Salls 2013
Salls 2013
To cite this article: Joyce Salls OTD OTR/L , Jeryl D. Benson EdD OTR/L , Mary A. Hansen PhD ,
Kari Cole MS OTR/L & Amanda Pielielek MS OTR/L (2013) A Comparison of the Handwriting without
Tears Program and Peterson Directed Handwriting Program on Handwriting Performance in Typically
Developing First Grade Students, Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, 6:2,
131-142, DOI: 10.1080/19411243.2013.810958
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19411243.2013.810958
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, 6:131–142, 2013
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1941-1243 print / 1941-1251 online
DOI: 10.1080/19411243.2013.810958
The purpose of this study was to compare the results of two handwriting curricula
on handwriting performance in typically developing first grade students. Each class-
room implemented a different handwriting curriculum throughout the school year as
part of the regular first grade curriculum. One classroom implemented the Handwriting
Without Tears program, while the other used the Peterson Directed Handwriting cur-
riculum. Handwriting skills were assessed at the beginning, middle, and end of the
school year. Student handwriting performance improved for both classrooms over the
course of the school year. This suggests that for typically developing first grade students,
implementation of a structured curriculum may be key to developing good handwriting
skills.
131
132 J. Salls et al.
change to the College Board SAT test (2005) requiring students to handwrite the essay
portion of the test, the ability to write legibly remains an important skill beyond the ele-
mentary school years. Children with poor or illegible writing may experience a variety of
academic challenges, including inability to complete written assignments in a timely man-
ner, lower grades due to illegibility, and producing shorter creative writing assignments due
to writing fatigue (Chase, 1986; Graham, 1980; Tseng & Cermak, 1993).
Prior studies have noted that handwriting is a leading area of referrals for school-based
occupational therapists (Asher, 2006; Marr & Cermak, 2002) and therefore a major focus
of intervention (Hammerschmidt & Sudsawad, 2004; Hoy, Egan, & Feder, 2011). It has
been estimated that 98% of school-based occupational therapy referrals are related to issues
with handwriting (Tait, 1998; Marr & Dimeo, 2006). Although teachers are the primary
instructors of handwriting in the elementary school curriculum, when children experience
difficulties with handwriting, they are frequently referred to occupational therapy for
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
to support a reliable and efficient method of instruction could provide the practitioner
with the documentation needed to support professional reasoning. The purpose of this
study was to compare two handwriting curricula, Handwriting Without Tears (HWT)
and the Peterson Directed Handwriting Program, on handwriting performance in typ-
ically developing first grade students. Specifically, the research question under study
was, For typically developing first graders, is there a difference between handwrit-
ing performance in first grade students who receive instruction in the HWT versus
the Peterson Directed Handwriting Program, as measured by three different instru-
ments?
Methods
Instruments
Several instruments were utilized in the current study to measure various aspects of
handwriting quality of students in the two classrooms, including the Print Tool, the
134 J. Salls et al.
Table 1
Student Demographics by School
Right- Left-
Total Male Female Handed Handed Age
Curriculum n n % n % n % n % m Range
Peterson 17 11 64.7 6 35.3 16 94.1 1 5.9 6.6 6.0–7.1
HWT 14 6 42.9 8 57.1 12 85.7 2 14.3 6.5 6.0–6.11
Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (5th edition; VMI), and the Minnesota
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
Handwriting Assessment (MHA; Reisman, 1999). The Print Tool instrument is used for
evaluating use of capitals, lowercase letters, and numbers for children in grades K to 4
(Olsen & Knapton, 2006). With the Print Tool, eight basic printing skills are assessed:
memory, orientation, placement, size, start, sequence, control, and spacing. Four scores are
available from the Print Tool: the total percent correct, the capital total percent correct,
the lowercase total percent correct, and the number total percent correct. The second mea-
sure used to assess handwriting in the current study was the Beery Test of Visual Motor
Integration (VMI). The VMI is a widely used pencil-and-paper test that requires the child
to copy a series of geometric forms from a test booklet. Graphic responses are scored using
the criterion listed in the manual. The VMI is a norm-referenced standardized assessment
and has been shown to have high reliability and validity (Beery & Beery, 2006). Both scale
scores and standard scores from the VMI were used in the current study. The third tool
utilized in the study to assess handwriting was the Minnesota Handwriting Assessment
(MHA). This norm-referenced standardized tool is used to analyze the handwriting skills
of first and second grade students. It addresses standard manuscript and D’Nealian styles of
print. Students are instructed to copy a sample sentence that contains eight words. Scores
are based on rate and on quality, as indicated by five subscales (legibility, form, align-
ment, size, and spacing). Inter-rater reliability is high (r = .98), and test-retest reliability is
moderate (r = .72; Feder & Majnemer, 2003).
