FSD Final Answer Qa
FSD Final Answer Qa
Broca’s area is a part of the brain located in the frontal lobe, usually in the
left hemisphere. It plays a key role in the production of speech and language
processing. This area is responsible for organizing thoughts into
grammatically correct sentences and coordinating the motor functions
required for speech, such as controlling the lips, tongue, and vocal cords.
Damage to Broca’s area results in a condition called Broca’s aphasia, where
a person understands language but struggles to form proper sentences.
Their speech may be slow, effortful, and broken, though meaning is often
clear. For example, instead of saying “I am going to school,” someone with
Broca’s aphasia may say “go… school.” Despite these difficulties in speech
production, comprehension usually remains intact. Broca’s area works in
close connection with Wernicke’s area and other brain regions to allow fluent
communication. Modern neuroscience also shows that Broca’s area
contributes to functions beyond language, such as problem-solving and
action planning. In short, Broca’s area is essential for expressing thoughts
through structured speech, and its damage highlights the strong link
between brain structures and language abilities.
Wernicke’s area is a brain region located in the posterior part of the left
temporal lobe, close to the auditory cortex. It plays a central role in language
comprehension. This area allows humans to understand spoken and written
language and to give meaning to words and sentences. Damage to
Wernicke’s area leads to Wernicke’s aphasia, a condition in which people can
produce fluent speech, but their sentences often lack meaning. For example,
a patient may speak long and grammatically correct sentences that are
nonsensical, such as “The clock runs fastly with the shoes.” Such individuals
often remain unaware of their errors because their comprehension is
impaired. Unlike Broca’s area, which focuses on speech production,
Wernicke’s area is specialized for understanding language and linking words
to concepts. It works together with Broca’s area through a bundle of nerve
fibers called the arcuate fasciculus, creating a strong connection between
comprehension and production. In short, Wernicke’s area ensures that
language has meaning. Without it, people may speak fluently but fail to
communicate effectively. This shows that understanding and producing
language depend on different but interconnected brain regions.
Although Behaviorist Theory contributed greatly to early studies of learning, it has several
weaknesses. First, it ignores the innate abilities of humans. Children are not blank slates; they are
born with certain mental capacities. Second, the theory places too much importance on imitation
and reinforcement, treating learners as passive receivers of language. In reality, children are
creative—they often produce new sentences they have never heard before. Third, the approach is
mechanical, focusing only on stimulus-response patterns, while real communication is more
complex. Fourth, the theory fails to explain how children can understand or produce sentences
that they have never been taught or reinforced for. For example, children can say grammatically
correct sentences without direct modeling. Fifth, it does not account for problem-solving skills in
situations where clear stimuli are missing. Lastly, Behaviorism underestimates the speed of
language acquisition in young children. They acquire complex structures much faster than the
theory predicts. Because of these limitations, Behaviorist Theory was challenged by Chomsky’s
Innate Theory, which emphasized inborn linguistic knowledge. Still, behaviorist principles like
reinforcement remain useful in classrooms and child training.
13. Discuss the evidence of ‘Childhood Aphasia’ in support of Maturation Theory. Explain with
examples.
Research on childhood aphasia provides strong evidence for the Critical Period Hypothesis
(CPH). Aphasia is a language disorder caused by brain damage, usually in the left hemisphere.
Lenneberg observed that children recover from aphasia more easily than adults. In adults, left-
hemisphere damage often causes permanent speech loss. However, children’s brains are more
flexible. If one side is injured, the other hemisphere can take over language functions. For
example, children with damage to the left hemisphere were sometimes able to regain nearly
normal speech, while adults with similar damage often remained aphasic. Even in extreme cases
where the left hemisphere was removed surgically, children could relearn language almost fully
if the surgery occurred before puberty. This recovery shows that the young brain has plasticity
during the critical period. In contrast, after the brain matures and lateralization is complete,
recovery is limited. Thus, studies on childhood aphasia strongly support Maturation Theory by
proving that the ability to learn or recover language depends heavily on biological development
and age.
14. Discuss the evidence of ‘Down Syndrome Cases’ in support of Maturation Theory. Explain
with examples.
