A Case Study On Sibling Rivalry and The Use of A Social Skills Training Model
A Case Study On Sibling Rivalry and The Use of A Social Skills Training Model
5-7-2010
Recommended Citation
Maiorano, Michelle, "A case study on sibling rivalry and the use of a social skills training model" (2010). Theses and Dissertations. 125.
http://rdw.rowan.edu/etd/125
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Rowan Digital Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an
authorized administrator of Rowan Digital Works. For more information, please contact [email protected].
A CASE STUDY ON SIBLING RIVALRY AND THE USE OF A SOCIAL SKILLS
TRAINING MODEL
by
Michelle Maiorano
A Thesis
Michelle Maiorano
A CASE STUDY ON SIBLING RIVARLY AND THE USE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
TRAINING MODEL
2009/10
John Klanderman, Ph.D. and Roberta Dihoff, Ph.D.
Master of Arts in School Psychology
The purpose of this study was to use social skills training on an individual child to
enhance the relationship with her siblings. The social skills sessions focused on factors
such warmth, rivalry, conflict and status/power between the two siblings. These were
measured by the Sibling Rivalry Questionnaire which the mother filled out before the
sessions began and after they were completed. The results did not find a major difference
between the pre and post tests. Warmth increased slightly, rivalry decreased slightly,
there was no difference in the amount of conflict, and there was a slight decrease in
relative status/power from pre to post test. These factors did not seem to be affected by
the social skills sessions and therefore did not improve the sibling relationship.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
List of Figures
CHAPTER
I. Need
Purpose
Hypotheses
History
Definitions
Assumptions
Limitations
Summary
II. Causes of Sibling Conflict
III. Participants
Design
Procedure
Measures
Analysis
Summary
IV. Findings
V. Introduction 24
Interpretation of Findings 24
Limitations 25
Conclusions 25
Future Study 26
References 27
LIST OF FIGURES
FIGURE PAGE
Need
because people are surrounded by members of their family from the day they are born
and many years following their birth. The dynamics of a family are difficult to
understand because every family is different in how they relate to one another. Also,
family relations have an influence of later ones in life and one of the most crucial
relationships to look at are the ones between siblings. A sibling relationship can
ultimately benefit or destroy the family as a whole so it is crucial find ways to facilitate
Purpose
The purpose of the study was to use social skills training on an individual child to
enhance the relationship with her siblings. Past research on aiding the sibling relationship
has mainly focused on the child rearing practices of the parent, but less research has
looked upon teaching the child new social skills. By increasing the positive interactions
between siblings, it could ultimately benefit children's relationships in the future and
Hypotheses
The child who completes the social skills training will have increased warmth,
decreased sibling rivalry, less sibling conflict, and reduced status/power differential
between siblings.
History
The foundation of this paper was based upon past research that used social skills
Among Young Children: A Social Skills Training Model.' This study looked at new
approach to improving sibling relationships by using social skills training model. The
researchers hypothesized that children who received social skills training would have
more positive interactions with their siblings in comparison to children in the control
group. Twenty-one families were randomly assigned to the experimental group and
twenty-one were assigned to the control group. A videotape was used to record the
interactions of the sibling before experiment took place. During four sessions the
experimental group was taught 6 social skills: how to initiate play appropriately, how to
accept play, how to refuse play, perspective taking, how to deal with angry feelings, and
how to manage conflict. A model by McGinnis and Goldstein (1990) was use to teach
these skills. Post measure tests were used after the treatment, such as videotaping the
sibling behavior and questionnaires distributed to parents which assessed the sibling
was administered to fathers and mothers one week before and after the training sessions.
The results indicated suggested that the social skills training may be associated with the
competitive and agonistic behavior, lower levels of problematic behaviors and lower
status/power differential. This research laid the framework for the present study except
that this study will use the social skill training for an intervention on one child rather than
a group.
Definitions
and sisters.