Procedure
A sample of two first grade classrooms was chosen based on the handwriting curriculum
being implemented. In one classroom, the teacher was trained in the Peterson Directed
Handwriting curriculum. The Peterson Directed Handwriting curriculum is based on
the principles of motor learning utilizing a cognitive approach to letter formation with
reinforcement rooted in directed practice and repetition (Nelson, 2006). In the other class-
room, the teacher was trained to implement the HWT program. The HWT Program is a
sensorimotor-based curriculum engaging the children in learning tasks that reinforce letter
formation via multiple sensory systems (Olsen, 2003). Both teachers had prior experience
with the programs they were teaching. The children were assigned a number based on a
classroom roster and were tested on all three measures (Print Tool, VMI, and MHA) at
the beginning, midpoint, and end of the school year. The evaluators were eight occupa-
tional therapy graduate students who were trained by the first two authors to administer and
score each of the three instruments. Training for the tools consisted of administration and
scoring of the tool until intra-rater reliability exceeded 95% for each evaluator. Training
Comparison of Two Handwriting Curricula 135
to address reliability between evaluators consisted of discussion on all items where there
was disagreement until consensus was reached. Evaluators began data collection after 100%
agreement was reached on three separate trials. During data collection, each instrument was
scored independently by two evaluators. When there was disagreement in the scoring, the
evaluators and the second author reviewed the scoring together until 100% agreement was
reached. All evaluators were blinded to the curriculum of the children they were testing.
Data Analysis
To compare the effectiveness of the two handwriting curricula, two-way repeated-measures
analyses of variance (ANOVA) were run, with type of instruction as the between-groups
independent variable having two levels (HWT and Peterson), and time as the within-groups
independent variable having three levels (baseline, midpoint, and final). Repeated-measures
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
ANOVAs were run using several dependent variables, including total score, capital score,
lowercase score, and number score from the Print Tool; raw score, scale score, standard
score, and percentile from the VMI; and rate, legibility, form, alignment, size, and spacing
scores from the MHA. A variety of instruments were used to measure several aspects of
handwriting in the study, because corresponding results across instruments would provide
stronger evidence of the effectiveness of one handwriting curriculum over the other than
would results based on only one instrument. Additionally, the instruments chosen for use
in the study measure different aspects of handwriting.
Results
Print Tool
Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations for each of the Print Tool scores for the
classrooms using the HWT and Peterson curricula at each time point. Scores on the Print
Tool were similar at each time point for the two classrooms.
Table 2
Print Tool Descriptive Statistics and Significance Levels for Tests of Simple Effects
Repeated-measures ANOVA results based on the Print Tool showed that Mauchly’s
Sphericty assumption was met for Print Tool Total (p = .10), Print Tool Capital (p =
.15), Print Tool Lowercase (p = .08), and Print Tool Number (p = .18). Therefore, the
univariate approach to repeated-measures ANOVA was utilized, as the univariate tests are
more powerful than their multivariate counterparts when the sphericity assumption is met.
Results showed that there were no significant interactions between handwriting method
and time for Print Tool Total scores (F(2, 58) = 1.91, p = .16, η2 = .06), Print Tool
Capital scores (F(2, 58) = 1.46, p = .24, η2 = .05), Print Tool Lowercase (F(2, 58) =
1.75, p = .18, η2 = .06), or Print Tool Number (F(2, 58) = 0.02, p = .98, η2 = .00). With
no significant interaction effects, the main effects for handwriting curricula and time were
investigated.