Down Syndrome is a genetic disorder often linked with mental retardation and delayed language
development. Lenneberg and colleagues studied children with Down Syndrome to test the
Critical Period Hypothesis. Their three-year study of 54 individuals revealed that language
development in these children continued only up to puberty. After that, progress stopped. For
instance, children below 14 years could make noticeable improvements in vocabulary and
communication. However, older individuals with Down Syndrome showed little or no further
development. This finding supports Maturation Theory, which states that the ability to acquire
language declines after the critical period due to brain maturation and lateralization. The
evidence suggests that even with special conditions like Down Syndrome, age strongly
determines how much language can be learned. In short, Down Syndrome cases illustrate that the
brain’s biological clock limits language learning, reinforcing the idea that successful acquisition
is restricted to early childhood.
15. Discuss the evidence of ‘Second Language Acquisition’ in support of Maturation Theory.
Explain with examples.
Studies on second language learning provide strong evidence for the Critical Period Hypothesis.
Penfield and Roberts (1959) found that children are far better at acquiring a second language
than adults. Young learners often reach native-like fluency, while older learners rarely do. For
example, immigrant children who move to a new country before puberty usually speak the
second language fluently, with accurate grammar and pronunciation. In contrast, adults learning
under similar conditions often retain a foreign accent and face difficulties with grammar.
According to Maturation Theory, this happens because younger learners are still within their
critical period, when the brain is flexible. After puberty, the brain loses plasticity due to
lateralization, making it harder to achieve full mastery. This does not mean adults cannot learn
new languages, but their learning is slower and less complete. Therefore, second language
acquisition research supports the idea that age is a crucial factor in language learning.
16. Discuss the evidence of ‘Cases of Childhood Extreme Deprivation’ in support of Maturation
Theory. Explain with examples.
Cases of extreme deprivation, often involving feral children, provide dramatic evidence for the
Critical Period Hypothesis. One famous case is Isabelle (1930s), who lived in isolation with her
deaf mother until age six. After receiving training, she learned language normally because she
was still within the critical period. Another case is Genie (1970s), who was kept in isolation
without language exposure until age 14. Unlike Isabelle, Genie failed to acquire normal grammar
and could only learn basic vocabulary. Her case suggested that after puberty, language
acquisition becomes nearly impossible. Similarly, the case of E.M., a deaf child isolated from
language until adolescence, also showed severe limitations in learning grammar despite later
training. These examples confirm that if children miss exposure to language during the critical
period, they cannot fully acquire it later. Thus, deprivation studies strongly support Maturation
Theory by proving that age sets strict biological limits on language learning.
17. Discuss the evidence of ‘Deaf children with hearing parents’ in support of Maturation
Theory. Explain with examples.
Deaf children born to hearing parents often face linguistic deprivation because they are not
exposed to sign language early in life. Research shows that these children struggle with language
if they begin learning sign language after puberty. For example, studies found that older deaf
children enrolling in deaf schools could build vocabulary but had poor grammar, similar to
Genie’s case. One case is Chelsea, a deaf woman who only received hearing aids at age 31. She
learned many words but could not develop proper grammar, proving that starting beyond the
critical period limited her language ability. Another case, E.M., a deaf boy from Mexico, also
struggled with tenses despite exposure in adolescence. These findings confirm that language
must be acquired early, when the brain is most sensitive. If missed, later acquisition remains
incomplete. Therefore, deaf children with hearing parents provide strong evidence for the
Critical Period Hypothesis, supporting Maturation Theory.
18. Differentiate between Critical Period Hypothesis and Sensitive Period Hypothesis.
The Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) argues that there is a strict, fixed window for language
learning, usually ending at puberty. If language is not learned during this time, it becomes very
difficult or even impossible. For example, Genie’s case showed that missing the critical period
prevented full language development. In contrast, the Sensitive Period Hypothesis (SPH)
suggests that language can still be learned after childhood, but with reduced efficiency. It does
not see the boundary as absolute. According to SPH, learning is easier during early years but still
possible later, though learners may face challenges with pronunciation or grammar. For instance,
adults can learn second languages successfully, but usually less fluently than children. In short,
CPH views the time limit as strict and final, while SPH allows for gradual decline in ability.
Both agree that early exposure is best, but SPH is more flexible than CPH.
19. Discuss View of Knowledge according to Social Constructivism theory.
Social Constructivism views learning as a collaborative process rather than an individual effort.
