Unit 3 - Learning and Memory
Unit 3 - Learning and Memory
� Perceptual learning
� Relational learning
Types of Memory
Lashley’s Search for the engram
� Karl Lashley
� “engram”—permanent change in the brain accounting for the
existence of memory; a memory trace - the physical representation of
what has been learned - A connection between two brain areas would
be a possible example of an engram.
� if learning depends on new or strengthened connections between two
brain areas, a knife cut somewhere in the brain should interrupt that
connection and abolish the learned response.
� He trained rats on mazes and a brightness discrimination task and
then made deep cuts in varying locations in their cerebral cortices - no
knife cut significantly impaired the rats’ performances - the types of
learning that he studied did not depend on connections across the
cortex.
� Lashley also tested whether any portion of the cerebral cortex is
more important than others for learning - He trained rats on mazes
before or after he removed large portions of the cortex - The lesions
impaired performance, but the deficit depended more on the amount
of brain damage than on its location - Learning and memory
apparently did not rely on a single cortical area.
� therefore proposed two principles about the nervous system:
� equipotentiality—all partsof the cortex contribute equally to
complex behaviors such as learning, and any part of the cortex can
substitute for any other.
� mass action—the cortex works as a whole, and more cortex is
better
� Richard F. Thompson and his colleagues used a simpler task
seeking the location of engram.
� sought the engram of memory not in the cerebral cortex but in the
cerebellum.
� studied classical conditioning of eyelid responses in rabbits.
They presented first a tone (CS) and then a puff of air (UCS) to
the cornea of the rabbit’s eye. At first, a rabbit blinked at the air
puff but not at the tone. After repeated pairings, classical
conditioning occurred and the rabbit blinked at the tone also.
� Investigators recorded the activity in various brain cells to
determine which ones changed their responses during learning.
� Thompson - determined the location of learning.
� sequence of brain areas from the sensory receptors to the motor neurons
controlling the muscles – a damage any one of those areas, learning will
be impaired, but we cannot be sure that learning occurred in the damaged
area.
� For example, if the learning occurs in area D, damage in A, B, or C will
prevent learning by blocking the input to D. Damage in E or F will
prevent learning by blocking the output from D.
� Thompson and colleagues reasoned as follows: Suppose the learning
occurs in D. If so, then D has to be active at the time of the learning, and
so do all the areas leading up to D (A, B, and C). However, learning
should not require areas E and beyond. If area E were temporarily
blocked, nothing would relay information to the muscles, so we would see
no response, but learning could occur nevertheless, and we could see
evidence of it later
� Thompson’s research identified one nucleus of the cerebellum - the lateral
interpositus nucleus (LIP) - essential for learning.
� At the start of training, those cells showed little response to the tone, but as
learning proceeded, their responses increased
� When temporarily suppressed that nucleus in an untrained rabbit, either by
cooling the nucleus or by injecting a drug into it, and then presented the
CSs and UCSs, the rabbit showed no responses during the training. Then
they waited for the LIP to recover and continued training. At that point, the
rabbit began to learn, but it learned at the same speed as animals that had
received no previous training. Evidently, while the LIP was suppressed, the
training had no effect
� The next question was - does learning actually occur in the LIP, or does this
area just relay the information to a later area where learning occurs?
� To understand - investigators suppressed activity in the red nucleus, a
midbrain motor area that receives input from the cerebellum.
� When the red nucleus was suppressed, the rabbits again showed no
responses during training. However, as soon as the red nucleus had
recovered from the cooling or drugs, the rabbits showed strong learned
responses to the tone
� In other words, suppressing the red nucleus temporarily prevented the
response but did not prevent learning.
� Evidently, learning did not require activity in the red nucleus or any area
after it, although later research found that the red nucleus does contribute to
learning under certain circumstances
� Thompson and his colleagues concluded – “the learning occurred in the
LIP”.
Lashley and � The mechanisms for this type of conditioning are probably
Thompson in similar in humans.
Human � According to PET scans on young adults - developing a
learning conditioned eyeblink causes increases in the cerebellum, red
nucleus, and several other areas
� People who have damage in the cerebellum have weaker
conditioned eyeblinks, and the blinks are less accurately timed
relative to the onset of the air puff
� The cerebellum is critical for many other instances of classical
conditioning also, but only if the delay between the onset of the
CS and the onset of the UCS is short.
� the cerebellum is specialized for timing brief intervals, on the
order of a couple of seconds or less.