Assumptions
It was assumed in the present study that the parents were honest in their
assessments of the child's behavior so that the training could be accurately tested. Also it
was assumed that the parents were not biased toward any individual sibling.
Limitations:
There are several limitations to this study. One limitation is that since this is a
case study, it lacked external validity and therefore was difficult to generalize through the
population. Also, the social skills program only consisted of 40 minute long sessions one
a week, and it may be more beneficial to include more training sessions. Another
limitation was that there were a limited amount of social skills evaluated.
Summary
The need for the present study is to build a more positive relationship between
siblings at a young age in order to foster healthy relationships in the future. Past research
has focused very much on what parents can do to reduce the conflict between siblings but
not many studies have looked at the use of social skills. The foundation of this study was
based upon an experimental study by Kramer and Radey (1997) entitled "Improving
Sibling Relationships Among Young Children: A Social Skills Training Mode." The
affect of the social skills was rated using the Sibling Rivalry Questionnaire which the
mother filled out. It was assumed for this study that the mother was honest in her
assessment of the child's behavior. Some limitations of the study included lack of
external validity, limited amount of social skills evaluated, and lack of time when
Family dynamics has been studied for years because of the importance it has over
the individual. One of the most influential relationships for a person is the one with his or
her sibling. The sibling relationship is one of the longest lasting relationships in most
people's lives. In childhood, siblings often spend more time together than they do with
their parents (Crouter, McHale 1989). This may be the reason for why arguments
between siblings were the most common type of conflict found in families (Straus,
Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). Research consists of many possible factors that contribute to
sibling rivalry as well as the effects later on in life. Resolving sibling conflict have been
designed primarily around teaching parents the skills to intervene but little research has
focused on teaching children the skills to form a more healthy relationship. Therefore,
this section is separated into 5 main topics; the causes of sibling rivalry the effects of
sibling rivalry, parental responses, social skills training in general, and social skills
Research on sibling rivalry is quite diverse. For example Moser, Jones, Zaorski,
and Mirsalimi (2005) studied jealousy, envy, guilt, and resentment between siblings and
found that rivalry stems chiefly from competition over attention from the caretaker. Once
there was another child added to a family, the mother's love had to be shared between
multiple offspring which caused conflict. The only child becomes the older child and
5
moves from the central position of interest to one which requires sharing (Neubauer,
1983). Some view the competitive nature of siblings to be a negative outcome of sibling
rivalry. However, sibling rivalry could have positive consequences because children often
Parental attention and affection was not the only reason for sibling conflict. The
most common issue for sibling was sharing personal possessions for both older and
younger siblings (McGuire, Manke, Eftekhari, & Dunn, 2000). Parent behaviors were
rarely cited as sources of conflict. This could suggest that sibling rivalry is not primarily
influenced by parental love but that siblings have their own issues that are separate from
Some other factors that might be expected to affect sibling conflict are age,
gender, and biological kinship. For example, Erel, Margolin, and John (1998) studied 73
pairs of siblings between 3 and 6 years of age to assess the importance of age differences
in sibling conflict. The siblings were videotaped in a laboratory setting while the mother
filled out questionnaires concerning her marital relationship and their relationship with
her children. The results indicated that there was more negative behavior from older
siblings directed at younger siblings than reverse and negative sibling interactions were
linked with negative family interactions. Despite the relatively small age range, the
greater the space between siblings, the greater the warmth the young sibling gave to the
older siblings.
Sex differences also play an important role in sibling relations. Martin and Ross
(2005) hypothesized that male siblings would engage in more physical aggression than
female siblings. The results found that brothers engaged in more physical and verbal
aggression than girls but the most severe forms of aggression were only found when the
children were 2 /2 years of age. However, some forms of physical and verbal aggression
continued to be used by boys as they got older. It was also reported that older siblings,
while younger siblings aggressed more often when conflict was with older brothers.
