Thanks to visit codestin.com
Credit goes to www.scribd.com

0% found this document useful (0 votes)
133 views6 pages

Sweller CognitiveLoad

The document discusses cognitive load theory and its implications for teaching English as a second language to adult learners. It argues that unlike children, adults do not learn a second language the same way as a first language through immersion alone. As adults have different cognitive architectures than children for acquiring new knowledge, effective instruction for adults requires explicit teaching methods that consider the limitations of working memory. The document outlines principles of human cognitive architecture and their relevance to designing instruction that facilitates long-term learning of a second language.

Uploaded by

asayu
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
133 views6 pages

Sweller CognitiveLoad

The document discusses cognitive load theory and its implications for teaching English as a second language to adult learners. It argues that unlike children, adults do not learn a second language the same way as a first language through immersion alone. As adults have different cognitive architectures than children for acquiring new knowledge, effective instruction for adults requires explicit teaching methods that consider the limitations of working memory. The document outlines principles of human cognitive architecture and their relevance to designing instruction that facilitates long-term learning of a second language.

Uploaded by

asayu
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 6

ARTICLES

COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY AND TEACHING


ENGLISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE TO
ADULT LEARNERS
By John Sweller, University of New South Wales

Most second language teaching recommendations place a considerable emphasis on


“naturalistic” procedures such as immersion within a second language environment.
Immersion means exposing learners to the second language in many of their daily
activities, including other educational activities ostensibly unrelated to learning the
second language. While immersion may assist in learning a second language, anyone who
has lived in an immigrant society cannot fail to have noticed the many adults who learn
almost nothing of the second language despite years or even decades of immersion.
Furthermore, within an academic environment, even if immersion assists in learning
the second language, it is likely to be associated with a considerable decline in learning
the associated academic subjects. Simple immersion is unlikely to be effective.

While not always explicitly stated, the argument for immersion seems to be: “Look how
easy it is for people to learn their native language. Despite little explicit vocabulary or
grammar instruction, they pick it up easily and effortlessly within a few years simply by
immersion in their native language environment. If we use the same procedures for a
second language, it too will be learned easily, effortlessly, and largely unconsciously.”

For young children, this argument probably is valid. For adults beginning to learn a
second language, the argument is almost certainly invalid. Adults do not learn a second
language in the same way as young children learn a native language. The suggestion that
learning a second language should mimic the learning of a first language ignores much of
what we know of human cognitive architecture and its consequences for instructional
procedures. Cognitive load theory (Sweller, 2015, 2016; Sweller, Ayres, & Kalyuga, 2011)
uses our knowledge of human cognition to devise instructional procedures. That theory
will be used to structure the remainder of this article, beginning with our knowledge of
human cognition.

Human Cognitive Architecture


Categories of Knowledge
For current purposes, there are two categories of knowledge (Geary, 2012; Geary & Berch,
2016): Biologically (or evolutionary) primary and biologically secondary knowledge.
Primary knowledge is knowledge we have evolved to acquire over countless generations.

-1- TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,


ARTICLES

It is acquired easily and without conscious effort. It is modular in the sense that we may
have evolved to acquire different types of knowledge during different evolutionary epochs
and so the cognitive procedures associated with the acquisition of different types of
primary knowledge are likely to differ. Learning a native language provides an example
of a category of biologically primary knowledge. We have evolved to acquire listening and
speaking skills in a native language and so can acquire the skills without conscious effort
or explicit instruction.

Biologically secondary knowledge is required for cultural reasons. We have evolved to


acquire secondary knowledge in general but we have not specifically evolved to acquire a
particular category of secondary knowledge. The manner in which secondary knowledge
is acquired tends to be similar irrespective of its category but vastly different to the
acquisition of primary knowledge. All categories of secondary knowledge are acquired
with conscious effort and unlike primary knowledge, are best acquired with explicit
instruction (Kirschner, Sweller, & Clark, 2006; Sweller, Kirschner, & Clark, 2007).
Learning a second language as an adult provides an example of secondary knowledge
acquisition as do most of the topics covered in educational institutions. We invented
education to deal with biologically secondary information. Learning to listen to and speak
a second language as an adult requires conscious effort on the part of the learner and
explicit instruction on the part of instructors. Little will be learned solely by immersion.
Furthermore, since learning to read and write are biologically secondary because we have
not evolved to acquire these skills, they also require conscious effort by learners and
explicit teaching by instructors, irrespective of whether we are dealing with a native or
second language.