Vygotsky introduced the concept of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD), which explains
how learners can achieve more with the help of teachers or peers than they can alone. The actual
level of development represents what a learner can do independently, while the potential level
represents what they can do with guidance. For example, a child may not be able to solve a math
problem alone but can succeed with a teacher’s help. Over time, this guidance helps the learner
internalize the skill. Learning, therefore, happens first at the social level (interaction with others)
and later at the individual level (internal understanding). In short, Social Constructivism
emphasizes that real learning depends on collaboration, dialogue, and guidance within a
supportive community.
In Social Constructivism, motivation is seen as both intrinsic and extrinsic. On one hand,
learners are internally motivated by their own curiosity and desire to understand. On the other
hand, because learning is a social activity, external motivation also plays an important role.
Rewards, recognition, and encouragement from peers and teachers can inspire learners. For
example, in a group project, students may feel motivated not only to learn for themselves but
also to contribute to the success of their team. Vygotsky emphasized that motivation grows
stronger when learners are actively engaged in meaningful social interactions. Unlike
Behaviorism, which sees motivation mainly as response to rewards or punishments, Social
Constructivism highlights that learners are driven by both personal goals and social belonging.
Thus, motivation arises from a combination of inner drive and social influences.
22. Discuss Implications for learning of Social Constructivism theory.
Broad Question 1
Explain the idea of Universal Grammar (UG) and Language Acquisition Device (LAD) in Innate
Theory. Elaborate on the evidences in support of Universal Grammar. Discuss the
counterarguments and criticisms on Innate Theory.
Introduction
The Innate Theory of language acquisition, proposed by Noam Chomsky in 1959, challenged the
Behaviorist view of language as a mere habit. Chomsky argued that children are not passive
learners but are biologically programmed to acquire language. According to him, humans are
born with an inbuilt mechanism called the Language Acquisition Device (LAD), which contains
a set of grammatical rules known as Universal Grammar (UG). This theory suggests that
language learning is natural, quick, and possible for every human child.
The LAD is an internal mechanism that allows children to identify patterns in the language they
hear. It helps them form grammatical rules without formal teaching. Within the LAD lies
Universal Grammar (UG), which refers to the basic principles and structures common to all
languages. For example, every language has nouns, verbs, and rules for forming questions. While
languages differ in surface forms, their deep structures follow shared patterns. This explains why
children across cultures learn language in similar stages, such as babbling, one-word speech, and
two-word combinations.
Evidence Supporting UG
1. Poverty of the Stimulus: Children learn complex grammar despite limited input. Parents rarely
correct grammar, yet children develop accurate structures.
2. Creativity of Language: Children often produce sentences they have never heard before, such
as “I goed there,” showing they apply internal rules, not just imitation.
3. Critical Period Hypothesis: Eric Lenneberg argued that language must be acquired before
puberty, supporting the idea of biological programming.
4. Language Universals: Across cultures, languages share common features such as word
categories and structural rules, proving the existence of UG.
5. Species Specificity: Only humans can acquire complex language, which highlights the
uniqueness of UG.
Lack of Clarity: Scholars disagree on what exactly UG contains. Some argue it has hundreds of
principles, while others suggest it may be as simple as a single operation.
Overemphasis on Biology: Critics claim the theory ignores the role of social interaction,
environment, and culture in language learning.
Cross-Linguistic Diversity: Linguists like Evans and Levinson argue that languages differ so
widely that it is hard to claim universal grammar rules.
Empirical Challenges: Some children fail to learn grammar even with exposure, suggesting that
environment and quality of input are also crucial.
Alternative Theories: Social Constructivism and Cognitive theories highlight learning through
communication, culture, and usage rather than innate devices.
Conclusion
Broad Question 2
What are the Theoretical Assumptions behind Maturation Theory? Elaborate on the evidences in
support of and against Critical Period Hypothesis.
Introduction
The Maturation Theory, proposed by Eric Lenneberg in 1967, explains language acquisition as a
biological process tied to human growth. Building on Chomsky’s Innate Theory, Lenneberg
emphasized that language learning depends on the brain’s maturation. Central to this theory is
the Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH), which argues that humans can acquire language fully only
within a limited time frame—childhood to puberty. After this period, the ability declines
drastically. This idea remains one of the most debated topics in linguistics and psychology.
2. Critical Period for Learning: There is a specific time span—roughly from early childhood to
puberty—when the brain is most capable of acquiring language.
3. Brain Lateralization: Initially, both hemispheres of the brain can handle language. Around age
two, lateralization begins, and by puberty, the left hemisphere becomes dominant for language.
After this, the right hemisphere cannot fully take over, limiting new language learning.