One might infer that kinship should play a role in the relationships between
siblings. Jankowiak and Diderich (2000) tested the hypothesis that there would be a
closer bond between biological siblings than half siblings. They examined factors such as
family cohesion, solidarity, and sibling rivalry. They hypothesized that sibling closeness
would vary by gender, age and personality but not genetic relatedness. The participants
numerous and the data was collected over seven years. The results revealed a difference
in emotional attachment and affection between full and half siblings. There was greater
Sibling conflict is also related to family functioning and well being in general.
Stewart (1990) focused on how the family system can affect individuals in the family and
also the family subsystems, such as the sibling relationship. All families who welcome a
new child face stressors that change the organization of the family in order to
accommodate the new relationship. Even the mere presence of a parent in the room can
have an effect on sibling conflict. Corter, Abramovitch, and Pepler (1983) found that
conflict was higher when the mother was present than when children were alone.
Children also interacted less when their parents were in the room yet had higher rates of
7
conflict. Brody, Stoneman, McCoy, and Forehand (1992) focused on the effects of family
functioning and hypothesized that sibling conflict would be associated with specific
parenting styles, the unequal treatment of siblings and the amount of conflict within the
family. Researchers interviewed 152 children and their parents about the problems they
encountered in the family, how problems were typically resolved and sibling interactions.
The results indicated that when siblings were treated equally during family discussions
Pike, Coldwell, and Dunn (2005) also examined how the parents' relationship
with their children affected the siblings' relationship. It was hypothesized that if parent-
child relationships were positive, then sibling relationships would be warmer with less
conflict. Also, it was predicted that positive sibling relationships would result in higher
levels of prosocial behavior. The results found showed that the quality of the sibling
relationship was linked to the older child's family adjustment but not the younger
siblings' adjustment.
The relationship between parents and siblings was also studied by Feinberg,
McHale, Crouter and Cumsille (2003). They predicted that if siblings were treated
differently by their parents than it would result in more positivity in the sibling
relationship over time. The theory behind this prediction was that individual
differentiation between siblings results in less competition and therefore less rivalry.
Participants were families studied over a two year period. The siblings were first and
second born adolescents. The parents and the siblings were interviewed at separate times
and conflict with parents was measured as well as parents' warmth, conflict in the sibling
relationship, and warmth in the sibling relationship. Siblings who had discrete,
8
individualistic and different relationships with their parents had less sibling rivalry and
more sibling warmth. Siblings with little or no difference in their relationships with their
parents showed lower levels of warmth and higher levels of conflict with one another.
But evidence has also supported the opposite between differentiation and conflict.
Stocker, Dunn and Plomin (1989) found that maternal differential behavior was
associated with more conflict between siblings. There tended to be more competition and
less friendliness among the siblings when the mother's control, affection, attention and
The affect of families does not only have an impact on the individual but plays a
role in different elements across the family relationships. Maternal negativity was found
to be a factor that predicted sibling conflict (Jenkins, O'Connor, Dunn, Rasbash, &
Behnke 2005). A mother's negative emotions towards a child did not influence that
particular relationship, but caused that child to have a negative relationship with
their siblings.
number of theories surrounding it. Modeling and social learning theory suggest that
children model the behavior with their siblings that they have learned in their
relationships with their parents. Also, the attachment theory suggests that children form
schemas based on their relationships with their parents that they later on shape the
relationships, martial relations between the parents can also cause sibling conflict.
Stocker and Youngblade (1999) focused on the relationship between marital conflict and
children's conflict with one another. It was found that marital conflict resulted in less
warmth towards the children and more sibling rivalry. Martial conflict also resulted in
greater hostility by the children towards both parents. This could be because children
model the behavior that they observed between their parents or it could be that parents
are so preoccupied with the conflict in their marriage that they are unavailable to manage
In 1932 Piaget suggested that children's social and moral understanding was
influenced by their interactions with other children. This theory has also been continued
sociocognitive abilities earlier through their relationships with siblings. They were
capable of interpreting feelings and intentions of their younger siblings (Dunn, 1988).