� Many instances of learning take place in other brain areas. For
example, learning to avoid a taste because of subsequent illness
depends on the amygdala
Classical Conditioning
ROLE OF AMYGDALA
• The amygdala is important in classically conditioned emotional responses
• Eg:- classically conditioned emotional response is established by pairing a neutral
stimulus (such as a tone) with an aversive stimulus (such as a brief foot shock) - after
these stimuli are paired, the tone becomes a CS; when it is presented by itself, it elicits the
same type of responses as the unconditioned stimulus does.
• After being processed by the auditory cortex, information about the CS (the tone) reaches
the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. This nucleus also receives information about the US
(the foot shock) from the somatosensory system. Thus, these two sources of information
converge in the lateral nucleus, which means that synaptic changes responsible for
learning could take place in this location
Auditory cortex – lateral nucleus of Amygdala + somatosensory system = converge in
lateral nucleus – changes in synaptic
� The lateral nucleus of the amygdala contains neurons whose axons project
to the central nucleus.
� Terminal buttons from neurons that transmit auditory and somatosensory
information to the lateral nucleus form synapses with dendritic spines on
these neurons - When a rodent encounters a painful stimulus, somatosensory
input activates strong synapses in the lateral nucleus.
� As a result, the neurons in this nucleus begin firing, which activates neurons
in the central nucleus, evoking an unlearned (unconditioned) emotional
response. If a tone is paired with the painful stimulus, the weak synapses in
the lateral amygdala are strengthened. The synaptic changes responsible for
this type of learning take place within this circuit.
Study on
Aplysia
ROLE OF GLUTAMATE
� the lateral amygdala responsible for acquisition of a conditioned
emotional response involve a series of synaptic changes called long-term
potentiation (LTP). LTP involves NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate) receptors
and AMPA (amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid) glutamate
receptors.
� LTP is accomplished through the activation of NMDA receptors and the
insertion of additional AMPA receptors into the postsynaptic membrane.
These synaptic changes in the glutamate system serve to increase the
Excitatory Post Synaptic Potential (EPSP) to the postsynaptic cell.
� LTP among glutamate synapses in the lateral amygdala plays a critical
role in the establishment of conditioned emotional responses.
� The AMPA opens🡪 let the ions enter
inside-depolarizes the neurons🡪 results in
the eviction of Mg2+ from the NMDA
receptors that blocks it(due to electrostatic
force)🡪 NMDA receptors open🡪 Let the
Ca2+ inside🡪 triggers enzyme synthesis
🡪 triggers the AMPA receptors from the
other regions of the cell membrane to move
and insert on the postsynaptic dendritic
spines🡪 More glutamates can now bind
with the number of receptors increases🡪
results in eventual increased EPSP
(Excitatory postsynaptic potentiation)🡪
results in stronger synaptic
connection(neuroplasticity)
Operant Conditioning- ROLE OF BASAL GANGLIA
Frontal
association cortex
Pontine lateral zone Dentate
ventrolater Primary
nucleus of of nucleus of
tegmentum cerebellum cerebellum al thalamus motor cortex
primary motor
cotex
Somatosensory
system
Basal ganglia
• Direct pathway- Excitatory
• Indirect pathway- Inhibitory
• Hyper direct pathway-Quick inhibitory
Role of Mirror Neurons in imitating and comprehending movements
� Perceptual learning involves changes in synaptic connections in the extrastriate cortex that
establish new neural circuits.
� At a later time, when the same stimulus is seen again and the same pattern of activity is
transmitted to the cortex, these circuits become active again.
� This activity constitutes the recognition of the stimulus—the readout, or replay, of the visual
memory.
� Yang and Maunsell (2004) trained monkeys to detect small differences in visual stimuli whose
images were projected onto a specific region of the retina. After the training was complete, the
monkeys were able to detect differences much smaller than those they could detect when the
training first started. However, they were unable to detect these differences when the patterns
were projected onto other regions of the retina. Recordings of single neurons in the extrastriate
cortex showed that the response properties of neurons that received information from the
“trained” region of the retina—but not from other regions—had become sensitive to small
differences in the stimuli. Neural circuits in that region alone had been modified by the training.
(https://www.jneurosci.org/content/24/7/1617.short)
� When stimulated the extrastriate and auditory association cortex as patients were
undergoing seizure surgery, the patients reported memories of images or sounds—for
example, images of a familiar street or the sound of the patient’s mother’s voice (seizure
surgery is performed under a local anesthetic so that the surgeons can test the effects of
brain stimulation on the patients’ cognitive functions).