Repeated-measures ANOVA results showed that there were no significant between-
group main effects for any of the Print Tool scores, indicating that the differences in
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
handwriting as measured by the Print Tool were not statistically significant for the class-
rooms receiving the HWT versus Peterson curricula. Specifically, the between-group
differences were not significant for the Print Tool Total (F(1, 29) = .02, p = 90, η2 = .00),
Print Tool Capital (F(1, 29) = 0.62, p = .44, η2 = .02), Print Tool Lowercase (F(1, 29) =
0.88, p = .34, η2 = .03), or Print Tool Number (F(1, 29) = 0.02, p = .89, η2 = .00). Again,
these results suggest that there is little difference in the effect on handwriting, as measured
by the Print Tool, of the HWT versus Peterson curricula.
There was a significant main effect for time for each of Print Tool scores, indicating
that, as would be expected during the course of a school year, scores increased over time.
Specifically, the within-group differences for time were significant for the Print Tool Total
(F(2, 58) = 21.91, p = .00, η2 = .43), Print Tool Capital (F(2, 58) = 7.94, p = .00, η2 =
.22), Print Tool Lowercase (F(2, 58) = 54.56, p = .00, η2 = .65), and Print Tool Number
(F(2, 58) = 12.07, p = .00, η2 = .29). As measured by the Print Tool, the groups receiv-
ing the two handwriting curricula saw similar and statistically significant gains over time.
In order to investigate further whether there were differences between the two curricula at
any individual time point, tests of simple effects were carried out. As is seen in Table 2, the
differences between HWT and Peterson classrooms were not significant at any time point,
with each p-value in Table 2 being larger than .05. Therefore, both curricula seem to be
equally effective as measured by the Print Tool.
VMI Results
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics and tests of simple effects for the VMI Raw, Standard,
Scale and percentile scores for the classrooms receiving the HWT and Peterson curricula
at the baseline, midpoint, and final time points.
Mauchly’s test showed that the sphericity assumption was met for VMI Raw (p =
.711), VMI Standard (p = .51), VMI Scale (p = .61), and VMI Percentile scores (p =
.35). Repeated-measures ANOVA results showed that there were no significant interactions
between handwriting method and time for VMI raw scores (F(2, 58) = 1.02, p = .37,
η2 = .03), VMI standard scores (F(2, 58) = 1.55, p = .22, η2 = .05), VMI scale scores
(F(2, 58) = 1.95, p = .15, η2 = .06), or VMI percentile scores (F(2, 58) = 1.93, p = .16,
η2 = .06). With no significant interaction effects, the main effects for handwriting curricula
and time on the VMI scores were investigated.
Repeated-measures ANOVA results showed that there were significant between group
main effects for the VMI standard scores (F(1, 29) = 4.48, p = .04, η2 = .13), VMI scale
scores (F(1, 29) = 4.34, p = .045, η2 = .13), and VMI Percentile scores (F(1, 29) = 5.21,
Comparison of Two Handwriting Curricula 137
Table 3
VMI Descriptive Statistics and Significance Levels for Tests of Simple Effects
Percentile 59.4 50.5 .33 71.6 47.5 .01∗ 69.4 49.9 .03∗
(24.4) (25.2) (19.1) (30.0) (19.5) (26.7)
p = .03, η2 = .15); however, effect sizes for each of these scores were small. Between-group
differences for the VMI raw scores were statistically significant at the alpha = .10 level of
significance (F(1, 29) = 3.3, p = .08, η2 = .10). These results show that differences between
handwriting of students receiving the HWT versus the Peterson curricula were statistically
significant as measured by the VMI. Effect sizes were small indicating that, while results
were statistically significant, there is little practical significance between the two curricula.