Another investigation found that children's affection and cooperation towards their
siblings when they were three years of age predicted their ability to take the perspective
of another seven months later (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1995).
Futhermore, Teti, Bond, and Gibbs (1986) founds that preschool aged children with
siblings more than three years younger than them were more likely to create intellectually
and socially stimulating environments for their younger sibling and further their
Research has also focused on the course of sibling rivalry over time. Stocker,
Burwell, and Briggs (2002) looked at sibling conflict in middle childhood and its
relationship to psychological adjustment in early adolescence. The authors proposed that
conflict between children at the age of 10 would lead to increased depression, anxiety,
and bad behavior two years later. It was also expected that sibling conflict earlier in
childhood would lead to hostility with the mother and adjustment problems later. The
results supported the predictions, suggesting that sibling rivalry either resulted in poor
family adjustment over time, or that anxious, depressed ten year olds were more likely to
Sibling relationships do not end in childhood. They are a constant factor in the
lives of adults as well. In early adulthood, it was found that sibling relationships have
three different components; warmth, conflict and rivalry (Stocker, Lanthier, & Furman,
1997). Also, research found that young adults who rated their sibling relationships as
close had higher scores on measures of emotions and cognitive empathy than those who
had distant relationships with siblings (Shortt & Gottman, 1997). Siblings tend to drift
apart during this time and communicate with each other through their parents until
older adulthood where they renew their relationship and become closer once again
(Cicirelli, 1980).
relationships and individual well-being. Children who had negative sibling relationships
were found to have high levels of anxiety, depression, poor self-esteem and school
adjustment (Dunn, Slomkowski, Beardsall, & Rende, 1994). Also conflict within the
adjustment, whereas supportive relationships were linked with decreased anxiety and
greater maturity (Stocker, Lanthier, & Furman, 1997). So, forming a healthy positive
11
sibling relationship early in childhood may help to form a beneficial relationship in later
From past research it is obvious that parents play an important role in the causes
influential predictor of future sibling behavior. When the time comes to resolve a sibling
Even before a second child is welcomed into the family, parents are given advice on how
to prepare their firstborn for the arrival of new sibling. Preparation is for a new sibling
was considered to be essential for enhancing the child's adjustment and to avoid negative
reactions towards the sibling that include direct aggression toward the baby, attention
seeking behavior, and regression (Legg, Sherick, & Wadland, 1974). Therefore authors
have agreed that children should be told about the new siblings' arrival well before the
due date because it was found that children responded more negatively when they were
unaware of the new addition to the family. Previous investigations also suggest that
parents can reduce feelings of resentment by reassuring the child that he or she is still
holds a special role in the family, spending time alone with older child, deemphasizing
the significance of the new baby, upholding children's individual rights, and pointing out
similarities between the siblings (Kramer & Ramsburg, 2002). Early experiences with
caring for the new sibling was suggested to give the older child the opportunity to
become more aware of the needs and concerns of others and contribute to perspective
unsure of whether and how to intervene. There has been some research that suggested
parents should not interfere with their children's fights. It was found that children fight to
gain parental attention and if they are left on their own they will settle their conflict
equitably. Interference only worsens the conflict because the children achieve their aim
of gaining attention from their parents and so the behavior continued. The parents'
judgment also leaves one of the children feeling like the loser and so he seeks out revenge
on the other sibling (Levi, Buskila, & Gerzi, 1977). Also, interference from parents
prevented children from working out their own problems and prevented them from
obtaining conflict resolution skills (Brody & Stoneman, 1987). A balance of power can
form if the children are left to work through their own issues.