(Penfield & Perot, 1963)
� Damage to regions of the brain involved in visual perception not only impairs the ability
to recognize visual stimuli but also disrupts people’s memory of the visual properties of
familiar stimuli.
� For example, Vandenbulcke et al., (2006) found that Patient J. A., who had sustained
damage to the right fusiform gyrus, performed poorly on tasks that required her to draw
or describe visual features of various animals, fruits, vegetables, tools, vehicles, or
pieces of furniture. Her other cognitive abilities, including the ability to describe
nonvisual attributes of objects, were intact. In addition, an fMRI study found that when
healthy individuals were asked to perform the visual tasks that she performed poorly,
activation was seen in the region of their brains that corresponded to J. A.’s lesion (in the
right fusiform gyrus).
� Kourtzi and Kanwisher (2000) found that specific kinds of visual information can
activate very specific regions of extrastriate cortex. A region of the extrastriate cortex,
MT/MST, plays an essential role in perception of movement.
� The investigators presented participants with photographs that implied motion—for
example, an athlete getting ready to throw a ball. They found that photographs like
these, but not photographs of people remaining still, activated area MT/MST. Even
though the photographs did not move, the participants’ memories presumably
contained information about movements they had previously seen.
� A functional-imaging study by Goldberg et al., (2006) asked people questions that
involved visual, auditory, tactile, and gustatory information. The researchers found that
answering the questions activated the regions of association cortex involved in
perception of the relevant sensory information. For example, questions about flavour
activated the gustatory cortex, questions about tactile information activated the
somatosensory cortex, and questions about visual and auditory information activated
the visual and auditory association cortex.
Retaining Perceptual Information in Short-Term Memory
� Most often we immediately after sensing a stimulus, we respond. But sometimes the situation
demands that we make the appropriate response after a delay, even after the stimulus is no longer
visible.
� For example: Crossing a road, parking a car
� Tasks like these demands comparing a perception with short term memory of something else we
had just perceived.
� Learning to recognize a stimulus involves synaptic changes in the appropriate regions of the
sensory association cortex that establish new circuits of neurons.
� Recognition of a stimulus occurs when sensory input activates these established sets of neural
circuits.
� Short-term memory of a stimulus involves activity of these circuits— or other circuits that are
activated by them—that continues even after the stimulus disappears.
� E.g.: learning to recognize a friend’s face produces changes in synaptic strengths in neural circuits
in the fusiform face region of our visual association cortex, recognizing that she is present involves
activation of the circuits that are established by these changes, and remembering that she is still in
the room even when we look elsewhere involves continued activity of these circuits (or other
circuits connected to them).
ROLE OF EXTRASTRIATE CORTEX
� Although the neural circuits responsible for learning to recognize particular stimuli appear
to reside in the sensory association cortex, perceptual short-term memories involve other
brain regions as well—especially the prefrontal cortex. Miyashita (2004) suggests that the
role of the prefrontal cortex in short-term memory is to “manipulate and organize to-be-
remembered information, devise strategies for retrieval, and also monitor the outcome” of
these processes (p. 435).
� Baier et al., (2010) note that successfully remembering recently presented information in
short-term memory requires two processes: filtering out irrelevant information and
maintaining relevant information. The investigators presented stroke patients with short-
term memory tasks that either required them to ignore extraneous, irrelevant information
or tested their ability to hold several pieces of information in mind. They found that
patients with damage to the left basal ganglia had difficulty filtering out irrelevant
information, and patients with damage to the right prefrontal cortex had difficulty retaining
more
Relational Learning
� Most of the real life learnings more complex and the memories of
objects and events are inter-related.
� Remembering an object of event may lead to retrieval of a series of
connected objects and events.
� Neural circuits in the visual cortex, especially extrastriata cortex
plays a crucial role in the recognition of a stimuli.
� These circuits are connected to the circuits in the other parts of the
brain, they are connected to the other and so on.
Role of the Hippocampus in relational learning
� Hippocampal formation consists of dentate gyrus, regions called the CA fields of the hippocampus,
and the subiculum.
� Inputs from
Amygdala Field CA1
Entorhinal
Regions of limbic cortex Entorhinal cortex Perirhinal Associati
all association regions of cortex Subiculum parahippocampal on cortex
� Without the hippocampal formation, we would be left with individual, isolated memories without
the linkage that makes it possible to remember—and think about—episodes and contexts.