There were significant main effects for time for only the VMI raw scores (F(2, 58) =
12.66, p = .00, η2 = .30) indicating that the raw scores increased over time. Further, there
were no significant changes in VMI standard scores (F(2, 58) = 0.67, p = .52, η2 = .02),
VMI scale scores (F(2, 58) = 1.13, p = .33, η2 = .04), or VMI percentiles (F(2, 58) =
0.89, p = 42, η2 = .03) over time. These results indicate, counter to results for the Print
Tool, that VMI scores were relatively stable over time, showing little increase for students
receiving both handwriting curricula.
With no main effects for time for these three standardized scores, it was important
to investigate simple effects, which show (1) whether there are differences in performance
across the three time points for either handwriting curricula and (2) whether there are differ-
ences at any single time point between the groups receiving the two different handwriting
curricula.
Figure 1 shows the VMI raw scores and standard scores for the two groups at each time
point. Similar patterns to the VMI standard scores are seen in the VMI scale scores and VMI
percentiles, so those values are not plotted. Supporting the means shown in Table 3, there
are differences between the VMI standard scores between the two schools, which were not
significantly different at Time 1 (p = .36) but were statistically significant at Time 2 (p =
.02) and Time 3 (p = .05). The differences at all time points were small. Figure 3 also shows
that students following the HWT curriculum showed larger gains in raw scores and standard
scores than students following the Peterson curriculum, and at Time 2, the standard scores
for the Peterson group decreased. Again, the differences between students receiving each
curricula at Times 2 and 3 were statistically significant, but those differences have little
practical meaning. Therefore, as seen by the VMI scores, there is little difference between
the two curricula, and VMI scores were relatively stable over time.
138 J. Salls et al.
Peterson
25 VMI Raw
Score
21.4
20.7
19.0 HWT VMI
20
Raw Score
19.8
18.8
18.2
Scores
15 Peterson
VMI
12.0 11.9 Standard
10.9 Score
10 HWT VMI
10.1 10.1 Standard
9.7
Score
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
5
Baseline Midpoint Final
Table 4
MHA Subscale Descriptive Statistics and Significance Levels for Tests of Simple Effects
0.00, η2 = .29), and size (F(2,58) = 9.7, p = 0.00, η2 = .25) subscales increased over time
for students using both curricula. Time was not a significant factor for legibility (F(2,58) =
57, p = .57, η2 = .02), form (F(2,58) = .46, p = .63, η2 = .02), or spacing (F(2,58) = .43,
p = .67, η2 = .01), indicating that the scores on those subscales remained consistent over
time.
Results for the MHA subscales are similar for the two classrooms. Statistical tests
showed few significant differences between the two groups receiving the two curricula.
The data shown in Table 4 indicate that scores for students receiving HWT are slightly
higher than scores for students receiving the Peterson curriculum on most subscales, but
most of these differences are not statistically significant.
In order to further explore the data, differences between the performance of boys and
girls on each of the instruments were examined. The sample sizes in the two classrooms
did not allow for valid statistical comparisons of gender between the two curricula, but
exploratory independent samples t-tests indicated that there were no statistically significant
differences between the genders on any of the instruments at alpha = .01. Therefore, the
study indicated that the performance by gender was similar.
skills. Three different handwriting instruments, each measuring different aspects of hand-
writing, were utilized. Overall, students receiving the two different handwriting curricula
performed similarly on each measure and subscale. Generally, students in the HWT class-
room showed higher gains sooner on several measures, but in most cases, the differences
were not statistically significant. For most instances when differences occurred, scores in
the HWT classroom were higher. The initial higher gains may be attributed to the vari-
ety, novelty, and multisensory nature of the materials presented in the HWT curriculum,
leading to increased motivation. It is also possible that these differences may have been
greater with a larger sample size. Additionally, it was noted that VMI scores remained rel-
atively stable over the course of the school year for both groups. It is possible that the VMI
is not sensitive enough to detect the changes that occur in handwriting during a 9-month
time period whereas the other two measures used did reveal changes in scores during the
same period of time. Although the VMI has been cited as a good predictor for learning the
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
beginning skills of writing letters in kindergarten (Daly, Kelley, & Krauss, 2003; Weil &
Cunningham Amundson, 1994), there is no evidence that it predicts handwriting ability in
first grade. Overvelde and Hulstijn (2011) found that the VMI was not an appropriate tool
for assessing handwriting ability in children in grades 2 and 3.