Other theories have supported the opposing view, which is that a parent can be a
constructive role in their children's arguments. Ihinger (1975) found that when parents
are consistent and logical in the way they enforce rules, the children's conflict will
decrease. The children learn the family values about fairness and equality and therefore
relationships. Kramer, Perozynski, and Chung (1999) used a microphone system in the
homes of 88 intact families with two children to identify sibling conflict and parental
management strategies. When parents did not intervene there was a higher occurrence of
subsequent sibling conflict. Younger siblings reacted more strongly to this laissez- faire
technique, whereas conflict among older siblings was greatest when their mothers
in conflicts with their two and four year olds. Their findings indicated that parents
children's disputes, addressed the children's issues, held position on these issues, and
enforced certain rules. Overall, parents were unbiased in supporting either the younger or
older children. When parents did not intervene, rules were often violated and older
Brody, Stoneman, McCoy and Forehand (1992) also found that intervening can be
beneficial to the children especially when discussions are open about the disputes and
focus on problem solving strategies. When the children were in an atmosphere of where
the children's views were considered equally and the parents used moderate levels of
control, it was associated with less conflicted sibling relationships. But, in other research,
it was found that parents beliefs on intervention strategies were not consistent with their
behavior. Perozynski and Kramer (1999) found that parents believed the most effective
way to intervene in their children's disputes was through child centered strategies, for
example, working with the children to resolve the conflict. But they found that the
strategy the parents used most often was passive nonintervention. This was the strategy
they had thought to be least effective and had the least confidence in but yet they used it
most often.
Research has proposed a few strategies for mothers to use to promote prosocial
sibling interactions between preschool aged children and infant siblings (Kramer and
Washo, 1990). The behaviors included high lighting the activities and abilities of one
child to the other, encouraging the older child to help perform care giving tasks for the
14
infant, including the children in activities together, praising positive interactions. It was
found that by mothers using these behaviors there were higher levels of prosoical
behavior but siblings were not able to sustain the positive interaction when the mothers
were absent.
concentrated on parental intervention, there is still not a clear answer as to whether or not
a parent should interfere with their children's disputes. But previous studies have
suggested that it is possible for children to solve their own disputes without the help of a
parent. Ross, Ross, Stein and Trabasso (2006) asked 64 sibling dyads to reexamine an
ongoing conflict and attempt to resolve it through discussion. It was found that almost
two thirds of the dyads were able to resolve the conflict. This suggests that once children
were given time to calm down after the initial argument and were given the chance to
resolve the differences later, many did so. It appeared that older siblings took the lead in
proposing, promoting and gaining agreement to their plans for resolving the conflict
whereas the younger siblings' roles was to make suggestions or modifications to the plan.
Howe and Ross (1990) also found that first borns' references to second-borns' about
feelings and skills were positively linked with perspective taking and friendly sibling
relations across all settings. So, it appears that children do have the capacity to solve their
disputes without the help of a parent and the role of the eldest child is key in the problem
solving process. Conflict resolution within the sibling relationship and its link to other
relationships was examined and found that children's conflict with their friends at 6 years
of age was related to the conflict strategies of their siblings three years earlier (Dunn &
Herrera, 1997).
If siblings are capable of solving their own problems, without the intervention of
parents, then social skills training could play a very beneficial role in their relationship.