� Acquisition of both major categories of relational, declarative memories—episodic and
semantic—appears to require the participation of the hippocampus. Manns et al., (2003) found
that five patients with damage limited to the hippocampal formation showed an anterograde
amnesia for semantic as well as episodic information.
� Perceptual memories appear to be located in the sensory association cortex, the
regions where the perceptions take place. Presumably, episodic memories, which
consist of an integrated sequence of perceptual memories, are also located there.
� However, the semantic memories are not simply perceptual memories. A
degenerative neurological disorder known as semantic dementia suggests that the
temporal cortex plays an important role in storing semantic information.
Semantic dementia is caused by degeneration of the neocortex of the
anterolateral temporal lobe (Lambon Ralph and Patterson, 2008). At least in the
early stages of the degenerative process, the hippocampal formation and the rest of
the medial temporal lobe are not affected.
Reconsolidation of memory: Role of hippocampus
� What happens to memories of events as time goes on? If we learn something new about a particular subject,
our memories pertaining to that subject must somehow be modified.
� Maintenance of memory is an active process. Because a consolidated LTM can become susceptible to
disruption and restoration, a process termed “reconsolidation”.
� One of the side effects of a procedure known as electroconvulsive therapy is a period of retrograde amnesia.
The procedure, used to treat cases of severe depression, involves the application of electricity through
electrodes placed on a person’s scalp. The current excites so many neurons in the brain that it produces a
seizure. Presumably, the seizure erases short-term memories present at the time and thus prevents
consolidation of these memories.
� Misanin et al., (1968) found that long-term memories, which are normally not affected by seizures, were
vulnerable to disruption by electroconvulsive shock (ECS) if a reminder of the original learning experience
was first presented. The investigators found that ECS given right after a learning experience prevented
consolidation, but ECS given a day later did not. Apparently, the ECS given right after training disrupted the
brain activity initiated by the training session and consequently interfered with consolidation. The ECS given
the next day had no effect, because the memory had already been consolidated. However, if animals were
given a “reminder” stimulus one day after training, which presumably reactivated the memory, an ECS
treatment administered immediately afterward caused amnesia for the task when the animals were tested the
following day. Reactivation of the memory made it susceptible to disruption. These studies involved stimulus-
response learning.
� More recent studies have found that long-term, well-consolidated relational memories are also susceptible to
disruption. Presumably, the process of reconsolidation, which involves neural events similar to those
responsible for the original consolidation, makes it possible for established memories to be altered or
attached to new information (Nader, 2003).
� Events that interfere with consolidation also interfere with reconsolidation and can even erase memories or at
least make them inaccessible. For example, Debiec et al., (2010) trained rats on two fear conditioning tasks.
They paired two different tones (CSa and CSb) with shocks delivered to two different parts of the body (USa
and USb). After training, they randomly presented only one of the shocks (USa or USb) to each animal
followed by an infusion of anisomycin (which blocks protein production) or a placebo into the lateral
amygdala. Protein production is an important step in longterm potentiation and memory formation. The next
day, they tested the animals for retention of the conditioned responses. They found that the animals did not
freeze when they presented the tone that had been paired with the US that had been presented just before the
infusion of anisomycin on the previous day. In other words, presenting one of the shocks “reopened” the CS-
US memory for possible reconsolidation, which made the memory link fragile. Blocking protein synthesis
prevented the memory from going back to its original state.
Hippocampal neurogenesis and consolidation
� Neurogenesis is the production of new neurons in the
brain
� Radial glia-like neural stem cells (RGLs) in
the dentate gyrus subregion of the hippocampus give
rise to dentate granule cells (DGCs) and astrocytes
throughout life, a process referred to as adult
hippocampal neurogenesis
� The newly generated neurons makes synaptic
connections with the dentate gyrus and CA 3.
� The rate of neurogenesis gradually declines with age,
however active
� The rate of neurogenesis and the survival of new
neurons are responsive to the changes in the
organism's environment.
� Antidepressant drugs increase hippocampal
neurogenesis whereas stress hormones decrease it.
Morris (1982) water maze experiment on rats
� Neurogenesis in the
hippocampus in the Rats
exposed to relational
learning increased whereas
no effects on the rats
exposed to –R learning
� Because relational learning
involves hippocampus
whereas S-R learning does
not involve it.
Role of cortex in semantic memory-Evidence