The findings of this study support the use of either curriculum in teaching typically
developing children the essential skills of handwriting. What appears to be most important
is the implementation of a formal curriculum that allows for ongoing handwriting instruc-
tion and practice. Hoy et al. (2011), in an extensive review of the literature, supported that
effective handwriting interventions require meeting at least twice a week for 20 sessions,
and improvements in handwriting speed require even more practice. This is consistent with
the motor learning theory, which emphasizes the role of practice and experience in improv-
ing skilled movement (Zwicker & Harris, 2009). Zwicker and Hadwin (2009), in a study
comparing a multisensory approach to handwriting to a cognitive approach in a group of
first and second grade students, found no significant differences between the two groups.
This could support the idea that implementation of any formal handwriting curriculum with
typically developing children may improve handwriting skills. However, with the growing
number of children with special needs who are receiving occupational therapy services
due to handwriting deficits, it may be beneficial to implement a multisensory program that
can be effective in both environments. Additionally, use of the HWT program may sup-
port inclusion, providing students with special needs the opportunity to learn and reinforce
handwriting skills in the regular classroom setting. Finally, occupational therapists can play
a key role in handwriting instruction through early intervention services, including response
to intervention. This could include providing consultation to school administrators and staff
regarding the importance of handwriting curricula, screening children, and support to class-
room teachers on evidenced-based handwriting instructional methods that would benefit
all students. Benson, Salls, and Perry (2010) suggest collaboration between teachers and
occupational therapists for curriculum selection and implementation.
One limitation to the design of the current study was the fact that a control group,
which did not implement a handwriting curriculum, was not utilized. However, a control
group was not considered necessary as current literature already supports that handwriting
improves with instruction (Shimel, Candler, & Neville-Smith, 2009). In addition, with-
holding handwriting instruction from a group of first graders for an entire school year was
deemed unethical. Therefore, this limitation to the design is seen as a positive aspect of
the current study, since students in both classrooms were able to receive formal handwrit-
ing instruction. Another limitation is the small sample size, limiting generalizability of the
study. Post hoc power analyses given the smaller sample sizes utilized in the study indicate
Comparison of Two Handwriting Curricula 141
that the current study had low power. However, the study served as a pilot study for the dis-
trict in question, as they utilized the results of the study in determining which handwriting
curriculum to implement. The setting in which the study was conducted, a suburban middle-
class school district, further limits generalizability. Finally, this study compared only two
handwriting curricula. Future studies could compare the effectiveness of other commercial
handwriting programs as well as the efficacy of these curricula in improving handwriting
performance in students with special needs.
References
Asher, A. V. (2006). Handwriting instruction in elementary schools. American Journal of
Occupational Therapy, 60, 461–471.
Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2006). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor inte-
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
gration with supplemental developmental tests of visual motor integration and motor coordination
and stepping stones age norms from birth to age six: Administration, scoring and teaching manual
(5th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.
Benson, J. D., Salls, J., & Perry, C. (2010). A pilot study of teachers’ perceptions of two handwriting
curricula: Handwriting Without Tears and the Peterson directed handwriting method. Journal of
Occupational Therapy, Schools, and Early Intervention, 3, 319–330.
Bonney, M. (1992). Understanding and assessing handwriting difficulty: Perspectives from the
literature. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 39, 7–15.
Cahill, S. M. (2009). Where does handwriting fit in? Strategies to support academic achievement.
Intervention in School and Clinic, 44, 223–228.
Chase, C. I. (1986). Essay test scoring: Interaction of relevant variables. Journal of Educational
Measurement, 23(1), 33–41.
Daly, C. J., Kelley, G. T., & Krauss, A. (2003). Brief report—Relationship between visual-motor inte-
gration and handwriting skills of children in kindergarten: A modified replication study. American
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57, 459–462.
Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2003). Children’s handwriting evaluation tools and their psychometric
properties. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 23(3), 65–84.
Graham, S., & Miller, L. (1980). Handwriting research and practice: A unified approach. Focus on
Exceptional Children, 13(2), 1–16.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Mason, L., Fink-Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., & Saddler, B. (2007). How do
primary grade teachers teach handwriting? A national survey. Reading and Writing. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1007/s11145-007-9064-z
Hammerschmidt, S. L., & Sudsawad, P. (2004). Teachers’ survey on problems with handwriting:
Referral, evaluation, and outcomes. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 58, 185–192.
Hoy, M., Egan, M., & Feder, K. (2011). A systematic review of interventions to improve handwriting.
The Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 78(1), 13–25. Retrieved from ProQuest Nursing
& Allied Health Source.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act. (2004). Pub. L. 108–446, 20 U.S.C 1400
et seq.
Marr, D., & Cermak, S. (2002). Consistency of handwriting in early elementary students. American
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57, 161–167.
Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Fine motor activities in Head Start and
kindergarten classrooms. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57, 550–557.
Marr, D., & Dimeo, S. B. (2006). Outcomes associated with a summer handwriting course for
elementary students. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60, 10–15.
McHale, K., & Cermak, S. A. (1992). Fine motor activities in elementary school: Preliminary find-
ings and provisional implications for children with fine motor problems. American Journal of
Occupational Therapy, 46, 898–903.
No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. (2002). Pub. L. No. 107-110, 115 Stat.1425.
142 J. Salls et al.
Nelson, R. H. (2006). The Peterson Method: A research-based strategy for teaching and learn-
ing motor skills for written language. Retrieved from http://www.peterson-handwriting.com/
ServicePage/PdhStrategy.pdf
Olsen, J. Z. (2003). Handwriting Without Tears. Potomac, MD: Handwriting Without Tears.
Olsen, J. Z., & Knapton, E. (2006). The print tool (2nd ed.). Cabin John, MD: Handwriting Without
Tears.
Overvelde, A., & Hulstijn, W. (2011). Handwriting development in grade 2 and grade 3 primary
school children with normal, at risk, or dysgraphic characteristics. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 32, 540–548.
Reisman, J. (1999). Minnesota Handwriting Assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Corporation.
Sheffield, B. (1996). Handwriting: A neglected cornerstone of literacy. Annuals of Dyslexia, 45,
21–35.
Shimel, K., Candler, C., & Neville-Smith, M. (2009). Comparison of cursive handwriting instruc-
Downloaded by [Chulalongkorn University] at 06:19 09 January 2015
tion programs among students without identified problems. Physical & Occupational Therapy in
Pediatrics, 29, 170–181
Tait, I. (1998). Survey of school based therapists on evaluation and reference. (Unpublished master’s
thesis). The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio.
Tseng, M. H., & Cermak, S. A. (1993). The influence of ergonomic factors and perceptual-motor
abilities on handwriting performance. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47, 919–926.
Wallace, R. R., & Schomer, J. H. (1994). Simplifying handwriting instruction for the 21st century.
Education, 114, 413–417.
Weil, M. J., & Cunningham Amundson, S. J. (1994). Relationship between visuomotor and hand-
writing skills of children in kindergarten. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 48,
982–988.
Weintraub, N., Yinon, M., Hirsch, I., & Parush, S. (2009). Effectiveness of sensorimotor and
task-oriented handwriting intervention in elementary school-aged students with handwriting diffi-
culties. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 29(3), 125–134. Retrieved from ProQuest
Nursing & Allied Health Source.
Zwicker, J., & Hadwin, A. (2009). Cognitive versus multisensory approaches to handwriting inter-
vention: A randomized controlled trial. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 29(1),
40–48. Retrieved from ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source.
Zwicker, J., & Harris, S. (2009). A reflection on motor learning theory in pediatric occupational
therapy practice. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 76, 29–37.