The strategies they learn through the training could last for future disputes, whereas the
intervention from a parent may only work when the parent is present. Some previous
studies have looked at how relationships can benefit from the teaching social skills to
better manage that conflict. Oden and Asher (1977) investigated whether social skills
training would benefit children in friendships. Third and fourth grade children were
coached in social skills and the results indicated that the coaching procedure was
effective in children peer acceptance. These children had even greater inclusion by peers
one year later. Mize and Ladd (1990) also focused on peer acceptance and social skills
training. Children were taught 4 skills: leading peers, asking questions to peers, making
comments to peers and supporting peers. The researchers found that increases in skills
strategies. The link between peer relations and sibling relations is apparent in other
studies. Past research found that social cognitive skills were related to successful
communication with both siblings and friends (Cutting & Dunn, 2006). Also, peer
interactions with younger siblings (Kramer & Gottman, 1992). Not only can social skills
training have a positive influence on a sibling relationship but it could also help in future
The foundation of the present study was based upon past research that used social
Relationships Among Young Children: A Social Skills Training Model.' This study
looked at new approach to improving sibling relationships by using social skills training
model. The researchers hypothesized that children who received social skills training
would have more positive interactions with their siblings in comparison to children in the
control group. A videotape was used to record the interactions of the sibling before
experiment took place. During four sessions the experimental group was taught 6 social
skills: how to initiate play appropriately, how to accept play, how to refuse play,
perspective taking, how to deal with angry feelings, and how to manage conflict. A
model by McGinnis and Goldstein (1990) was use to teach these skills. Post measure
tests were used after the treatment, such as videotaping the sibling behavior and
questionnaires distributed to parents which assessed the sibling relationship. The Sibling
and mothers one week before and after the training sessions. The results indicated
suggested that the social skills training may be associated with the parents' reports of
increased warmth, decreased sibling rivalry, stable levels of competitive and agonistic
behavior, lower levels of problematic behaviors and lower status/power differential. This
research laid the framework for the present study except that this study will use the social
skill training for an intervention on one four year old child rather than a group.
Chapter III
Sibling rivalry and the use of social skills training was the topic of interest and not
much past research has looked at the two together. The present study greatly replicates
that by Kramer and Radey (1997) but differs in that this is a case study. Throughout
chapter three the methodology of the current study will be discussed including the
Participants
The participant in this study was a four year old female child. She was from a
white middle class family that consisted of a father, mother, two year old female sibling,
Design
The independent variable in this study was the sibling relationship, which was
measured using the Sibling Relationship Questionnaire (Furman and Buhrmester, 1985)
competition and quarrelling. The dependent variable was the social skills training.
Procedure
The researcher met with the parent one week before the social skills training
sessions began and administered the SRQ to the mother. The child was then visited in the
home for four weekly 40-minute sessions on social skills training. During these four
sessions, 6 relevant social skills were taught. These consisted of (1) how to initiate play
appropriately, (2) how to accept play, (3) how to refuse invitations to play, (4)
perspective taking, (5) how to deal with angry feelings and (6) manage conflict. These
skills were taught following the model by McGinnis and Goldstein (1990). For example,
during the first session on initiating play the researcher explained how to appropriately
start play and then used dolls to model the behavior. The child was then asked to use the
dolls to model behavior. Different scenarios were used to demonstrate the skill and the
instructor provided feedback for the child. Each of the follow sessions progressed in the
same manner as this except teaching different skills. The mother was given a handout at
the end of each session explaining the purpose of that session and specific suggestion for
the mother to use on the child when performing that specific skill. At the end of the four
weeks the researcher administered the SRQ to the mother to see if the child's progress in
Measures
The Sibling Rivalry Questionnaire (SRQ) from Furman and Buhrmester (1985)
was administered to the mother one week before and one week after the training sessions.
She rated her child's behavior on 48 items using a 5-point Likert scale that assessed the
how typical each behavior is of her child's interactions with the siblings (1 = hardly at all,
5 = extremely much). The responses on the SRQ are separated into four categories;
Warmth and Closeness, Rivalry, Conflict, and Relative Status/Power. Example items
from these scales include: "How much do both and this sibling do nice things for
each other?" (Warmth and Closeness), 'Who usually gets better by mother, or
19
this sibling?' (Rivalry), "How much do and this sibling disagree and quarrel with
each other?" (Conflict), and " How much does show this sibling how to do things
Analysis
The hypotheses for this study were the child that completed the social skills
training will have increased warmth, decreased sibling rivalry, stable levels of
siblings. The researcher compared the results from the first SRQ to the last SRQ to see if
there were any improvements by running a correlation analysis and graphing the results.