Human Cognitive Architecture Associated with Biologically Secondary


Knowledge
Learning a second language as an adult conforms to the structures and processes
associated with acquiring any other category of biologically secondary knowledge (Sweller
& Sweller, 2006). In this section I will briefly outline those structures and processes.

Information-store principle. In order to function, we must store immeasurably large


amounts of information in long-term memory. The difference between people who are
more as opposed to less competent in any area including competence in a second
language is heavily determined by the amount of knowledge held in long-term memory
(Ericsson & Charness, 1994; Nandagopal & Ericsson, 2012).

Borrowing and reorganising principle. How do we acquire the huge amounts of


biologically secondary information that constitute substantive areas such as learning a
second language? Such knowledge is mainly obtained from other people by reading what
they write and listening to what they say. For example, anyone who deliberately studies
information produced by others, either written (e.g. in a textbook) or spoken, is using
this principle. Once knowledge is obtained, it is usually reorganised by combining it with
previously stored information. Based on this principle, learning a second language
requires

-2 TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,


ARTICLES

copious, explicit, written or spoken instruction.

Randomness-as-genesis principle. While most of the biologically secondary


information we process is obtained from other people, that information must first be
generated. Usually, the process of generation occurs during problem solving by a random
generation and test process. If we are unable to obtain information from others, we must
attempt to generate it. While there is no alternative to generate and test when we are
unable to obtain accurate information, this process tends to be inaccurate and clumsy.
One merely needs to observe a second language learner attempting to generate spoken or
written text from an inadequate knowledge base to realise the deficiencies of the process.
It needs to be emphasised that pure random generation rarely, if ever, occurs because we
rarely take any action in the complete absence of knowledge. The major point is that in
the absence of complete knowledge, action is determined by a combination of knowledge
and random generation. The more knowledge available, the less random generation is
needed.

Narrow limits of change principle. When dealing with novel information, in order
to avoid having to test an impossibly large number of possibilities thrown up by the
randomness-as-genesis principle, only a few elements of information can be dealt with
at a time. As a cognitive structure, working memory is extremely limited in capacity and
duration when dealing with novel information from the environment. Working memory
only can hold about seven items (Miller, 1956) and process about three to four items
(Cowan, 2001) of information simultaneously. Furthermore, it can only hold information
without rehearsal for about 15–20 seconds. Students learning a second language are
constantly dealing with novel information. A sentence that may be easily parsed in a
native language (see the next principle) may impose an impossibly high working memory
load in a second language. All instructional procedures need to account for the fact that
students are constantly under a high cognitive load.

Environmental organising and linking principle. While working-memory is


severely limited when processing novel information, it has no known limits when
processing familiar information transferred from long-term memory. Triggered by
environmental signals, appropriate information can be transferred from long-term to
working memory in order to allow us to generate action relevant to our environment. In
this way, information stored in long-term memory under the information-store principle
transforms us. We can carry out activities that otherwise would be beyond us. The more
information pertaining to a second language that is stored in long-term memory, the
better we are able to use that language.

Based on this cognitive architecture, the purpose of instruction is to facilitate the storage
of relevant information in long-term memory. Learning means storing information in
long- term memory. That process of storage needs to take into account the
characteristics of the human cognitive system and in particular, the limitations of
working memory that are directly relevant to instructional design issues.

-3 TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,


ARTICLES

Instructional Implications
Learning biologically secondary information, such as a second language, requires close
consideration of the above cognitive architecture. Instructors need to keep in mind that
novice adult learners first must process this category of information in a limited-capacity,
limited-duration working memory before transferring that information to an unlimited-
capacity, unlimited-duration long-term memory. There are general rules of instruction
that apply to all categories of biologically secondary information and some that apply
specifically to second language learning by adults.

One general rule is that instruction needs to be organised in a manner that reduces
unnecessary working memory load. It should be explicit in line with the borrowing and
reorganising principle. Learners should not be asked to induce relevant information by
using the randomness-as-genesis principle. In second language learning, this means
teachers should explicitly present the grammar and vocabulary of the second language
rather than expecting learners to induce the information themselves (see Kirschner et
al., 2006, for alternative formulations that emphasise implicit learning) as occurs when
dealing with a biologically primary task such as learning a native language as a child. We
are good at assimilating information from others. It is a biologically primary skill that
reduces cognitive load compared to inducing the information ourselves.

