 2010 Mandarin Classroom lectures Discourse boundaries and phrasing units, emphasis
van Wijk & Kempen, 1987 Dutch Picture description task Self-repairs types
Table 4: Predictors (input features) used in various studies modeling pitch contours and duration. [back].
Study Language Predictors (input features) Output
French Prosodic attitudes applied to sentences, dependency relations applied to syntactic constituents of read text or operands/operators of F0, duration
Bailly & Holm 2005 spoken math, cliticization typically applied to determiners and auxiliaries, narrow focus applied to words, lexical tones in Mandarin
Bellegarda et al. American ToBI transcription F0
2001 English 40 factors, unspecified
Brazilian Position and magnitude of underlying phrase stress, and a set of dynamical control parameters Duration
Barbosa 2007, 2009 Portuguese
Kochanski et al. Mandarin Lexical tone, prosodic strength, position in word F0
2003
Fletcher & McVeigh Australian Number of phonemes, nature of syllabic peak, position in word, position in intermediate & intonational Duration
1993 English Phrase, degree of stress, grammatical function of word, position in foot
Fujisaki et al. 2005 Mandarin Syllable duration, amplitude of preceding tone command, which constrain timing and amplitude of tone command F0
German Boundary depth, strength, nucleus schwa/non-schwa, type of intoneme, part-of-speech, phrase index in sentence, number of phones F0
Mixdorff & Jokisch in syllable onset, number of phones in syllable rhyme, duration of preceding phrase, amplitude of preceding phrase command,
2001 duration of current phrase, distance from preceding phrase command, coda voiced
Mohler & Conkie American 43 unspecified features, including accent type F0
1998 English
French Declarative, question, exclamation, incredulous question, suspicious irony, obviousness, Inter Perceptual Centre F0, duration
Morlec et al. 2001 Group ratio (IPCG_ratio)
Ni et al. 2006 Mandarin Tone, sentence modality F0
American Tone, lexical stress, focus, position in sentence F0
English,
Prom-on et al. 2009 Mandarin
French, German SFC: Utterance level modality and prosodic attitude at the utterance level; syntactic structure mathematical operators F0
IGM: 14 input parameters: Syllable level nature of the phones included in the components of the syllable (syllable, onset, rhyme);
Raidt et al. 2004 word level accentuation and the part of speech; phrase level or higher neighboring boundaries and composition of current unit
Japanese Position of accentual phrase in utterance, number of morae, accent type, number of words, part of speech of first and last words, F0
Sakurai et al. 2003 conjugation of first and last words
Dutch Perceived boundary strength Duration,
Sanderman & stylized F0
Collier 1996 contour
American Vowel type, coda type, syllable stress, syllable position in word, number of syllable in word, pitch accent type, phrase accent type, F0
Sun 2002 English number of syllables from major phrase break, part of speech, word position in sentence, number of words from major phrase break
American No. of phonemes in the syllable, nature of syllabic peak (tense / lax vowel / diphthong / sonorant consonant), Position of syllable in Duration
van Santen & Shih English, foot, Position of syllable in phrase and clause, Stress assigned to syllable, and nature of pitch movement, Function/content role of the
2000 Mandarin parent word
Table 5: Degrees of freedom (number of free parameters to be specified) of various models at the local level. [back]
Model Study df Free local parameters
SFC Bailly & Holm 2005 5 3 F0 values per vocalic nucleus, 1 lengthening factor, Temporal scope of function
8 Tone template (5 pitch values), word strength, position of template relative to syllable, length of template
Stem-ML Kochanski et al. 2003 relative to the syllable
5 Amplitude of 1st command of syllable, amplitude of 2nd command of syllable, onset time of 1st command of
syllable, end time of 1st command (and onset of the 2nd command if the second command exists) of syllable,
Fujisaki Fujisaki et al. 2005 end of 2nd command if 2nd command exists
15 F0 peaks for each tone (2 parameters, bottom and top frequencies of voice register of speaker, bottom and top
values of voice register on the RONDO scale, damping ratio of forced vibration, peak coordination (2
parameters), parameters controlling rising characteristics of tone (2), parameters controlling falling
Tone transformation Ni & Hirose 2000, Ni et al. 2006 characteristics of tone (2)
4 F0 topline (height, slope), F0 baseline (height, slope), height of F0 turning point (target value), time of F0
Sagging transition Pierrehumbert 1981 turning point
qTA Prom-on et al. 2009 3 Target height, target slope, rate of target approximation
Target approximation Sun 2002 3 Target height, target slope, rate of target approximation
Tilt Taylor 2000 5 Tilt, tilt amplitude, tile duration, tilt alignment, syllabic position