Summary
of this study. This is a case study of a four year old girl examining the use of social skills
training on her sibling relationship. The social skills lessons lasted four weeks (40 minute
sessions) and results were determined by the Sibling Relationship Questionnaire that was
Findings
The pre and post tests using the Sibling Rivalry Questionnaire were compared
using a correlation analysis. The hypotheses for this study were the child that completed
the social skills training will have increased warmth, decreased sibling rivalry, stable
The mean score for warmth was measured by using the means from the scale
admiration of sibling, and affection. The pre test mean was 8.6 and the post test mean
was 10 (Figure 4.1). There was a slight increase in the amount of warmth after the social
skills training.
10
9 ................ .Pre Test
7 mmE Post Test
The rivalry score consisted of the average scale scores of maternal and paternal
partiality. The pre test score was 7.5 and the post test score was 7 (Figure 4.2). There was
21
a minimal decrease in the amount of rivalry that existed between the siblings after the
7.6
7.4
7.2 Pre Test
. -. Post Test
6.8
6.6
Pre Test Post Test
The conflict score consisted of the average of the quarrelling, antagonism and
competition. The pre test score was 9.7 and the post test score was 9.7 (Figure 4.3). There
was no difference in the amount of conflict before and after the social skills training.
5 .
Pre Test
5
Post Test
sibling, minus the scale scores of nurturance by sibling and dominance of sibling. The pre
test score was 12 and the post test score was 11 (Figure 4.4). There was a slight decrease
22
Figure 4.4 Status/Power
10.5
Pre Test Post Test
23
Chapter V
Introduction
The purpose of the study was to use social skills training on an individual child to
enhance the relationship with her siblings. Past research on aiding the sibling relationship
has mainly focused on the child rearing practices of the parent or social skills training in
different areas, but less research has looked upon teaching the child new social skills. The
issues specifically addressed in this study were warmth between the siblings, rivalry,
conflict, and relative status/power. The results found that social skills training did not
have much impact on the child's relationship with her sibling. There was a slight increase
in the amount of warmth after the training, minimal decrease in the amount of rivalry, no
difference in the amount of conflict, and slight decrease in the relative status/power after
Interpretation of Findings
The present study was not consistent with past research on social skills training
and sibling rivalry. Kramer and Radey (1997) found that by using social skills with the
children there was reports of increased warmth, decreased sibling rivalry, stable levels of
competitive and agonistic behavior, lower levels of problematic behaviors and lower
status/power differential. This present study found that the social skills training did not
produce the same positive effects on the sibling relationship. Other previous research on
social skills training found that children had the capacity to solve their disputes without
the help of a parent and the role of the eldest child is key in the problem solving process
(Howe & Ross, 1990). But this was also inconsistent with the findings in the present
study which found that teaching the oldest child social skills did not improve problem
Limitations
There were some limitations to the present study. Using one child as a case study
hindered the process because a small sample size was not enough to generalize to the
population. Also having more participants, which different sibling relationships, would
have made the study more reliable. Another limitation was the small time span used for
the social skills training. The current study took place for four weeks with one 40 minute
training session each week. This may not have been enough time for the child to
completely absorb the social skills being taught. More time each week could have
reinforced the material taught to the child. The mother's lack of participation in the social
skills training also affected the study. If the mother was more willing to support the social
skills and reinforce the material being taught, even when the researcher was not present,
Conclusions
Family relationships play an important role to an individual's life and one aspect
is the sibling interaction. Future relationships with peers and partners are often affected
improve upon sibling relations at an early age. The present study gained background
knowledge from previous studies which explained that social skills training can have a
positive influence on siblings. Even though the present study did not yield the same
results it did have strengths. The strengths of this study consisted of valid measurements,
a cooperative participant, and planned training sessions based on previous research. The
weaknesses were a small sample size that did not allow for generalization, the time span
which was too short, and the mother's lack of commitment to the study. These findings
do not represent the entire population, but from the results it was suggested that social
skills training, by itself, does not produce positive sibling relationships. The relationship
between the participant and younger sibling did not change much from pre to post test so
future research must be done in order to find the most beneficial use of social skills
Future Study
Future studies in this area could build upon the present study by having more
participants involved and create an experiment. This could create the opportunity to
generalize throughout the population. Also, studies should increase the amount of social
skills sessions taught to the child so that the child can have more time to practice these
skills. The role of the parent should also be used more often in future research. The
mother or father should be aware of the skills taught to their child and be consistently
reinforcing these skills. By making these changes to the present study, social skills
training may have the opportunity to create a more positive environment for siblings and
Brody, G. H., & Stoneman, Z. (1987). Sibling conflict: Contributions of the siblings
Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., McCoy, J. K, & Forehand, R. (1992). Contemporaneous and
and family discussions about sibling problems. Child Development, 63, 391-400.