Another general rule is that the amount of biologically secondary information that is
provided at a given time should not exceed working memory limits. For example,
expecting adult learners to simultaneously learn a second language, particularly at the
beginning levels, while also acquiring information concerning other curriculum areas
such as science or history is likely to be counter-productive (for a counter view, see
European Union, n.d.). We can learn a native language at the same time as we learn other
things because we have evolved to do so. We have not evolved to learn a second language
in the same way. Learning a second language and learning other curriculum areas should
be kept separate whenever possible.

Instruction should be specifically devised to reduce working memory load. There are
many examples but three will be emphasised here. First, avoid split-attention which
occurs when multiple sources of information must be mentally integrated. For example,
when learners need to have vocabulary translations provided, as they frequently do,
provide translations close to the original, connecting them with arrows or, if using
electronic instruction, allowing the translation to appear by clicking on the relevant word.
Requiring learners to go to a separate dictionary imposes an additional cognitive load.
Learners should not be required to search for needed information.

Another recommendation is to avoid redundancy. Unnecessary information frequently is


processed with learners only finding after the event that they did not need to process the
additional information in order to learn. I discussed split-attention above by suggesting
that learners should not be required to split their attention between novel vocabulary and

-4 TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,


ARTICLES

its translation. Instead, the translation should be provided in a manner that eliminated
the need to search for it. If, however, the translation is not required because it is already
known, rather than physically integrating it with the original material, it should be
eliminated. Providing a translation is likely to increase unnecessary cognitive load, if only
slightly, due to redundancy.

The redundancy effect leads to the expertise-reversal effect. As indicated above,


information such as translations that are essential for novice learners should be
physically integrated but as expertise increases, rather than integrating the translation, it
should be eliminated entirely once it becomes redundant. In other words, an instructional
design that is suitable for novices, gradually loses its effectiveness with increasing
expertise and may become dysfunctional for more expert learners.

The expertise-reversal effect has implications for immersion in a second language


environment. While attempting to teach a second language by immersion is
counterproductive for novice adult learners, with increasing expertise, immersion is likely
to become increasingly effective. Once they need the practice associated with immersion,
it should be introduced and is likely to be effective. The fact that immersion can be
effective for more experienced second language learners, not to mention native language
learners, is no excuse to introduce it for adult beginners.

These instructional recommendations flow directly from cognitive load theory. They
contradict many current instructional practices that routinely ignore most of what we
know of evolutionary educational psychology and human cognitive architecture. It needs
to be emphasised that instructional recommendations based on cognitive load theory
have been extensively tested using randomised, controlled trials and have been
demonstrated to be effective. Details, along with many other instructional effects may be
found in Sweller et al. (2011).

References
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental
storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114.
Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance; its structure and acquisition. American
Psychologist, 49, 725–747.
European Union. (n.d.). Action plan on language learning and linguistic diversity. Retrieved
February 6, 2017, from http://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?uri=uriserv:c11068
Geary, D. (2012). Evolutionary Educational Psychology. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan
(Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 597–621). Washington, D.C.:
American Psychological Association.
Geary, D. & Berch, D. (2016). Evolution and children’s cognitive and academic development. In D.
Geary & D. Berch (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on child development and education (pp.
217–249). Switzerland: Springer.

-5 TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,


ARTICLES

Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not
work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential
and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for
processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Nandagopal, K. & Ericsson, K. A. (2012). Enhancing students’ performance in traditional
education: Implications for the expert-performance approach and deliberate practice. In K.
Harris, S. Graham, & T.Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology hanbdbook (Vol. 1, pp.
257–293).
Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
Sweller, J. (2015). In academe, what is learned and how is it learned? Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 24, 190–194.
Sweller, J. (2016). Working memory, long-term memory and instructional design. Journal of
Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 360–367.
Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.
Sweller, J., Kirschner, P., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not
work: A reply to commentaries. Educational Psychologist, 42, 115–121.
Sweller, J. & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary
Psychology, 4, 434–458.

Author Bio
Professor John Sweller, Ph.D. is an Australian educational psychologist who is best known for formulating an influential theory of cogn

-6 TESL Ontario | CONTACT Magazine | August,

You might also like