4. Loss of Plasticity: As the brain matures, it loses flexibility, making language acquisition more
difficult in adulthood.
1. Childhood Aphasia: Children with brain damage often recover language abilities, while adults
rarely do. This shows that the young brain is flexible during the critical period.
2. Down Syndrome Cases: Lenneberg’s study of 54 children showed that language progress
stopped after puberty, supporting the time-bound nature of learning.
3. Second Language Acquisition: Research shows that children exposed to a second language
before puberty achieve near-native fluency, while adults often struggle with accent and grammar.
4. Extreme Deprivation Cases: The case of Isabelle, who learned language after isolation at age
six, shows recovery within the critical period. In contrast, Genie, isolated until 14, never fully
acquired grammar, proving limits beyond puberty.
5. Deaf Children with Hearing Parents: Those deprived of sign language early often fail to
master grammar later, even if they acquire vocabulary, showing that timing is crucial.
Unclear Age Boundaries: Researchers disagree on when the critical period begins and ends.
Some suggest as early as age five, while others propose it extends beyond puberty.
Sensitive Period Hypothesis: Many scholars now argue for a “sensitive period,” where language
learning is easier during childhood but still possible later, though less efficient.
Adult Success Cases: Studies (e.g., Birdsong, 1992) show that adults can achieve near-native
competence, especially in second language learning, challenging the strictness of CPH.
Other Influences: Factors such as motivation, exposure, and social environment also affect
learning, which CPH tends to overlook.
Case Study Limitations: Feral children like Genie may have suffered from abuse or brain
damage, not just late exposure, which complicates conclusions.
Conclusion
Maturation Theory and the Critical Period Hypothesis provide a biological explanation for why
children learn languages so quickly and why adults face difficulties. Evidence from brain studies,
language disorders, and deprivation cases strongly supports the theory. However,
counterarguments highlight its rigid boundaries and neglect of environmental and social factors.
A balanced view suggests that while childhood is the most favorable time for language learning,
motivated adults can also succeed. Thus, the theory remains influential but requires refinement
through modern research.
Broad Question 3
What are the Theoretical Assumptions behind Behaviorist Theory? Explain different types of
Behavioral Learning. Discuss the drawbacks of the theory.
Introduction
The Behaviorist Theory of language learning, developed in the early 20th century by
psychologists such as John B. Watson and B. F. Skinner, views language as a learned behavior
rather than an innate ability. Rooted in the principles of stimulus–response psychology, the
theory dominated educational practice for decades. It emphasizes the role of the environment,
habit formation, and reinforcement in shaping how humans acquire and use language. Although
later challenged by Chomsky’s Innate Theory, Behaviorism remains important for understanding
certain aspects of learning.
1. Language as Habit Formation: Language is acquired like any other habit—through repetition
and practice.
2. Learning through Experience: Children learn language by interacting with their environment,
not through innate abilities.
5. Human Specificity: Only humans can acquire complex language, learned as separate habits
rather than as an integrated system.
1. Classical Conditioning
Discovered by Ivan Pavlov, this type of learning occurs when a neutral stimulus is repeatedly
paired with a natural stimulus. In Pavlov’s experiment, dogs learned to salivate at the sound of a
bell when it was associated with food. Applied to language, children may learn to connect words
with objects or actions through repeated association.
Overemphasis on Imitation: Children are not passive imitators; they often produce new, creative
sentences they have never heard before.
Mechanical Nature: It treats language learning as a rigid stimulus–response process, ignoring the
complexity of communication.
Fails to Explain Novel Sentences: Children understand and generate sentences beyond their
input, something Behaviorism cannot explain.
Limited Problem-Solving Skills: In situations without clear stimuli, the theory cannot explain
how learners respond.
Underestimates Speed of Learning: Children acquire grammar much faster than simple habit
formation predicts.
Conclusion
The Behaviorist Theory provided a systematic explanation of learning and strongly influenced
early teaching practices, such as the Audio-Lingual Method. By highlighting reinforcement and
environmental input, it offered practical methods for child-rearing and classroom instruction.
However, its neglect of creativity, innate factors, and cognitive processes limits its explanatory
power. Later theories, especially Chomsky’s Innate Theory, corrected these gaps. Even so,
Behaviorist principles like repetition, reinforcement, and habit-building continue to be valuable
tools in education, especially for beginners and simpler learning tasks.