Corter, C., Abramovitch, R., & Pepler, D. (1983). The role of the mother in sibling
Crouter, A., & McHale, S. (1989). Childbearing in dual and single earner families:
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (2006). Converstaions with siblings and with friends: Links
University Press.
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1995). Young
Dunn, J., & Herrera, C. (1997). Conflict resolution with friends, siblings, and mothers: A
Dunn, J., Slomkowski, C., Beardsall, L., & Rende, R. (1994). Adjustment in middle
childhood and early adolescence; Links with earler and contemporary sibling
Erel, O., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1998). Observed sibling interaction: Links with
288-298.
Feinberg, M. E., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Cumsille, P. (2003) Sibling
Ihinger, M. (1975). The referee role and norms of equity: A contribution toward a theory
125-139.
Jenkins, J., O'Connor, T., Dunn, J., Rasbash, J., & Behnke, P. (2005). Change in
Kramer, L., & Gottman, J. M. (1992). Becoming a sibling: "With a little help from my
Kramer, L., Perozynski, L. A., & Chung, T. (1999). Parental responses to sibling conflict:
1401-1414.
Kramer, L, & Radey, C. (1997). Improving sibling relationships among young children:
Kramer, L., & Ramsburg, D. (2002). Advice given to parents on welcoming a second
Kramer, L., & Washo, C.A. (1990, July). Parentalfacilitationand gatekeeping of sibling
Legg, C., Sherick, I., Wadland, W. (1974). Reaction of preschool children to the birth of
Levi, A. M., Buskila, M., & Gerzi, S. (1977) Benign neglect: Reducing fights among
Martin, J. L, & Ross, H. S. (2005). Sibling aggression: Sex differences and parents'
Development, 9, 173-190.
Mize, J., & Ladd, G. W. (1990). A cognitive-social learning approach to social skill
388-397.
Moser, C. J., Jones, R. A., Zaorski, D. M., & Mirsalimi, H. (2005). The impact of the
Oden, S., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Coaching children in social skills for friendship making.
Perozynski, L., & Kramer, L. (1999). Parental beliefs about managing sibling conflict.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moraljudgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Pike, A., Coldwell, J., & Dunn, J. F. (2005). Sibling relationships in early/middle
523-532.
Ross, H. S., Filyer, R. E., Lollis, S. P., Perlman, M., & Martin, J. L. (1994).
Ross, H., Ross, M., Stein, N., & Trabasso, T. (2006). How siblings resolve their conflicts:
Shortt, J. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Closeness in young adult sibling relationships:
Stocker, C. M., Burwell, R. A., & Briggs, M. L. (2002). Sibling conflict in middle
Stocker, C. M., Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1989). Sibling relationships: Links with child
715-727.
with children's sibling and peer relationships. Journalof Family Psychology, 13,
598-609.
Straus, M., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors. New York:
Doubleday.